U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Hunters and Gatherers of Pictures: Why Photography Has Become a Human Universal

Leopold kislinger.

1 Independent Researcher, Leonding, Austria

Kurt Kotrschal

2 Department of Behavioral Biology and Konrad Lorenz Forschungsstelle, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

3 Domestication Lab at the Konrad-Lorenz Institute of Ethology, Wolf Science Center, University of Veterinary Medicine, Ernstbrunn, Austria

Associated Data

The original contributions presented in the analysis are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Photography is ubiquitous worldwide. We analyzed why people take, share, and use personal photographs, independent of their specific cultural background. These behaviors are still poorly understood. Experimental research on them is scarce. Smartphone technology and social media have pushed the success of photography, but cannot explain it, as not all smartphone features are widely used just because they are available. We analyzed properties of human nature that have made taking and using photographs functional behaviors. We did this based on the four levels, which Nikolaas Tinbergen suggested for analyzing why animals behave in a particular way. Including findings from multiple disciplines, we developed a novel conceptual framework—the “Mental Utilization Hypothesis of Photography.” It suggests that people adopt photography because it matches with core human mental mechanisms mainly from the social domain, and people use photography as a cognitive, primarily social coping strategy. Our framework comprises a range of testable predictions, provides a new theoretical basis for future empirical investigations into photography, and has practical implications. We conclude that photography has become a human universal, which is based on context-sensitive mental predispositions and differentiates itself in the social and societal environment.

Introduction

Photography is ubiquitous around the world, with the number of people taking and using personal photographs steadily increasing (Lee and Stewart, 2016 ; Canon, 2018 ). More than 90 percent of all photographs (henceforth photos ) are taken with smartphones (Carrington, 2020 ), and more than half of the world's population uses smartphones or mobile phones to take, view, and share photos (Statista, 2019 ; Kemp, 2021 ). Smartphones integrate photography with many other functions, notably with access to the internet and social media (Smith, 2011 ; GSMA and NTT DOCOMO, 2014 ). This has rapidly shifted photography from an exclusive activity of socio-economically capable minorities toward engaging a majority of the world's 7.8 billion people.

We examined the question why people take, view, own, share, and use personal photos, and why photos are important to them. We consider the distribution of smartphone technology and social media a precondition for the sweeping success of photography, but insufficient to explain it, as not all smartphone features or technologies are widely used just because they are available. The technology to make audio-recordings, for example, has not been adopted by many people (Milgram, 1976 ). Although smartphones are capable of easily recording the voices of loved ones, conversations, the sounds of a birthday party, or of a strange city, people rarely use this function (GSMA and NTT DOCOMO, 2014 ; Lutter et al., 2017 ).

There is extensive research on the psychological bases of pictorial representation and art (e.g., Deacon, 2006 ; Donald, 2006 ; Dutton, 2009 ). No theory, however, has suggested an integrated psychological basis of the wide range of photography-related behaviors. Photography differs significantly from other visual representation techniques. The invention and further technical developments in photography have conveyed images with characteristics that drawings, paintings, maps, or plans do not have: (a) photos are realistic in a special way; (b) photos are produced by technical devices; (c) part of the information in photos is there by chance; (d) people tend to believe that what they see in photos really happened that way; and (e) photos can be created easily, quickly and effortlessly. We will describe these properties in more detail at the beginning of the following section.

Milgram ( 1976 ) assumed that taking and using photos conveys specific abilities, which can be best understood if cameras and photos are regarded as “evolutionary developments” (p. 7). We followed this approach and hypothesized that the urge to take, view, share, and use photos is based on human nature (Wilson, 2012 ; Kotrschal, 2019 ), i.e., on evolved context-sensitive predispositions and mechanisms, mainly rooted in the social domain. We examined this hypothesis on the basis of the four levels of Tinbergen ( 1963 ) to analyze and explain “natural” traits, i.e., those which evolved via the Darwinian processes. These levels relate to (1) the physiological mechanisms underlying a certain behavior, (2) it's ontogeny, (3) evolutionary history, and (4) adaptative value. This frame guided half a century of behavioral research and may be considered the research program of organismic biology in general (Bateson and Laland, 2013 ; Nesse, 2013 ).

We place photography in the context of the coherent theory of the evolution of life (Darwin, 1859 , 1879/2004 ; Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ) and human nature as an outcome of this evolution. The four levels proposed by Tinbergen are the theoretical and practical formulation of this context. Since there is only a single Darwinian theory of evolution, and culture is part of human nature (Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ), the biological context should allow us to develop a unified explanation, coherent with contemporary knowledge particularly on the proximate mechanisms (i.e., current physiological mechanisms and their ontogeny). According to the four levels of Tinbergen, our central research questions are: What are the cognitive and physiological mechanisms underlying taking and using photos? How does taking and using photos develop ontogenetically?—which is important for understanding the development of inter-individual variation. What is the phylogenetic basis for photographic behavior? What may the functions and adaptative value of taking and using photos be? In this respect, a contribution of taking or using photos for survival and individual reproductive success may not be obvious in modern humans, but to qualify as an evolutionary function, the proof of a direct effect would not be needed. Rather, it would be sufficient to find a plausible positive effect on a person's social and mental well-being, which, in turn, on a population level, would entail a positive, supportive effect on societal and biological fitness.

Our aim was to create a theoretical framework, which describes why and in what way taking, viewing, sharing, and using personal photos are functional behaviors in terms of what is presently known about human nature. The development of this framework was based on the integration of available empirical findings on photography from multiple research areas with findings from biology, psychology, and neuroscience. We consider cultural and biological traits as closely interconnected and interacting in driving evolution and individual behavior (e.g., Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ; Kotrschal, 2019 ). To the best of our knowledge, a similarly comprehensive integration of findings into a coherent theoretical framework has not been attempted before. Our framework generates a number of predictions about the specific characteristics of personal photos and photography-related behaviors, which can be tested through empirical investigations.

Based on our framework and data on the global availability of smartphones and social media, we intended to show that photography qualifies as a human universal (Murdock, 1945 ; Brown, 1991 ; Antweiler, 2016 ; Christakis, 2019 ; Kotrschal, 2019 ). The concept human universal is traditionally associated with traits, activities, characteristics, or institutions, which are observed in all cultures and societies worldwide, like social organization, cooking, language, music, or weapons (Brown, 1991 ). According to this view, photography would not be a human universal. Historically, photography is a new development and did not exist in the traditional societies described by ethnology. For traits or behaviors, which have only recently become universal, Brown ( 1991 ) introduced the term “‘new’ universals” (p. 50). He cited dogs, tobacco, metal tools, and plastic containers as likely examples. Hence, according to Brown's classification, photography is a “new universal.” We describe photography as a human universal, which is based on context-sensitive predispositions, which differentiates itself over ontogeny in the societal environment. Our evolutionary approach does not suggest categorizing photography as a stereotypic behavior based on “innate” dispositions. In line with the present concepts of human social behavior and human universals, we emphasize context-sensitivity, inter-individual variability and individual uniqueness of photography-related behaviors within the frame of the human reaction norm (Woltereck, 1909 ), as comparable, for example, with language or music.

Materials and Methods

Specific characteristics of photographs and photography.

Our focus is on personal photography , that is, on photography-related behaviors, including taking, viewing, sharing, and using photos, which are performed for personal reasons and without commercial intent (Chalfen, 1987 ; Kindberg et al., 2005 ). In particular, we refer to photography-related behaviors, which people perform immediately and voluntarily (spontaneously), without intentional preparation or planning beforehand. We specifically referred to characteristics of photos and behaviors related to photos, which other representational pictures and behaviors associated with them do not have:

Photos Are Realistic Images

An object depicted in a photo can share a large number of visual features with the object that was seen in the environment at a specific point in time from a specific location (Bradley and Lang, 2007 ). Because of this characteristic, photos are called realistic images (DeLoache et al., 1998 ). When individuals see a photo, a retinal image can be formed, which is similar to the image that would be formed if they saw the represented event or object in the environment in real life (Perrett et al., 1991 ). When investigating the neural bases of recognizing or categorizing objects (e.g., faces, bodies, sites, or objects), neuroscientists and cognition researchers often assumed that there is an equivalence between the photographic representation and the perceptible object in the environment and presented photos of objects as stimuli instead of the real objects in question. Important psychophysiological mechanisms underlying photography-related behaviors are related to the fact that photos of objects elicit reactions in certain areas of the brain similar to events, which are effectively seen in the environmeint.

