An official website of the United States government

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

Stress, Sport Anxiety, Neuroticism, and Coping in Student-Athletes: Implications for Patient Mental Health

David william contreras , psyd, megan d granquist , phd, atc, luci a martin , phd.

  • Author information
  • Article notes
  • Copyright and License information

Address correspondence to Megan D. Granquist, PhD, ATC, University of La Verne, 1950 Third Street, La Verne, CA 91750. Address email to [email protected] .

Issue date 2023 Sep.

Patients’ mental health has been recognized as important in providing patient-centered care in athletic training.

To evaluate stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping in student-athletes.

Cross-sectional study.

University athletics.

Patients or Other Participants

The sample comprised 86 student-athletes competing in National Collegiate Athletic Association (NCAA) Division III.

Main Outcome Measure(s)

The Perceived Stress Scale, Sport Anxiety Scale-2, neuroticism scale of the Big Five Inventory, and Brief COPE were used to measure stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping, respectively.

Sport anxiety was positively related to stress ( r = 0.446, P < .001) and neuroticism ( r = 0.311, P < .01) and not related to emotion-focused coping ( r = 0.270, P = .804). Neuroticism was a negative predictor of emotion-focused coping ( b = −0.373, P < .001), and sport anxiety and stress were predictors of dysfunctional coping ( b = 0.120, P < .05; b = 0.037, P < .05). Stress, sport anxiety, and neuroticism were not predictors of problem-focused coping.

Conclusions

Support was evident for the relationship among stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping in NCAA Division III student-athletes. Such results warrant future exploration to inform behavioral interventions targeting student-athlete psychosocial factors to promote improved performance, reduce injury risk factors (eg, stress, personality, coping), and enhance student-athlete mental health and well-being.

Keywords: well-being, personality, patient-centered care

Student-athletes’ sport anxiety was positively related to stress and the personality factor of neuroticism.

The personality factor of neuroticism was a negative predictor of emotion-focused coping.

Sport anxiety and stress were predictors of dysfunctional coping.

The Stress-Injury Model describes 3 interrelated categories of antecedents to injury risk: stress, personality, and coping. 2 Given that identification of mental health issues and injury prevention are within the scope of athletic training practice, in this research, we examine these 3 categories of stress (including sport anxiety), personality (specifically neuroticism), and coping.

Student-athletes experiencing higher levels of perceived stress reported greater sport anxiety. 3 When stress and sport anxiety were combined with a personality factor such as neuroticism, athletes were less able to deal with stressors that may arise during a competition, which in turn influenced their coping response. 4 , 5 Continued evaluation of sport anxiety in athletes is warranted given that it has been associated with increased risk for injury in players with starting status, 6 positively associated with athletes’ wellbeing, 7 and linked to overall satisfaction in athletes. 8 Sport anxiety has also been negatively related to satisfaction in sport activities, 9 shown to mediate the protective factor of athletic involvement, 10 and associated with increased incidences of eating disorders in collegiate athletes. 11 Continued research into these variables is needed because of a variety of potentially severe outcomes, such as mental health concerns, 12 – 14 poor academic performance, 15 injury during competition, 6 burnout, 8 and diminished sport performance. 6

Authors 4 have investigated the relationship between sport anxiety and coping strategies; however, to date, the relationships between factors such as stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping strategies in National College Athletic Association (NCAA) Division III student-athletes have not been addressed. Elite-level athletes may have better-developed mechanisms for coping with general life stressors and sport anxiety, given that their higher level of competition requires a better-developed set of psychological and physical skills; therefore, Division III athletes may have unique responses to life stressors and sport anxiety and may use different mechanisms for coping. 16

When considering that athletes who experience the life stressors associated with being a traditional college student must also handle the stressors associated with being a student-athlete and may have competitive trait anxiety, which predisposes them to becoming anxious or stressed in sport-related experiences, it becomes apparent that further exploration of the individual differences that either contribute to or hinder an athlete’s coping response to stressors is needed. We used coping as an outcome with the rationale that athletes cannot change their stressors or personalities, but coping can be targeted by athletic trainers and mental health providers. If we can determine what leads to certain coping strategies, then these strategies can be targeted for intervention. Therefore, the purpose of our study was to examine the relationships between stress, sport anxiety, the personality factor neuroticism, and coping strategies in a sample of NCAA Division III student-athletes.