Photos Are Produced by Technical Devices

Drawings and paintings can also be realistic images. In contrast to photos, the creation of drawings and paintings involves the hands of the artists who created them, and important visual characteristics resulted from the dispositions, ideas and decisions of these artists. Photos are created by technical devices, and viewers know this fact.

Part of the Information Came Into the Photo by Chance

The people who use cameras choose a certain perspective, a certain frame and a certain moment when they press the shutter button. Photographers use this selection to control the characteristics and meanings of photos. In complex natural scenes, photographers cannot control all of the information that gets into the photos. Some information comes into the pictures by chance (Talbot, 1844/2011 ). This is hardly the case with representative drawings or paintings.

People Assume They See Reality in Photos

People tend to believe that what they see in photos really happened that way—even if photos are posed, manipulated or forged (Wade et al., 2002 ; Nightingale et al., 2017 ). This phenomenon is still poorly understood. It is possibly related to the knowledge of the viewers that they see a picture that was produced by a technical apparatus. This knowledge could be linked to the assumption that the picture is little affected by the personal attitudes and intentions of the person who made it (Miller, 1973 ; Gu and Han, 2007 ).

Photos Can Be Created and Understood Easily, Quickly, and Effortlessly

Unlike drawings, paintings, maps, or plans, photos can be created easily, quickly and effortlessly. Three-year-old children can take informative and expressive photos (Magnusson, 2018 ). Without complex knowledge or skills, people can take photos that they and other people find excellent (De Looper, 2016 ). Complex events represented by photos are quickly and easily understood. A single quick glance is enough for viewers to understand, for example, an interaction between two individuals (Hafri et al., 2013 ).

Taking, sharing, and using photos are not behaviors, which have all of a sudden appeared as something completely new and an emergent property of culture. We hypothesized that they are deeply rooted in organismic and cultural evolution. The basic cognitive and physiological factors underlying photography-related behaviors are common to all people. Some of these factors may vary relatively little between individuals, but others, for example, related to individual personality structure may show great inter-individual variability. But even such a pronounced inter-individual variability is far from random, as much of ontogeny seems to depend on context-sensitive human dispositions (e.g., Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ; Kotrschal, 2019 ). Such dispositions are the result of non-random interactions between genes, epigenetics, and the social and societal environments during ontogeny. They frame the way people tend to take, view, share, and use photos.

Empirical Data and Findings on Photography-Related Behaviors

Empirical data and findings on taking, viewing, recognizing, sharing, and using photos come from a variety of disciplines, such as psychology, neuroscience, human-computer interaction, and anthropology. In analyzing the questions on the level of the cognitive and physiological mechanisms underlying photography-related behaviors, we referred to studies that examined the following questions: Which cognitive processes in the brain play a special role in photographing (Barasch et al., 2017 ; Blitch, 2017 )? How do people's brain responses to photos they have taken themselves differ from their responses to photos taken by others (Sellen et al., 2007 ; St. Jacques et al., 2011 ; Diefenbach and Christoforakos, 2017 )? Which brain responses do photos elicit in which viewers see a person with whom they are connected through a close emotional relationship (Bartels and Zeki, 2004 ; Gobbini et al., 2004 ; Leibenluft et al., 2004 ; Master et al., 2009 ; Eisenberger et al., 2011 )? Which brain responses do photos evoke in which viewers see themselves (Devue et al., 2007 ; Butler et al., 2012 )? Which neural processes form the basis for viewers to find a picture beautiful or ugly (Kawabata and Zeki, 2004 ; Jacobs et al., 2012 )?

To describe the ontogenesis of photography-related behaviors, we refer to studies that examined the development of the ability to recognize the representational properties of photos (DeLoache et al., 1998 ; for review, see Bovet and Vauclair, 2000 ), as well as to studies, which examined the age at which children start taking photos and for what purposes they use cameras (Mäkelä et al., 2000 ; Sharples et al., 2003 ; GSMA and NTT DOCOMO, 2014 ).

In analyzing the evolutionary roots of photography-related behaviors, we refer to studies of the ability of non-human primates and other animals to recognize objects depicted in photos (Bovet and Vauclair, 2000 ; Kano and Tomonaga, 2009 ; Aust and Huber, 2010 ). Information was also provided by investigations into the question how people develop the ability to recognize objects pictured in photographs (Deregowski et al., 1972 ; Miller, 1973 ; Bovet and Vauclair, 2000 ).

Table 1 briefly summarizes some of the research that will be used to analyze the level related to the adaptative value of photography-related behaviors. Every single referenced study provides a number of answers that are not always consistent with the answers from the other studies. The answers given are therefore rather examples of content to which we refer in the article than representative information.

Questions and studies used to analyze the adaptative value of photography-related behaviors.

What do people photograph?Crandall et al., ; Hu et al., ; De Looper, Family, friends, themselves, pets, activities, celebrations, food, fashion, nature, famous places, and landmarks
In which situations and for what purposes do people take photos?Bourdieu, ; Chalfen, ; Kindberg et al., ; Barasch et al., People take photos of significant events for personal and/or social purposes
Does the emotional state of people influence whether and what they photograph?Chalfen, ; Gillet et al., ; Diefenbach and Christoforakos, Photographing is influenced by states in which people experience a pleasant event, which is also related to the processing of uncertainty
How does taking photos influence how the photographers and the photographed individuals experience a situation?Burgess et al., ; Mols et al., ; Diehl et al., Photographing increases the pleasant experience of a situation and the feeling of social connectedness
How does photographing an event affect how well photographers later remember this event?Henkel, ; Barasch et al., ; Blitch, ; Jain and Mavani, Photographers tend to remember better visual features of the photographed event later, but less non-visual features
What are the characteristics of photos that people consider successful or “good”?Kirk et al., ; Bakhshi et al., The photos have enough desirable visual and/or representational characteristics
What do people do with the photos they have taken?Schiano et al., ; Kindberg et al., ; Kirk et al., ; Broekhuijsen et al., People keep photos, edit some, share them, use them for social purposes and/or autobiographical remembering
How do people use the photos they have taken on online social networking services, and does seeing photos in social media affect the emotional state of the viewers?Krämer and Winter, ; Hu et al., ; Lee et al., ; Malik et al., ; Pittman and Reich, ; RSPH and YHM, People use photos to evoke attention and engagement, and to share important information. Seeing the photos affects emotional arousal and evaluations
How do people use photos in connection with courtship and mating behaviors?Piazza and Bering, ; Sedgewick et al., ; Gale and Lewis, People create and show photos of themselves in which they are represented in the way they want to be seen by potential sexual or romantic partners
What importance do personal photos have for families?Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton, ; Petrelli and Whittaker, ; Whittaker et al., ; Frohlich et al., Photos are one of the most precious possessions of families. Photos symbolize the roots, importance, or meaning of a family

Results: the Four Levels of Tinbergen ( 1963 ) As a Theory Frame

Psychophysiological mechanisms underlying photography-related behaviors.

Researchers have used photos as stimuli. Thus, quite some knowledge on the psychophysiological mechanisms involved in recognizing and viewing photos has accumulated, but experimental research on the mechanisms involved in taking photos is essentially lacking (except for Blitch, 2017 ). The success of photography, however, is primarily related to features of taking photos (Krämer and Winter, 2008 ; Hu et al., 2014 ; Lee et al., 2015 ; De Looper, 2016 ; Malik et al., 2016 ; Carrington, 2020 ). These include various activities and outcomes. These activities are, for example, associated with relating to individuals or objects as well as creating and appropriating images of them and their desirable properties. Outcomes may be associated with a sense of control and efficacy. The rapid global spread of photography was not driven by new opportunities to get, acquire, or exchange photos taken by other people, but mainly by the increased availability of inexpensive cameras, particularly smartphones, and opportunities to share one's own photos electronically. For this reason, we address in this section the specific mechanisms that form the neural basis of taking personal photos. The following description of the processing steps in taking pictures corresponds to hypothetical predictions. We mainly employ findings on processes in primates including humans from various contexts, which can be related to the psychophysiological mechanisms involved in taking photos. Figure 1 shows a hypothetical model including the major steps of taking a photo.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-654474-g0001.jpg

Hypothetical process model including the major processes and activities that occur in an individual who engages in taking a personal photo. The place within the sequence where the processes and activities are located indicates either when they occur or when they first occur. In order to keep the presentation clear, possible feedback effects of activities on the antecedent steps are not shown. Downward arrows mean “then occurs”; a horizontal arrow means “interacts with”.