Participants

From 102 original participants, complete data from 86 participants were included in the analyses. Participants consisted of a purposive sample of Division III student-athletes (55% female, 45% male; mean age = 19.62 ± 1.39 years). The participant sample was racially or ethnically diverse as 6.8% identified as African American, 1.2% as American Indian, 4.1% as Asian-American, 21.9% as Hispanic or Latino, 16.4% as mixed ethnicity, and 49.3% as White or European American (see Table 1 ).

Participant and Demographic Information (N = 86)

Some participants did not provide data regarding ethnicity.

Percentages add up to 99.9% due to rounding error.

After we obtained institutional review board approval, volunteers were sought in collaboration with the university athletics department. Student-athletes were recruited through email and flyers posted throughout the university. All student-athletes who were enrolled in a weight training course were asked to engage in the study. Initial data were gathered using hardcopy survey packets but were later obtained using an online survey. Participants were asked to sign a consent form and complete the survey, which took approximately 45 minutes. Those who completed the survey were entered into a raffle to win 1 of ten $20 gift cards.

Instrumentation

Stress was measured using the 10-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS10). 17 The PSS10 was adapted from the 14-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS14) and demonstrated a slight improvement in psychometric properties over the PSS14 with internal reliability ranging from 0.84 to 0.86. The PSS10 measures perceived stress over the course of 1 month on a Likert scale ranging from 0 ( never ) to 4 ( very often ). High scores on the PSS10 indicate high levels of perceived stress, and low scores indicate low levels of perceived stress. 17 A sample item from the PSS10 is “In the last month, how often have you been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly?” The PSS10 has been validated with a collegiate population (α = .89). 18 Additional support for the use of the PSS10 in a collegiate athlete population was evidenced by an internal consistency coefficient of 0.90 in a sample of 123 injured student-athletes. 19

Sport Anxiety

The Sport Anxiety Scale-2 (SAS-2) 20 was used to measure sport anxiety via the composite score. This 15-item measure was adapted from the original Sport Anxiety Scale to provide a measure that was suitable for both child and adult populations. A sample item from the SAS-2 is “Before or while I compete in sports, it is hard to concentrate on the game.” The SAS-2 demonstrates strong psychometric properties with an internal consistency coefficient of 0.91. 20 Normative data were gathered from a sample of 1038 child athletes and 593 college students participating in organized sports activities (eg, intercollegiate athletics, intramural sports, club sports). 20 Items are divided into 3 subscales (somatic, worry, concentration disruption) consisting of 5 items each. Reliability coefficients for each subscale were 0.84 for somatic, 0.89 for worry, and 0.84 for concentration disruption, indicating strong reliability coefficients at both the total score and subscale levels. 20 In a collegiate student-athlete population, internal consistencies were 0.87 for somatic anxiety, 0.91 for worry, and 0.70 for concentration disruption. 21

Personality

The Big Five Inventory (BFI), 22 consisting of 44 short-phrase items, was used to measure the neuroticism trait of the Big Five personality domain. Items are responded to on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 ( disagree strongly ) to 5 ( agree strongly ). The neuroticism subscale consists of 8 items, and respondents are asked to indicate the extent to which they agree or disagree with each statement beginning with the phrase, “I see myself as someone who…”

Despite the scales of the BFI containing only 8 to 10 items, good psychometric properties are still evident throughout the scale. Researchers 23 have shown an internal consistency coefficient of 0.87 for neuroticism 22 and test-retest stability of 0.70. Convergent and divergent validity were examined in a community dataset of BFI self-reports and peer ratings, yielding correlations of 0.52 for neuroticism. The BFI has also demonstrated satisfactory reliability in an athlete population with a reliability coefficient of 0.79 for neuroticism. 5

The Brief COPE was administered to measure the use of coping behaviors. 24 The Brief COPE was derived from the original 60-item COPE scale to meet the needs of time-sensitive health care settings and to address the redundancy of items on the COPE. 24 The Brief COPE comprises 28 items grouped into 14 subscales: active coping, planning, positive reframing, self-distraction, acceptance, using instrumental support, using emotional support, religion, venting, denial, behavioral disengagement, substance use, humor, and self-blame. Questions are answered on a Likert scale ranging from 1 ( I haven’t been doing this at all ) to 4 ( I’ve been doing this a lot ). Carver 24 found α reliabilities ranging from .50 (venting) to .90 (substance use).