Initial Steps in Taking Photos

The first steps in taking a photo do not involve conscious awareness (Custers and Aarts, 2010 ). A mother, for example, responds spontaneously to the happy expression on the face of her 6-year-old son at his birthday party, or a hiker responds to the overwhelming panorama at a mountain top. In these examples, the perceptual input activates a fast, low-level system of subcortical structures related to affective processing (Baxter and Murray, 2002 ; Pourtois et al., 2013 ), including neurons responding to the visual information and others responding to relevance and information related to primary (evolutionarily developed) or individually acquired reward value. Some of these structures project to the midbrain dopaminergic system (Dommett et al., 2005 ; Schultz, 2006 ). In turn, dopaminergic projections from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) in the midbrain reach the ventral striatum, including the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), amygdala, hippocampus and other areas of the mesolimbic system (Berridge and Robinson, 1998 ; Alcaro et al., 2007 ), which functions as the central neural basis for approach and motivation. This mesolimbic system overlaps with the social behavior network in the brain, responsible for the control of social behavior (Goodson, 2005 ; O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011 ).

The activity of the dopaminergic neurons in the brain of the mother who sees her happy son corresponds to a “wanting” reaction (Berridge and Robinson, 1998 ). It makes her son's excited face salient and attractive. The fact that the mother likes what she perceives may be related to the release and processing of endogenous opioids (Panksepp, 1998 ; Kringelbach and Berridge, 2009 ; Hsu et al., 2013 ). Whereas dopamine conveys motivational incentives, endogenous opioids convey “liking,” but also have a calming effect and reduce neural responses to pain, stress and anxiety (Carter, 1998 ; De Kloet et al., 2005 ).

In everyday life, people usually take photos of pleasant events (Chalfen, 1987 ; Sharples et al., 2003 ; Hu et al., 2014 ). We assume, however, that the motivation to take a picture is often also related to the activation of a mental representation of a negative context, which is processed non-consciously. In our example, this negative context would be that the mother knows that her son celebrates his last birthday party before entering school. As her own mental representations of school are ambivalent, she develops an anticipatory concern regarding the situation of her son, which is threatening and creates mental stress (Ulrich-Lai and Herman, 2009 ). Representations of such threats correlate particularly with activities in the amygdala (Baxter and Murray, 2002 ; Pourtois et al., 2013 ), triggering a cascade of adaptive neural and neuroendocrine reactions (De Kloet et al., 2005 ; Schiller et al., 2008 ; Ulrich-Lai and Herman, 2009 ; Hostinar et al., 2014 ). They include the activation of the stress systems leading to an increase in excitement and alertness.

Hence, we suggest that two conflicting representations are activated in the mother's brain, each associated with a different behavioral response than the other. The mother needs to mobilize cognitive and behavioral resources to be able to balance the two possible meanings and reactions, which in essence employ different parts of her brain. The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) plays an important role in this. The ACC lies inside the frontal cortex, where it extends around the dorsal side of the corpus callosum, the nerve tract that connects the two cerebral hemispheres. It integrates and organizes emotional and cognitive information related to coping with pain, fear, anxiety, and stress, and potential motor responses, and is a major neural basis of cognitive control (Bush et al., 2000 ; Shenhav et al., 2013 ). Cognitive control is defined as regulating reactions to pieces of information that are in conflict with one another and in which automated processing may lead to errors (Miller and Cohen, 2001 ). The goal of cognitive control is to integrate conflicting information into representations that support appropriate behavioral decisions.

The mother's medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) signals that there is something out there that offers the opportunity to collect or appropriate something valuable—mPFC is a central part of the neural basis of appropriating or collecting something (Anderson et al., 2005 ; Turk et al., 2011 ). Based on the dopaminergic processes involved, the motivation for appropriating something can be very strong: mPF and ACC have the greatest densities of dopaminergic projections from the midbrain of all areas in the cortex (Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1993 ; Cohen et al., 2002 ).

Based on her photography-related knowledge, the mother categorizes what she perceives as “something that is photographed.” What is going on out there, could enable her to create a valuable picture. According to the assumptions of Event Cognition (Newtson, 1973 ; Zacks et al., 2001 ), “a children's birthday party” is not represented in the mother as a continuous, uniform event, but in the form of a few interconnected discrete units or steps, such as welcoming the guests, eating the birthday cake, blowing out the candles, and so on. The mother has detected that such a discrete step of the party has occurred. A photo of it could represent much of her son's birthday party. Activation patterns in prefrontal and hippocampal areas switch on photography-related memory contents that are connected to one another and retained in various locations widely spread over the cerebral cortex (Tonegawa et al., 2015 ). Context and scene are associated with possible outcomes of taking a photo with a smartphone camera, including a coarse anticipatory representation of the possible photo and its use.

Still without the involvement of conscious processes, the representation of the goal to take a photo is activated in structures of the mother's anterior prefrontal Cortex (PFC) (Soon et al., 2008 ; Custers and Aarts, 2010 ). Processes in OFC, mPFC, ACC, and ventral striatum analyze whether the goal to take a photo can be achieved in the given situation, and whether it is worth the effort. The result is the decision that the photo is worth the effort.

Steps Accompanied by Conscious Awareness

For taking the photo, representations from different explicit and implicit memory and processing systems must be integrated. Our mother is now consciously recognizing (Dehaene and Naccache, 2001 ; Damasio, 2010 ) that she is perceiving something that might be worth photographing. She takes her smartphone and points the camera at her son, who is surrounded by friends. She controls what will be seen in the picture. OFC, ACC, amygdala, and the anterior insula build the neural bases of various valuation, filtering, ordering and decision processes (Hsu et al., 2005 ). The mother's working memory (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974 ; Miller and Cohen, 2001 ) processes, maintains and integrates different pieces of information of internal and external origin.

The mother takes a photo of her son, a person with whom she is connected through a close positive emotional relationship. Seeing him activates areas in the mother's brain that have a high density of the peptide hormones oxytocin and vasopressin (Bartels and Zeki, 2004 ). Oxytocin and vasopressin are produced in the hypothalamic Nucleus preopticus (NPO), stored in pituitary, and are involved in the development and maintenance of close selective social relationships (Carter, 1998 ; Panksepp, 1998 ; Scheele et al., 2013 ; Hostinar et al., 2014 ). They also support the control and suppression of threat-related information (Nelson and Panksepp, 1998 ; Donaldson and Young, 2008 ; Scheele et al., 2013 ). Particularly oxytocin is involved in the development and maintenance of close selective social relationships or attachment and conveys the feeling of social connectedness (Carter, 1998 ; Panksepp, 1998 ; Scheele et al., 2013 ; Hostinar et al., 2014 ). Both hormones are associated with activating the mesolimbic reward system (Donaldson and Young, 2008 ). Oxytocin release correlates with opioid activities, reduces stress and thereby causes a calming effect (Nelson and Panksepp, 1998 ). In fact, there is a strong antagonism between oxytocin release and glucocorticoids synthesis, i.e., metabolic hormones that are produced and released in response to stressors (Carter, 1998 ; Hostinar et al., 2014 ; Preckel et al., 2015 ).

The mother's vmPFC assigns a positive value to the neural representations of the situation, photographing in general, and the intended photo in particular. On a non-conscious processing level, however, the anticipatory representation of the threat of her son's potentially negative experiences at school is still effective. This threat is primarily processed in the amygdala, but the mother's vmPFC projects into the amygdala and, thereby, inhibits its activity, which reduces fear and anxiety (Andolina et al., 2013 ; Hostinar et al., 2014 ). In addition, vmPFC, OFC and ACC project to the hypothalamus and reduce the activity of the mother's stress systems (Ulrich-Lai and Herman, 2009 ; Hostinar et al., 2014 ). Her implicit processing mechanisms suggest that she can now safely ignore the threat (Schiller et al., 2008 ).

When she recognizes a sufficient correspondence between the characteristics of the picture on the smartphone display and the mental representation of the desired photo, she presses the shutter button. She creates a permanent external picture of her son in a particular context, a representational digital object, which she possesses and can share with others. An important part of the value the picture has for her is related to the fact that she has created it herself. Actually, people can reliably distinguish between photos that they have taken themselves and photos taken by others (Sellen et al., 2007 ; St. Jacques et al., 2011 ).

A mountain hiker who discovers something she wants to photograph may have a different experience than a mother at her son's birthday party. She likes to hike alone and enjoys nature and silence. When looking at the mountain landscape, the anticipation of a longer period of non-self-chosen solitude has been activated. The hiker can take a picture, which will allow her to share her experience with her friends. Unlike our example mother, the hiker has more time for taking the picture, because the landscape does not change as quickly as social situations at a party. The hiker can use this time for creatively composing a photo, which will be different from ordinary photos depicting similar landscapes and which the viewers will find beautiful, useful, or important (Thagard and Stewart, 2011 ; Ellamil et al., 2012 ). She associates and integrates the incoming visual information with certain conceptual and emotional categories as well as with internal representations of existing extraordinary landscape pictures. The neural bases of these operations include structures of two cortical networks that are usually not active at the same time. One of these networks is activated when people focus their attention on external stimuli, the other network when attention is focused on thoughts, memories or imagery (De Pisapia et al., 2016 ).

Ontogeny of Recognizing, Taking, and Using Photos

Human babies recognize certain photos at an age of 3 months or even earlier (for review, see Bovet and Vauclair, 2000 ). In a cross-cultural study, DeLoache et al. ( 1998 ) showed that 9 months old babies treated pictured objects as if they were real objects, explored them with their hands, tried to touch them, or to take them out of the picture. At the age of 19 months, human children understood that pictures are both concrete real objects, but also representations of other objects. From about 1-year of age, children begin to create traces on two-dimensional surfaces with suitable materials (Thomas and Silk, 1990 ; Wright, 2010 ). At the age of two, children begin to name the meanings of their drawings or paintings. They also know that pictures are made with specific intentions to represent objects or events (Preissler and Bloom, 2008 ). Children aged 3- to 4-years know what properties of pictures are helpful if they are used to convey ideas of objects to other people, and that there are better and worse pictures for this purpose (Allen et al., 2010 ). They know that pictures containing a lot of visual details are best used to tell others what objects look like.

Many children like to draw. As much as they develop joy and zeal in drawing, they usually have little interest in owning the pictures as soon as they are done (Thomas and Silk, 1990 ; Cox, 2005 ; Cherney et al., 2006 ; Wright, 2010 ). If they have mastered a special pictorial challenge, they proudly show their picture and look at it together with others, but they do not go for drawings they made the week or the month before to look at them again. The fascination lies in the activity of drawing itself, in experiencing the ability to create a picture with a certain meaning—and to use this to relate to others (Cox, 2005 ; Wright, 2010 ). The early ontogenetic development of competences related to producing and using representational pictures happens in the social environment, usually the family. The family is also one of the most productive places of personal photography (Chalfen, 1987 ; Petrelli and Whittaker, 2010 ). The most successful photography exhibition of all time even had “family” in its title: The Family of Man ( 1955 ).

Children see the photos their parents keep in photo albums, photo books, boxes or computer folders. The photos of the ancestors—and their actions, experiences, relationships, occupations, and possessions—that a family owns can give children a sense of social belonging, societal significance, and security (Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton, 1981 ; Chalfen, 1987 ; Petrelli and Whittaker, 2010 ). These photos are heritable assets of knowledge. They are usually linked to oral or written information, which shows and tells to whom the children belong and whom they can trust. Through mechanisms of social learning, family traditions of photography emerge (Mäkelä et al., 2000 ; Sharples et al., 2003 ; Petrelli and Whittaker, 2010 ). Children get to know certain ways of using cameras and photos early on (Mäkelä et al., 2000 ; Sharples et al., 2003 ). They experience how their mother or father reacts to certain events by taking photos—usually positive, which supports this behavior via positive reinforcement learning. Children also realize that taking, viewing, and sharing photos is repeatedly done in certain social contexts, for example at a birthday party, graduation, or wedding. They learn that photographers keep some pictures and discard others and may shape their own taste along this.

Many children start taking photos themselves at preschool age (Sharples et al., 2003 ; Magnusson, 2018 ). Seven- to 15-year-olds take and use photos in connection with the playful and explorative use of electronic devices (Mäkelä et al., 2000 ; Sharples et al., 2003 ). They use cameras and photos for joking, like making faces or adopting funny poses, for expressing feelings, or telling stories. According to an international survey, 81% of the 8- to 18-year-olds in Algeria, Egypt, Iraq, and Saudi Arabia used a mobile phone in 2013 (GSMA and NTT DOCOMO, 2014 ). Most of the children got their own mobile phone between 10- and 12-years of age and 55% had access to the internet. The features most used by children and adolescents were cameras (91%), followed by music players and video players. Many young people in their teens and early twenties take and use photos to create a sense of self and an identity (Schiano et al., 2002 ; RSPH and YHM, 2017 ). Social media provides them with a platform where they can use photos to express different characteristics of themselves and to experience other people's reactions (Krämer and Winter, 2008 ; Hu et al., 2014 ; Lee et al., 2015 ). Photos of family members and pals are especially important for people (Mäkelä et al., 2000 ; Sharples et al., 2003 ), and the value of these photos increases with the age of their owners (Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton, 1981 ).

Evolutionary Roots of Taking and Using Photos

Why do photos have the characteristics they have? Why are they important for people all over the world? Which meanings can almost only be represented and communicated through photos Kislinger, 2021 ), and which cannot? In this section, we will refer to cognitive and social building blocks, which are part of the evolved nature of modern humans and suggest answers to these questions. Figure 2 presents an overview of the evolutionary building blocks that underlie the success of photography to which we refer.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-654474-g0002.jpg

Schematic illustration of the building blocks that are part of the evolved nature of modern humans and form the basis of the importance of photos for people. The arrows show the order in which we describe the building blocks in our model.

Vision as a Central Element of Human Cognition

People take and use photos to represent important events in the environment. Representing features of the environment with survival value is one of the core functions of central nervous systems (CNSs) since they exist. Organisms developed sensory organs, which react to relevant physical and chemical events in the environment, as well as neurons, that is, cells capable of receiving, generating, and transducing signals for internal communication and for relating to the environment (Butler and Hodos, 2005 ; Gregory, 2008 ). By means of neural activation patterns, organisms have used “images” for hundreds of millions of years (Damasio, 2010 )—as representations of the environment enhancing predictability in interaction with this environment and, thus, survival. Although it is hard to imagine how a jellyfish with its dispersed nervous system should be able to form an image-like mental representation, the fact that its body responds to stimuli in a coordinated and adaptive way at least hints at such a possibility.

Mammals evolved out of mainly visually oriented reptiles (Northcutt, 2011 ; Striedter, 2020 ). During their first 100 million years of evolution, however, the reign of dinosaurs forced them underground or into a nocturnal lifestyle. This led to a reduction in visual orientation, while olfaction and hearing were optimized. Within the modern mammals we see a full reinstatement of trichromatic vision only in the primates, while most other mammals remain bi-chromatic as an adaptation to being active at dusk and dawn and at night. Due to specific properties of their central nervous systems, including retinae, primates can extract a broad range of information from the properties of light and its reflections in the physical world (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991 ; Gollisch and Meister, 2010 ). Vision is a central component of human cognition. Visual content dominates, for example, perception, memory, imagining, and dreaming (Posner et al., 1976 ; Zimmermann, 1989 ).