Data gathered from the Brief COPE were organized into problem-focused, emotion-focused, and dysfunctional coping aggregate scores as shown by Chiavarino et al 25 : problem-focused coping (active coping, planning, and instrumental support; α = .78); emotion-focused coping (positive reframing, humor, acceptance, religion, and emotional support; α = .72); and dysfunctional coping (self-distraction, denial, behavioral disengagement, self-blame, substance use, and venting; α = .70).

Data Analyses

A Pearson r correlation and multiple regression were conducted to examine relationships between stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and emotion-focused coping. Multiple regressions were calculated to examine the relationships between stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and problem-focused coping and between stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and dysfunctional-focused coping.

Analysis was completed using SPSS (version 23; IBM Corp). Data were screened for missing values and violations of statistical assumptions. We examined the assumptions of multiple regression analysis and found 2 violations of normality. The dysfunctional coping and sport anxiety factors were positively skewed (dysfunctional coping: skewness = 0.754, kurtosis = 1.012; sport anxiety: skewness = 1.004, kurtosis = 0.409). Means, SDs, and ranges for variables of interest can be found in Table 1 . Dysfunctional coping was transformed using a logarithmic function and was returned to normality (skewness = 0.103, kurtosis = 0.294). Regarding sport anxiety, logarithmic and square root functions were attempted but unsuccessful. Inverse transformation yielded a normally distributed dataset; however, the data were reflected to maintain interpretative statements. Specifically, after transformation, a constant (eg, maximum inverted SAS score + 1) was subtracted from each inverse transformation so the data could be interpreted as before the transformation (skewness = −0.103, kurtosis = −0.690).

The demographic variables of sex, race, age, and income were examined in relation to the 6 continuous variables of interest. Sex was coded such that male = 0 and female = 1, race as White = 1 and all others = 0, and income as less than $10 000 per year = 0 and $10 000 or more = 1. Sex and neuroticism were significantly related ( r = 0.221, P = .041), and race and emotion-focused coping were inversely related ( r = −0.269, P = .012). No other significant differences were found among demographic variables.

Tolerance, variance inflation factors, and correlations between predictor variables were examined for multicollinearity and deemed acceptable (tolerance ≥ 0.01, variance inflation factor < 10). Additionally, on visual examination of a scatterplot for each regression model, no evidence of severe violations of multicollinearity, homoscedasticity, or linearity was present. Given the relatively small sample size, adjusted R 2 values were used to examine the variance accounted for by each model.

Inferential Analysis

A Pearson r correlation indicated that neuroticism was significantly positively correlated with sport anxiety ( r 86 = 0.311, P = .004), and perceived stress was significantly positively correlated with sport anxiety ( r 86 = 0.446, P < .001). Similar results using partial correlations were evident while controlling for sex and race (see Table 2 ).

Bivariate and Partial Correlations Among Neuroticism, Stress, Sport Anxiety, Emotion-Focused Coping, Problem-Focused Coping, and Dysfunctional Coping a

Abbreviation: NA, not applicable.

Left, bottom diagonal shows bivariate correlations between variables. Right, upper diagonal shows partial correlations controlling for sex (female = 1, male = 0) and race (white = 1; all other = 0).

P < .01.

P < .05.

Multiple regression was used to examine the relationships between perceived stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and emotion-focused coping. Regression analysis demonstrated that perceived stress, sport anxiety, and neuroticism accounted for a significant portion of the variance in emotion-focused coping (adjusted R 2 = 0.150, F 3,82 = 5.98, P < .001). Neuroticism was a significant negative predictor of emotion-focused coping ( b = −0.373, t 85 = −4.22, P < .001; see Table 3 ).

Multiple Regression Analysis for Variables Associated With Emotion-Focused Coping

To evaluate the relationships between perceived stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and problem-focused coping, multiple regression was calculated. Perceived stress, sport anxiety, and neuroticism did not account for a significant portion of the variance in problem-focused coping (adjusted R 2 = 0.030, F 3,82 = 1.89, P = .13; see Table 4 ).

Multiple Regression Analysis for Variables Associated With Problem-Focused Coping

Multiple regression was computed to examine the relationships between perceived stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and dysfunctional coping. Perceived stress, sport anxiety, and neuroticism accounted for a significant portion of the variance in dysfunctional coping (adjusted R 2 = 0.218, F 3,82 = 8.90, P < .001). Perceived stress ( b = 0.037, t 85 = 2.36, P = .020) and sport anxiety ( b = 0.120, t 85 = 2.25, P = .027) were significant predictors of dysfunctional coping (see Table 5 ).

Multiple Regression Analysis for Variables Associated With Dysfunctional Coping

Post Hoc Analysis

To test the role of stress as a mediator between sport anxiety, neuroticism, and dysfunctional coping, we used the PROCESS macro in SPSS. 26 , 27

As indicated in Table 6 , a simple mediation analysis indicated that neuroticism was indirectly related to dysfunctional coping through stress. Neuroticism was significantly regressed onto the mediator, stress ( b = 0.47, SE = 0.09, P < .001). Furthermore, neuroticism had an indirect effect on dysfunctional coping ( b = 0.04, SE = 0.01, P = .004). Stress was a significant predictor of dysfunctional coping ( b = 0.05, SE = 0.01, P = .001), and the relationship between neuroticism and dysfunctional coping was reduced to nonsignificance ( b = 0.01, SE = 0.01, P = .314), reflecting full mediation. A 95% bias-corrected CI based on 5000 bootstrap samples indicated that the indirect effect ( ab = 0.0229) was entirely above zero (0.0084, 0.0415).

Stress as a Mediator of Neuroticism and Dysfunctional Coping

As shown in Table 7 , sport anxiety was significantly regressed onto the mediator ( b = 1.70, SE = 0.37, P < .001). Sport anxiety also had an indirect effect on dysfunctional coping ( b = 0.20, SE = 0.05, P < .001). Stress was a significant predictor of dysfunctional coping when controlling for sport anxiety ( b = 0.04, SE = 0.01, P < .003); however, the relationship between anxiety and dysfunctional coping remained significant ( b = 0.13, SE = 0.05, P = .021), signifying partial mediation. A 95% bias-corrected CI based on 5000 bootstrap samples indicated that the indirect effect ( ab = 0.0712) was entirely above zero (0.0234, 0.1383).

Stress as a Mediator of Sport-Related Anxiety and Dysfunctional Coping

The purpose of our study was to examine the relationships between stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping in student-athletes. Such a design is especially salient given the growing number of difficulties student-athletes face, specifically growing rates of substance use, academic dishonesty, injury, eating disorders, and stress. 12 – 14 , 28

These findings can inform the development of interventions targeting stressed, anxious, and neurotic student-athletes in the hope of helping them acquire more appropriate means of coping. High levels of stress and sport anxiety were the best predictors of dysfunctional coping in student-athletes, which suggests that they may benefit from interventions fostering adaptive means of coping with such feelings or thoughts. Additionally, stress serving as a significant mediator between the neuroticism and dysfunctional coping relationship highlights the need for interventions aimed at reducing their stress, which could in turn reduce dysfunctional coping. Dysfunctional coping spans a range of strategies, including self-distraction, denial, behavioral disengagement, self-blame, substance use, and venting. All strategies can lead to potentially problematic outcomes such as injury, poor sport performance, disordered eating, and academic dishonesty. 12 – 14 , 28

Decreases in neuroticism predicted increases in emotion-focused coping strategies, indicating that an emotion-focused factor such as acceptance may be affected by an individual factor such as neuroticism. This assertion makes sense in that, by nature, the neuroticism personality construct reflects emotion constriction and self-consciousness, qualities that are paradoxical to acceptance. Helping student-athletes identify whether they possess a degree of neuroticism may aid them in developing a less judgmental stance and more adaptive means of coping with their distress.

Neuroticism was not a significant predictor of dysfunctional coping. This finding was partially expected, as we originally thought that stress, competitive anxiety, and neuroticism would all account for a portion of the dysfunctional coping variance. Additionally, we thought that, because higher levels of neuroticism are considered problematic and overall relatively negative (eg, dysfunctional coping, stress, and competitive trait anxiety), it would account for some of the variance. The current results contrast with those of others 29 who found denial, behavioral disengagement, self-blame, and substance abuse to be unrelated to stress.