Living Together in Groups and Social Attachment

People primarily take photos of other people, especially people to whom they are emotionally connected (Chalfen, 1987 ; Hu et al., 2014 ; Lee et al., 2015 ). Processing stimuli with social significance has a long evolutionary history (Wilson, 1984 , 2012 ). The tegmental and diencephalic parts of the brains of birds, bony fish, and mammals feature an evolutionary extremely conservative—hence homologous—social behavior network (Goodson, 2005 ). This regulates social recognizing, mating, parental behavior, persistent bonding, expressive behavior, aggression, and responses to social stressors. Primates inherited this network virtually unchanged in structure and function from their ancestors. The primate ancestors of humans established close relationships with other individuals in their groups who were not reproductive partners or relatives (De Waal and Brosnan, 2006 ; Wilson, 2012 ). Social cohesion improved the ability of individuals and groups to adapt to variable environments, to survive and to reproduce. Living together in groups affected both behavior and cognition. In primates, the social domain hosts a substantial part of the motivation to orient to and perceive stimuli, and to carry out certain behaviors and actions. Important and mutually linked social behavior systems are attachment and care , that is, a close selective emotional connection with another individual—the caregiving attachment figure, or the other way round, the attached dependent (Bowlby, 1974 ; Carter, 1998 ; Panksepp, 1998 ). There is a strong antagonistic interaction between the feelings of safe attachment and distress (Panksepp, 1998 ). Threatening or stressful situations elicit the desire for social closeness, and societal cohesion increases in times of crisis. Support by an attachment figure provides a sense of security and calmness. Conversely, being isolated from attachment figures or other socially supportive individuals is perceived as a potential threat. This antagonistic interaction is relevant in terms of photography-related behaviors. Photos of attachment figures or of the attached dependents convey important potentials. In experiments, for example, merely seeing the photo of an attachment figure reduced physical pain as effectively as the actual closeness to that person (Master et al., 2009 ; Eisenberger et al., 2011 ).

Social Learning, Cultural Evolution, and Symbol Systems

An important ability of animals living in groups is profiting from experiences or interactions with other individuals, called social learning (Richerson et al., 2010 ; Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ). A system of characteristic behavior patterns and preferences, which are socially passed on through generations, is referred to as culture and its gradual change as cultural evolution (Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ). Cultural phenomena have probably played a greater role in human evolution than in any of the other animals showing cultural diversification (for example wolves or orcas) ever since the common ancestors of humans and chimpanzees (Richerson et al., 2010 ; Whiten, 2011 ).

Social learning is the base for tradition forming and transferring information via culture. The ancestors of humans used gestures, vocalizations, and found objects as signs for something that was not currently present in the environment to communicate with others (Seyfarth et al., 2005 ; Deacon, 2006 ; Arbib et al., 2008 ). Over many generations, groups gradually developed a complex system of gestural and vocal signs, as well as rules specifying how these signs were to be combined into larger units of meaning (Seyfarth et al., 2005 ; Arbib et al., 2008 ). As a crucial step in human evolution, humans began to use symbols , this is, signs that represent meanings based on rules and conventions. Symbols are part of an evolved cultural system, which regulates the relationships between individual signs and indicates how they are combined to represent units of meaning (Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ). The use of symbols for organizing and conveying information was a crucial step in human evolution. Human symbols are considered as discrete dimensions of inheritance and evolution which interact with genetic evolution. People developed systems of symbols to represent and communicate knowledge, rules and ideas (Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ; Tomasello, 2014 ). Language became the most important symbol system, likely also pushing brain development. Cultural evolution and genetic evolution interacted and led to a positive feedback selection between cognitive mechanisms, language, and social skills (Deacon, 1997 ; Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ).

Cooperation, Property, Status, Reputation, Courtship, and Mating

Among the evolutionary mechanisms, which favored cooperation in groups, direct and indirect reciprocity appear to be particularly relevant (Nowak, 2006 ). These mechanisms are also relevant in terms of taking and using photos. Direct reciprocity is effective when two individuals encounter each other repeatedly: one cooperates assuming that the other one will reciprocate later. Cooperation, hence, benefits both. The mechanism of indirect reciprocity explains cooperation in situations where one individual helps another individual whom the individual may not meet again or from whom no help is expected. This can still pay off, if the helpful behavior is observed by other group members. Indirect reciprocity describes the benefit of an altruistic act for the helping individual, which spreads via gossip or other information. In this detour, the helping individual acquires the reputation of being “generous,” i.e., able and willing to cooperate. This reputation supports access to resources and reproductive success (Nowak and Sigmund, 2005 ; Nowak, 2006 ). With the evolution of complex language—and later with the distribution of photos—the subset of a population that could receive information about the cooperative potential of an individual tremendously increases as compared to the number of people able to directly observe an individual's behavior. Photography and social networking services on the internet have increased the potential audience enormously.

In human societies, it is generally advantageous to regulate resource use and ownership through rules or conventions in order to avoid costly redundant conflicts (Stake, 2004 ). Depending on socio-economic background, people have developed specific rules about the appropriation of things as well as about the retention and distribution of property (Stake, 2004 ). Many animals appropriate things and retain them (Stake, 2004 ). Property-related experiences and behaviors are based on specific neural substrates, especially in the frontal cortex (Anderson et al., 2005 ; Turk et al., 2011 ). The brain structures involved are particularly rich in dopamine receptors. The acquisition of property is accordingly associated with strong motivation. When individuals acquire and possess valuable resources, it may also be beneficial to their status within their groups, or may even be to the benefit and status of these groups (Brown, 1991 ; Van Vugt and Tybur, 2016 ). Much of human social complexity is about status and prestige. This modulates, in turn, individual access to resources in a social dynamic between cooperation and competition (Nowak and Sigmund, 2005 ; Van Vugt and Tybur, 2016 ). Individuals can display their property and signal that they have a certain status within the social and cultural hierarchies of their group or society. To communicate this status, individuals may use symbols of their possessions. Individuals can also share their resources with others, be generous or even wasteful with their possessions to increase their prestige and, ultimately, their reproductive success (Buss and Schmitt, 1993 ). In women and men, the acquisition, retention, and use of resources or possession have specific characteristics (Brown, 1991 ; Buss and Schmitt, 1993 ).

Photography has provided people with effective means to signal their social status to a large audience (Krämer and Winter, 2008 ; Piazza and Bering, 2009 ). Distributing selfies with famous people or in front of famous sights, for example, is motivated by telling others about one's own potential to meet these famous people or to travel, and to communicate one's own interests and attitudes (Krämer and Winter, 2008 ; Diefenbach and Christoforakos, 2017 ). Many people also take and share status-relevant photos of themselves with their “belongings,” such as house, car, boat, their beautiful partner, or children (De Looper, 2016 ; Jain and Mavani, 2017 ). Empirical data suggest that people also use photos for enhancing their mate value in the minds of potential romantic or sexual partners (Piazza and Bering, 2009 ; Smith, 2016 ; Hobbs et al., 2017 ; Sedgewick et al., 2017 ; Gale and Lewis, 2020 ; Kemp, 2021 ; Morris, 2021 ). We will discuss this in more detail in the section on the functions of photography.

Memory and the Urge to Create Coherent Explanations for Events and Conditions

Humans improved their ability to use language to categorize behaviors, events, objects and states. They developed a special system of comprehensive memory for experiences, including social, called episodic memory (Tulving, 2005 ). Thereby, experiences of “what,” “with whom,” “when,” and “how it felt” are integrated in a way that individuals can consciously access their stored representations and have a comprehensive awareness of their own life as related to others. With the ability to represent, process, and communicate past and future, as well as possible or imagined events through language, came the urge to explain what happens in the world, to interpret the past and to predict the future (Pettitt, 2011 ). Humans developed an awareness of mortality, thinking about death, and the desire to overcome mortality. The earliest burial sites found with material traces of ritual practices are around 100,000-years old (Pettitt, 2011 ; Wilson, 2012 ). The desire for extending one's effectiveness beyond lifespan could also play a role in taking pictures (Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton, 1981 ; Chalfen, 1987 ). Many people retain photos of ancestors in a respectful way, in the implicit understanding that their descendants will do the same. This is reminiscent of animistic cultures, where identity and existence of people are deeply rooted in cults around ancestors (Frazer, 1911 ; Bird-David, 1999 ).