Interestingly, no statistically significant relationship was noted between competitive trait anxiety and emotion-focused coping. This outcome was unexpected and inconsistent with the previous literature, 30 which showed that student-athletes who possessed higher levels of competitive anxiety demonstrated a tendency to engage in emotion-focused strategies (eg, denial, disengagement, venting of emotions). This difference may be explained by how coping was grouped. Emotion-focused coping comprised positive reframing, humor, acceptance, religion, and emotional support skills, all of which can be seen as positive coping skills when compared with denial, disengagement, and venting of emotions, the coping skills that were included in the dysfunctional coping aggregate. The skills in the emotion-focused aggregate can be considered adaptive; therefore, individuals who scored higher in emotion-focused coping may have more adaptive responses to stressors.

Our study adds to the research in several ways. First, we are the first to examine the relationships between stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping strategies in student-athletes. We provide novel support for the role of neuroticism as a negative predictor of emotion-focused coping. We also observed support for the role of stress and sport anxiety in influencing coping strategies. Moreover, the positive relationship between stress, sport anxiety, and neuroticism was endorsed, which possibly suggests that domain-specific anxiety is influenced by individual differences such as stress and neuroticism.

Limitations of the Study

Although our work had several strengths, certain limitations should be considered when evaluating the findings. First, sample size was a notable limitation. The number of participants (N = 86) was sufficient for the purposes of the appropriate statistical analyses, yet the risk of type I error (eg, false-positive results) was increased. Additionally, our findings were significant at the P < .01 level, which further supports the notion that they did not occur based on chance alone. The insignificant findings related to neuroticism and emotion-focused coping findings do not appear to have occurred because of type II error.

Differences may have existed between athletes of different sexes, sport types, ethnic identities, or injury histories in terms of their levels of stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping. Including individuals from other institutions or, at a minimum, a greater number of student-athletes from the participating institution would have provided more statistical power and better generalizability of the findings.

Regarding other measurement limitations, although the Brief COPE has been aggregated into emotion-focused, problem-focused, and dysfunctional coping composites with success and sound psychometric values, 25 this was not specifically how the measure was designed and may have influenced the interpretability of the results. Moreover, the dysfunctional coping variable was transformed to satisfy statistical assumptions necessary for regression analysis. Because the initial dysfunctional coping variable was not perfectly normal and subsequently converted, such skewness could reflect a novel phenomenon in the current dataset. However, even after transformation, this phenomenon was still present, as dysfunctional coping was correlated with multiple psychosocial variables, thus indicating that transformation of the data did not obscure interpretative statements.

Our sample of NCAA Division III student-athletes was a noted strength of the investigation, yet this design was also limited in that Division III athletes may face stressors that are unique to their status as student-athletes who are not eligible for athletic scholarships. For example, Division III student-athletes may have increased financial stress and less accommodation by professors or university stakeholders, as sport participation is not as heavily integrated into the college experience, or increased academic demands. 31

Finally, as with many self-report designs, social desirability and recall bias may have affected the results, 32 and the length of the survey may have been partially responsible for the lack of responses, as it took approximately 45 minutes to complete.

Recommendations for Future Research

Future research into the relationships between stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping is necessary to elucidate what factors or combination of factors influences coping in athletes. The body of literature would benefit from continued examination of the role of personality characteristics in influencing athlete behavior, particularly as related to injury, mental health, and well-being.

Another area for inquiry is the sport-type differences in stress, sport anxiety, and coping. Authors have argued that sport-type differences in coping and types of stressors exist 33 ; however, despite the differences in coping strategies between male and female student-athletes, the outcomes regarding which strategies are typically employed by each group are largely mixed. 33 , 34

Ideally, future investigators should attempt to gather participants from each level of the NCAA or National Association of Intercollegiate Athletics competition to obtain a more complete picture of how factors such as stress, sport anxiety, and neuroticism influence behavior. Within-group differences in student-athletes may add to the complexity of investigating student-athlete stress, sport anxiety, and coping behavior.

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION

We explored the relationships between stress, sport anxiety, neuroticism, and coping strategies in NCAA Division III student-athletes. Sport anxiety was positively related to stress and neuroticism and not related to emotion-focused coping. Neuroticism was a negative predictor of emotion-focused coping, and stress and sport anxiety were predictors of dysfunctional coping. Stress, sport anxiety, and neuroticism were not predictors of problem-focused coping.