When taking a photo of another person, the photographer not just appropriates a picture of the light reflections from this person, but also of the visual, behaviorally relevant signals that this person emits at that particular moment. This may be part of the reason why many people consider appropriating a picture of a person to have “magical” (Frazer, 1911 ; Kittredge, 1929 ) or “animistic” (Bird-David, 1999 ; Harvey, 2005 ) properties. The term “animistic” refers to the belief that not only humans, but also animals, plants, lakes, mountains, etc. have souls and are animated (Harvey, 2005 ). With taking a picture of a certain person her or his personality and even “soul” may be captured, and the owner of this picture can change the condition of the pictured person—with potentially negative consequences (Hetherwick, 1902 ; Frazer, 1911 ; Hocart, 1922 ). Image magic has a long tradition going back far into human prehistory (Kittredge, 1929 ). Today, there is an ongoing struggle for legal regulation of the protection of one's image as part of personal rights and property rights, indicating that personal images still retain their special private status. Even on a rational base, the power that is conveyed by taking, owning, and using photos [e.g., Regulation (EU), 2016 ], is still a delicate topic in modern Western societies.

Language enables humans to integrate a huge amount of information into meaningful contexts and to create explanations of events in which these events appear ordered and understandable toward a goal, rather than meaningless, accidental and pointless (Kahneman, 2011 ). In addition to language, an evolutionarily older cognitive system remained (Evans, 2008 ; Kahneman, 2011 ), providing quick reactions to relevant events in the environment on the basis of minimal sensory information, for example, via faces with emotional expressions or expressive body poses (Kislinger, 2021 ). Certain events depicted in photos cause activations of evolutionarily old brain structures, like superior colliculus, pulvinar, and amygdala (Morris et al., 1999 ; Van Le et al., 2013 ; Almeida et al., 2015 ). Objects and events pictured in photos are not only recognized by humans, but by many other species (Bovet and Vauclair, 2000 ; Kano and Tomonaga, 2009 ). In some cases, the last common ancestor of humans and a species in question lived long ago, e.g., 220 million years in the case of pigeons (Aust and Huber, 2010 ). This either hints at an ancient ability shared via phylogenetic inheritance (homology) or at parallel evolution (analogy).

Functions and Adaptative Value of Photography

A “function” of a behavior describes a specific contribution of the individual expression of this behavior to survival and reproductive success (Jablonka and Lamb, 2014 ). Photography-related behaviors touch the evolutionary functional domains of well-being and social connectedness, which are at the core of human nature. These behaviors will therefore, directly or indirectly, relate to potential individual societal and—ultimately—reproductive success. We suggest that taking, owning, viewing, sharing, and using photos provide a specific and effective strategy for coping with complex environments fraught with uncertainty. Photography as a coping-strategy comprises four core domains: (1) making sense, (2) appropriating an image, (3) establishing and supporting social connectedness, and (4) courtship and mating. These four domains can be involved in different photography-related behaviors to different degrees.

Making Sense

“Making sense” plays a role in many photography-related behaviors (Harrison, 2002 ; Frohlich et al., 2013 ); it is particularly evident in the taking of photos (Chalfen, 1987 ; Gillet et al., 2016 ). Thereby, people assign a certain cause to an event—that is, they create an explanation for why this event occurs—or a certain meaningful order, which is consistent with a goal. The 6-year-old birthday boy from our example above laughs because he gets along well with other children, and other people want him to be happy. The photo of the hiker shows that being alone on a mountain top is great, because it gives one a deep personal feeling for nature, which still can be shared with friends via a picture. According to our framework, people build mental representations, which make an event understandable. As a consequence, the future course of the event appears predictable and controllable. Taking photos allows making sense immediately and intuitively, without the involvement of complex reasoning.

Sharing and viewing photos can also be used for making sense of events. The photo of a family reunion can show a group of laughing people who relate to each other in a friendly and nice way, even if a heated argument broke out at this meeting, which may have led to long-term insults and resentments. Particularly, people who were at this meeting can look at this photo to reassure themselves that, despite certain controversy, things are fine and people like each other. This is supported by the propensity of viewers to assume that what they see in photos reflects reality. Understanding photos does not require the mastery of a particular language, complex cultural knowledge, or elaborate thinking. A single photo can give a fairly comprehensive idea of an event—possibly better than any verbal description: “a picture is worth a thousand words” (The Post-Standard, 1911 ).

Appropriating an Image

The domain “appropriating an image” is related to the fact that people gain permanent access to valuable information by taking, sharing, or getting a photo—most frequently related to social relationships. Photographers relate to an event or object through the camera, select certain properties of this object and the scene in which it is contained, and create a focus. From the flow of the object's appearances, which they perceive over a certain period of time, they extract a single picture and fix it. It represents only a small fraction of the sensory information that is available in that situation. By selecting and organizing the information, which they include in the picture, they interpret the object or event. If they have managed to create the picture with the intended meaning, this success conveys the experience of effectiveness and competence (Krämer and Winter, 2008 ). This experience reduces emotional arousal and physiological stress responses to potential threats and supports coping with them (Bandura, 1997 ). A man looking out the window of the plane that is taking off, for example, may take a number of photos, thereby potentially also coping with his fear of flying. Taking photos may help to maintain control in a potentially stressful situation.

Establishing and Supporting Social Connectedness

Seeing an important person in a photo allows the viewer to relate emotionally to this person, although she or he is absent or may have passed away. Photos of their own children, parents, or romantic partners are particularly important to people (Bartels and Zeki, 2004 ; Gobbini et al., 2004 ; Leibenluft et al., 2004 ; Petrelli and Whittaker, 2010 ; Hu et al., 2014 ). Photos of loved ones enable people to feel close to them, provide a sense of security and calmness and reduce the sensation of pain (Master et al., 2009 ; Eisenberger et al., 2011 ). Photos, to some extent, can substitute for physical closeness. Viewing, owning, or sharing photos of family members or ancestors support developing cultural and genealogical roots (Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton, 1981 ; Petrelli and Whittaker, 2010 ). Hence, taking and using photos relates to establishing, maintaining and strengthening social connections (Kindberg et al., 2005 ; Barasch et al., 2014 ; Lee et al., 2015 ; Pittman and Reich, 2016 ).

Many people share their photos, and if a photo is liked and appreciated by others, the photographer experiences self-efficacy (Krämer and Winter, 2008 ) and self-esteem (Burrow and Rainone, 2017 ). This is exploited by the “like buttons,” an enormously popular feature of social media platforms (Kemp, 2021 ). Sharing photos contributes to a common understanding of the world. Sharing photos also enables people to convey others views that they enjoy, e.g., photos of hilarious events or natural sceneries. Photos of natural scenes (as opposed to human artifacts or urban environments) have a positive influence on the well-being of viewers (Berto, 2005 ; Valtchanov and Ellard, 2015 ) as “Biophilia” is a human universal (Appleton, 1975 ; Ulrich, 1983 ; Wilson, 1984 ; Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989 ). Viewing such photos relaxes, reduces emotional stress, and thereby regenerates depleted cognitive resources.

People often use photos to show others who they are and what role they play in society. Issues of identity, reputation, prestige, or status often play a role in personal photography (Chalfen, 1987 ; Barasch et al., 2014 ; RSPH and YHM, 2017 ). If one person photographs another person, this can be of value only for the photographer, or for both (Milgram, 1976 ). People can use photos to influence how other people perceive the pictured individuals, objects, or events and thus exert social control (Sharples et al., 2003 ; Diefenbach and Christoforakos, 2017 ). People being photographed, however, may also use this circumstance for their own goals, like for influencing how others perceive them (Harrison, 2002 ; Krämer and Winter, 2008 ; Jain and Mavani, 2017 ). Being photographed can immensely increase the size of the “audience.”

As humans are radically social in their nature, observing or monitoring the behavior of other people plays a central role in the motivation to use social media on the internet and to post photos (Joinson, 2008 ; Lee et al., 2015 ; Malik et al., 2016 ). People are usually aware of the presence of cameras. This may produce “audience effects,” i.e., the feeling of being watched influences behavior and makes people behave in a socially agreeable way (Bateson et al., 2006 ; Oda et al., 2015 ), by showing, for example, “photo faces.” Through this tendency, photography supports cooperative coexistence in complex societies and has an adaptative value both, on the individual and on the societal level.

We are well aware that people also distribute photos of atrocities. The impact of such photos can be used to boost the importance of the photographer or distributor, or even to hurt other people, to violate the rights of others, or to deceive (Smith et al., 2008 ; Kowalski et al., 2014 ). Manipulative to harmful photo use is facilitated by the fact that photographic forgeries are becoming increasingly difficult to detect, both in social and in journalistic media (Campbell, 2014 ; Nightingale et al., 2017 ). Various detrimental outcomes of taking and using photos have required legal regulation of photography-related behaviors [e.g., Regulation (EU), 2016 ]. The ubiquity of taking photos has massively reduced possibilities of intimacy and privacy. A vast dark side of photography exists outside of personal experience. The large social media providers use the shared personal photos as a data source. The acquisition of these data, their possession, the algorithms of their management and the extraction of information from them give the companies enormous power, which has not been put under democratic control until now (Zuboff, 2015 ).

Courtship and Mating

Courtship and mating are certainly part of the domain of establishing and strengthening social connections and attachment (Hazan and Shaver, 1987 ; Fisher et al., 2002 ). But they are directly relevant in terms of evolutionary function and as such encompass a range of distinct strategies and conflicts (Fisher et al., 2002 ; Buss and Duntley, 2011 ). The global prevalence of intimate partner homicide reflects the high value of the activities and resources that are at stake, as well as the severity of the conflicts in question (Stöckl et al., 2013 ). Sexual or reproductive behaviors shaped all living beings and played a central role in the evolution of human cognition (Miller, 2001 ; Nowak, 2006 ). Sexual themes and symbols are featured in some of the oldest preserved artifacts (Conard, 2009 ). With photos, a new type of visual cueing was developed that fulfills special functions in attracting potential partners, mate selection, and sexual behavior. The potentials of photography range from tender romantics to hardcore pornography.

“Beauty” plays a special role in this context. Many people want to take and use beautiful photos (Bakhshi et al., 2015 ; De Looper, 2016 ). Darwin ( 1879/2004 ) associated the “sense of beauty” (p. 114) with the context of sexual selection: the function of beauty is that the choosing female or male individuals are “excited” by it. Individuals considered to be beautiful manage to “excite attention” (p. 467). In this sense, beauty is a sensory signal that it could be advantageous to pay attention to, and approach, the sender of this signal. Among the hashtags (terms assigned to posted photos) that were most frequently used on Instagram in 2020, “Love” came first, “Art” fifth, and “Beautiful” sixth (Kemp, 2021 ). Instagram is the most photography-related social platform and was the fifth most visited website worldwide in 2020 (Kemp, 2021 ).

The invention of photography and its further technological developments, including digital communication, allowed people to create a new type of sensory cues relevant to courtship activities, mate selection, sexual intercourse, and (ultimately) reproduction. Photography has been used almost from the start to satisfy cravings for pictures of naked people and for erotic images. Retinal images of naked potential partners expressing interest in sexual activity has meant observers had access to reproduction for hundreds of thousands of years. Photos of sexual acts are among those images that are most emotionally arousing (Bradley and Lang, 2007 ; Wehrum et al., 2013 ) and pornography is one of the most prominent domains of internet use.

People also use photos to influence choices of potential romantic or sexual partners. The success of dating applications on the Internet has greatly increased the importance of photos in connection with courtship and mating (Piazza and Bering, 2009 ; Smith, 2016 ; Hobbs et al., 2017 ). Social media platforms and dating apps enable users to form relationships with people they have never seen before. Mobile dating applications are used by more and more people (Smith, 2016 ; Morris, 2021 ). People looking for partners create profiles on these apps that they use to present themselves. Photos of oneself play a central role in this. People show photos of themselves—often also taken by themselves—in which they are represented as they would like to be seen by potential romantic partners (Sedgewick et al., 2017 ; Gale and Lewis, 2020 ). The use of such photos enables people to reveal actual traits of themselves, but also to make themselves appear more attractive than they potentially are (Sedgewick et al., 2017 ; Gale and Lewis, 2020 ). People can also use symbolic self-made photos to create a desirable impression of themselves in potential romantic partners, for example photos of groups of nice, laughing people, pets, flowers, a beautiful garden, an elegant apartment, tourist attractions, dangerous environments, sporting events, or full bookshelves (Krämer and Winter, 2008 ; Piazza and Bering, 2009 ). Online dating is not only increasing rapidly among young adults, but also among the older population (Smith, 2016 ; Morris, 2021 ). Through dating apps, photos play an increasingly important role in mate selection, which played a central role in the evolution of human cognition (Miller, 2001 ). When photos are used in dating and courtship, there is also the characteristic connection between emotionally positive information and the processing of uncertainty (Berger and Calabrese, 1975 ; Knobloch and Solomon, 1999 ), addressed above. In this context, the positive information concerns one's own attractive properties. Uncertainty is associated with one's search, and potential negative outcomes of establishing relationships with people one does not know from face-to-face encounters.

The Mental Utilization Hypothesis of Photography

We propose that the success of smartphones as well as photography is based on core human mental mechanisms which are primarily related to the social domain. Photography exploits evolved cognitive and social predispositions. In this sense, our framework is a mental exploitation hypothesis, analogous to the Sensory Exploitation Hypothesis in evolutionary biology (e.g., Ryan, 1990 ). This hypothesis states that new preferences evolve along established pre-existing sensory biases and response tendencies, such as primates owing their social and/or sexual preference for red to their old predilection for this color, which usually indicates ripe fruits (Ghazanfar and Santos, 2004 ).

Sensory biases and preferences also play an important role in photography-related behaviors. The visual channel provides information, which is converted into, or affects, mental representations. In our framework, however, the focus is on a higher, more integrated level of processing, on which those mechanisms and functions are organized that control the mental representation of the world and flexibly adapt social behavior. In connection with photography, the term exploitation may have a negative connotation, such as photographers exploiting the people in front of the camera (e.g., Sontag, 1978 ). For this reason, we refer to our framework as the mental utilization hypothesis of photography. It suggests that photography fits the nature of human perception and mental processing like a key fits its lock.

Photography as a Coping Strategy

Along to the four levels of Tinbergen, our analysis of photography-related behaviors suggests that people take and use photos to cope with certain stressful and threatening events in specific ways. The conceptualization of photography as a coping strategy is counterintuitive against the background that people usually like to take photos and generally take, share and own photos of events associated with happiness, pleasure, love, or success (Chalfen, 1987 ; Sharples et al., 2003 ; Hu et al., 2014 ). Individuals who take or use such photos, we propose, experience a pleasant situation, but are also—non-consciously—exposed to threatening information or uncertainty. As examples, we mentioned the mother who photographs her 6-year-old son, the lonely hiker, and the man who is afraid of flying. Taking and using photos allows people to search for, and engage, in emotionally positive information. Successful coping through photography-related behaviors reduces complexity, uncertainty, and anxiety. Coping, or the exercise of cognitive control, does not have to be exclusively reactive, but can also be carried out proactively (Bandura, 1997 ). Coping through taking and using photos has features that can be described on a continuous scale, with reactive coping at one end and proactive coping at the other.

People use photography not only to cope with events with generally positive emotional value, but also in coping with negative events. For example, traffic accidents, high-rise fires or other disasters tend to lure in bystanders and onlookers taking smartphone photos of the scene or of the victims (Vollmuth, 2017 ; Newton, 2019 ). There is no research on the motives which drive such photography-related behaviors. They may be similar to the motives which make people watch horror or crime films (Bartsch and Mares, 2014 ). What people see confronts them with something extremely meaningful—threats that exist in the world, their own mortality and vulnerability (Arndt et al., 1997 ). Most of these bystanding photographers immediately share their products. Taking and sharing the photos, we suggest, enable people to make sense of threatening events to get along with them, but also use them to push their own importance and prestige within their networks.

Has Photography Become a Human Universal?