The current study had several strengths that should be considered. Notably, we sought to expand upon the body of research by examining the aforementioned factors in a sample of NCAA Division III student-athletes, a section of the student-athlete population that had been largely unexamined. 31 Even though we achieved this goal, the limitations should be considered when interpreting the findings and in the development of future research projects. Specifically, given the relatively small sample size (N = 86), the generalizability of the results is limited. The limited sample size also restricted our ability to more precisely determine what specific coping skills NCAA Division III athletes employed and how selection of such strategies was affected by stress, sport anxiety, or neuroticism.

These outcomes warrant future exploration, yet they can inform behavioral interventions targeting student-athlete psychosocial factors to promote improved performance, reduce injury risk factors (eg, stress, personality, coping), and enhance student-athlete mental health and well-being.

  • 1. Neal TL, Diamond AB, Goldman S, et al. Inter-association recommendations for developing a plan to recognize and refer student-athletes with psychological concerns at the collegiate level: an executive summary of a consensus statement. J Athl Train . 2013; 48 (5): 716–720. 10.4085/1062-6050-48.4.13 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 2. Williams JM, Andersen MB. Psychosocial antecedents of sport injury: review and critique of the stress and injury model. J Appl Sport Psychol . 1998; 10 (1): 5–25. 10.1080/10413209808406375 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 3. Petrie T, Russell RK. Academic and psychosocial antecedents of academic performance for minority and nonminority college football players. J Couns Dev . 1995; 73 (6): 615–620. 10.1002/j.1556-6676.1995.tb01805.x [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 4. Dias C, Cruz JF, Fonseca AM. The relationship between multidimensional competitive anxiety, cognitive threat appraisal, and coping strategies: a multi-sport study. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol . 2012; 10 (1): 52–65. 10.1080/1612197X.2012.645131 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 5. Kaiseler M, Polman RJCJ, Nicholls AR. Effects of the Big Five personality dimensions on appraisal coping, and coping effectiveness in sport. Eur J Sport Sci . 2012; 12 (1): 62–72. 10.1080/17461391.2010.551410 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 6. Petrie TA. Coping skills, competitive trait anxiety, and playing states: moderating effects on the life stress-injury relationship. J Sport Exerc Psychol . 1993; 15 (3): 261–274. 10.1123/jsep.15.3.261 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 7. Mellalieu SD, Hanton S, Fletcher D. A competitive anxiety review: recent directions in sport psychology research. In: Hanton S, Mellalieu, eds. Literature Reviews in Sport Psychology . Nova Science; 2009; 1–45. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 8. Vealey RS, Armstrong L, Comar WD, Greenleaf CA. Influence of perceived coaching behaviors on burnout and competitive anxiety in female college athletes. J Appl Sport Psychol . 1998; 10 (2): 297–318. 10.1080/10413209808406395 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 9. Rainey DW, Cunningham H. Competitive trait anxiety in male and female college athletes. Res Q Exerc Sport . 1998; 59 (3): 244–247. 10.1080/02701367.1988.10605511 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 10. Kimball A, Freysinger VJ. Leisure, stress, and coping: the sport participation of collegiate student-athletes. Leis Sci . 2003; 25 (2–3): 115–141. 10.1080/01490400306569 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 11. Gomes AR, Martins C, Silva L. Eating disordered behaviours in Portuguese athletes: the influence of personal, sport, and psychological variables. Eur Eat Disord Rev . 2011; 19 (3): 190–200. 10.1002/erv.1113 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 12. Byrne S, McLean N. Eating disorders in athletes: a review of the literature. J Sci Med Sport . 2001; 4 (2): 145–159. 10.1016/s1440-2440(01)80025-6 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 13. Davis C, Cowles M. A comparison of weight and diet concerns and personality factors among female athletes and non-athletes. J Psychosom Res . 1989; 33 (5): 527–536. 10.1016/0022-3999(89)90060-3 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 14. Brown GT, Hainline B, Kroshus E, Wilfert M. Mind, Body, and Sport: Understanding and Supporting Student-Athlete Mental Wellness . National Collegiate Athletic Association; 2014. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 15. Hanson SJ, McCullagh P, Tonymon P. The relationship of personality characteristics, life stress, and coping resources to athletic injury. J Sport Exerc Psychol . 1992; 14 (3): 262–272. 10.1123/jsep.14.3.262 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 16. Kerr G, Minden H. Psychological factors related to the occurrence of athletic injuries. J Sport Exerc Psychol . 1998; 10 (2): 167–173. 10.1123/jsep.10.2.167 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 17. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav . 1983; 24 (4): 385–396. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 18. Roberti JW, Harrington LN, Storch EA. Further psychometric support for the 10-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale. J Coll Counsel . 2006; 9 (2): 135–147. 10.1002/j.2161-1882.2006.tb00100.x [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 19. Malinauskas R. The associations among social support, stress, and life satisfaction as perceived by injured college athletes. Soc Behav Person . 2010; 38 (6): 741–752. 10.2224/sbp.2010.38.6.741 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 20. Smith RE, Smoll FL, Cumming SP, Grossbard JR. Measurement of multidimensional sport performance anxiety in children and adults: the Sport Anxiety Scale-2. J Sport Exerc Psychol . 2006; 28 (4): 479–501. 10.1123/jsep.28.4.479 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 21. Monsma E, Mensch J, Farroll J. Keeping your head in the game: sport-specific imagery and anxiety among injured athletes. J Athl Train . 2009; 44 (4): 410–417. 10.4085/1062-6050-44.4.410 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 22. John OP, Donahue EM, Kentle RL. The Big Five Inventory . Versions 4a and 54. University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research; 1991. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 23. Hampson SE, Goldberg LR. A first large cohort study of personality trait stability over the 40 years between elementary school and midlife. J Pers Soc Psychol . 2006; 91 (4): 763–779. 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.763 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 24. Carver CS. You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: consider the Brief COPE. Int J Behav Med . 1997; 4 (1): 92–100. 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401_6 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 25. Chiavarino C, Rabellino D, Ardito RB, et al. Emotional coping is a better predictor of cardiac prognosis than depression and anxiety. J Psychosom. Res . 2012; 73 (6): 473–475. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.10.002 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 26. Hayes A. The PROCESS macro for SPSS, SAS, and R. Accessed April 14, 2023. https://www.processmacro.org/index.html
  • 27. Kane L, Ashbaugh AR. Simple and parallel mediation: a tutorial exploring anxiety sensitivity, sensation seeking, and gender. Quant Meth Psychol . 2017; 13 (3): 148–165. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 28. Pinkerton RS, Hinz LD, Barrow JC. The college student-athlete: psychological considerations and interventions. J Am Coll Health . 1989; 37 (5): 218–226. 10.1080/07448481.1989.9939063 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 29. Ivarsson A, Johnson U, Podlog L. Psychological predictors of injury occurrence: a prospective investigation of professional Swedish soccer players. J Sport Rehabil . 2013; 22 (1): 19–26. 10.1123/jsr.22.1.19 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 30. Allen MS, Greenlees I, Jones M. An investigation of the five-factor model of personality and coping behaviour in sport. J Sports Sci . 2011; 29 (8): 841–850. 10.1080/02640414.2011.565064 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 31. Todd M, Brown C. Characteristics associated with superstitious behavior in track and field athletes: are there NCAA divisional level differences? J Sport Behav . 2003; 26 (2): 168–187. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 32. Van de Mortel TF. Faking it: social desirability response bias in self-report research. Austral J Adv Nurs . 2008; 25 (4): 40–48. 10.3316/informit.210155003844269 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 33. Nicholls AR, Polman R, Levy AR, Taylor J, Cobley S. Stressors, coping, and coping effectiveness: gender, type of sport, and skill differences. J Sports Sci . 2007; 25 (13): 1521–1530. 10.1080/02640410701230479 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 34. Anshel MH, Sutarso S. Relationships between sources of acute stress and athletes’ coping style in competitive sport as a function of gender. Psychol Sport Exerc . 2007; 8 (1): 1–24. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.01.003 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • View on publisher site
  • PDF (129.4 KB)
  • Collections

Similar articles

Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.

  • Download .nbib .nbib
  • Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

Add to Collections

IMAGES

  1. Mental Health of Athletes Term Paper Example

    college athletes mental health essay

  2. Emotional Health Management in Athletes

    college athletes mental health essay

  3. "The Impact Of Social Media On The Mental Health Of Student-Athletes

    college athletes mental health essay

  4. The Place of Mental Preparation of Athletes

    college athletes mental health essay

  5. Student-athletes deserve more mental health resources after COVID-19’s

    college athletes mental health essay

  6. The Daily Northwestern

    college athletes mental health essay

VIDEO

  1. DAY IN THE LIFE: The Stress of a College Athlete (school, sports, job)

  2. Oral Presentation