Several researchers discussed the creation and use of representational pictures as human universal (Deacon, 2006 ; Donald, 2006 ; Dutton, 2009 ). The creation of realistic visual pictures appeared more than 30,000-years ago and some of them have been preserved on cave walls (Guthrie, 2005 ; White et al., 2018 ). Photography, in connection with digital technology and smartphones, has made it possible for everyone to create, own, and share realistic pictures easily and effortlessly and to integrate such pictures in everyday life. Based on our analysis and statistical data (Statista, 2019 ; Carrington, 2020 ; Kemp, 2021 ), we conclude that taking, viewing, and sharing photos through the use of smartphones has become a human universal—a “new” universal, according to Brown's ( 1991 ) classification—that is based on context-sensitive predispositions, particularly connected with the radically social human nature, and differentiates itself in the societal domain (Kotrschal, 2019 ). Photography not only classifies as a human universal, but also as a unique human feature not shared with any other animal species—not only because other species lack the technical means of photography, but before all, they seem to lack the motivation and mental mechanisms behind the typical human urge to capture the world in images. We conclude that photography is closely matching the unique construction of the human mind and qualifies as a feature of human nature, i.e., the Conditio humana (Arendt, 1958 ; Kotrschal, 2019 ).

Figure 3 summarizes the conditions, components, and abilities that have made photography a human universal as proposed by the mental utilization hypothesis of photography. One element of Figure 3 relates to the specific social contexts and environmental features that generate photography-related behaviors, as suggested by the evolutionary building blocks of photography. They are (1) coexistence in large, complexly structured societies; (2) frequent encounters with strangers, the outcome of which is often difficult to predict; (3) strong mutual observation of behavior; (4) individuals' well-being and prosperity depend on judgments by strangers; (5) requirement to display one's own status symbolically in public; (6) continuous confrontation with the news of success or profit, as well as disaster, illness, or death; (7) large number of potential sexual or reproductive partners among strangers; (8) individuals have to make far-reaching decisions about their future lives; (9) requirement of communication with absent or distant people; and (10) requirement of quick communication with strangers across cultural or linguistic boundaries.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-654474-g0003.jpg

The Mental Utilization Hypothesis of Photography. The schematic illustration shows the proposed conditions, components, and abilities that made photography a human universal. An arrow means “provides the basis for” or “leads to”.

Limitations

The analysis of a particular behavior on the basis of the four levels of Tinbergen requires the integration of findings from a range of disciplines. Despite the referenced mechanisms and functions of taking photos, which represent the present state of knowledge, our conclusions remain necessarily speculative—because of the preliminary nature of all scientific results, because of the inherent pitfalls of attempting to integrate such diverse results into a comprehensive synthesis, and due to the space constraints of a journal article. In addition, there are very few empirical findings on taking photos, and they come only from the Western world. Thus, we may underestimate the cultural diversity in photography, although we are quite confident that the behavioral core is based on human nature, and therefore, should in principle, apply to all people. Within our conceptual frame we describe taking and using photos as functional outcomes of cognitive and social adaptations. It could certainly be argued that the success of photography is ultimately a byproduct of the accessibility, affordability and success of smartphones and social media, which results from marketing activities of powerful companies. But this is a different level argument not contradicting our utilization hypothesis. Our analysis of photography-related behaviors as coping strategies creates a picture of photography in which the benefits are generally greater than the cognitive and social costs, which also explains why photography became such a sweeping worldwide success once the smartphone technology became available.

The goal of producing an image that supports memory only plays a subordinate role in our description of photography-related behaviors. In this respect, our framework differs from explanations that describe the production of memory pictures as a central function of photography (Milgram, 1976 ; Kahneman, 2011 ; Frohlich et al., 2013 ; Henkel, 2014 ). These explanations are consistent with the fact that many people stated the retention of memories, when asked about the purpose of photographing (Chalfen, 1987 ; Kindberg et al., 2005 ; Broekhuijsen et al., 2017 ; Lee, 2018 ). Empirical findings, however, show that people lose many photos they have taken or never look at them again (Kirk et al., 2006 ; Whittaker et al., 2010 ). Furthermore, the experimental studies on the question of whether taking or seeing photos improves people's ability to remember past events produced a multitude of different and sometimes contradicting results (for review, see Foley, 2020 ). This was one of the incentives for us to attempt a new synthesis within an evolutionary theory frame.

Testable Predictions for Future Research

As shown in Table 2 , the mental utilization theory of photography allows generating a number of testable predictions. Ideally, these would be tackled by experimental and behavioral field studies in natural environments, in both everyday and lab situations. Rapidly developing mobile techniques (such as EEG headsets, eye-tracking devices, etc.) open up new possibilities for the investigation of the attention structures and specific cognitive mechanisms involved in taking and using photos.

A sample of testable predictions along the 4 levels of Tinbergen based on the mental utilization hypothesis of photography.

1. Making sense
   1a. Mechanisms: Studies of the neural substrates of taking photos will find that photographers make early basic decisions quickly and non-consciously. Researchers will also observe that photography-related behaviors and brain activity will utilize mechanisms that integrate external, emotionally positive information and internal, threatening information. The neural substrates of cognitive control and the regulation of emotions play a crucial role in this
   1b. Ontogeny: Children will preferentially be interested in photos of environments, behaviors, and events that they will soon face and that have both positive and threatening traits, and will prefer social contexts with humans and animals. Parents will take photos of their children especially in contexts where there are both positive and negative predictions. Older people will prefer photos in which their decisions and lives appear meaningful and successful
   1c. Evolutionary history: Despite increasing knowledge of fake photos, people will tend to believe that pictured events really did take place, as long as the events make sense in relation to their desires and experiences
   1d. Functions: People will prefer to take and use personal photos in situations in which they perceive emotionally positive events that they also associate with stress or threat. That way, taking photos will help people cope with social stress. If people want to convince others of the special importance of an object or event they will use photos more often than video clips
2. Appropriating an image
   2a. Mechanisms: Neuroscientific studies will find that photographing involves activities of brain structures that form the neural basis of appropriation and possession
   2b. Ontogeny: People will prefer photography-related behaviors when they are non-consciously processing the appropriation of a resource. In connection with identity, the importance of owning personal photos increases with age
   2c. Evolution: Collecting personal photos of events that the owners associate with beauty and/or success will enhance the owners' well-being
   2d. Functions: People will prefer ownership of self-made personal photos to photos taken by others of the same object or event, even if their own photos are of inferior quality
3. Establishing and supporting social connectedness
   3a. Mechanisms: Neurobiological studies will find that taking and using personal photos involves nodes and activities in the social behavior network in the brain—not only “wanting” and “liking” responses, but also mechanisms that are related to the processing of representations of being connected, alone, isolated, or abandoned
   3b. Ontogeny: Children will prefer to take and use photos of events that are relevant to their natural and social environment, especially family. Young people and adults will prefer to take, possess and use photos that show animals and people with whom they are, or want to be, emotionally connected. Older people will surround themselves in their home with photos of people who are or were important to them
   3c. Evolution: Viewing photos of close relatives and friends will have a positive effect on the well-being of the viewers at times when the pictured people are absent. This will entail a supportive effect on societal and biological fitness. Photos that evoke associations of pictured individuals or groups with social attachment, supportive relationships, and cooperation in viewers, will support the success of the pictured individuals or groups in societies. Variations in photography-related behaviors will change the environments in which they are performed, for example, as the increasing presence of cameras in public spaces influences people's behavior
   3d. Functions: Seeing photos depicting people with whom the viewers are connected by a close emotional relationship will strengthen the sense of social connectedness and provide a sense of security and calmness. Photos of oneself will be more efficient than verbal descriptions or video clips when the goal is influencing or controlling the characteristics that other people associate with oneself
4. Courtship and mating
   4a. Mechanisms: In the brains of people who are looking for sexual or romantic partners, seeing photos of potential partners will elicit intense motivational reactions, which are related to partner attraction and sexual arousal. There will be quantitative gender differences in this
   4b. Ontogeny: Sexually mature individuals of all ages will want to appear attractive in photos
   4c. Evolution: A stock of personal photos associated with beauty and success that a person owns will be recognized by potential partners as a valuable resource and directly or indirectly support the reproductive success of the owner
   4d. Functions: In a mating competition in social environments, in which information is exchanged without direct personal encounters, people who use photos to represent themselves as mates will be more successful than people who use words or video clips

We position viewing, sharing, and using personal photos within the coherent theory of the evolution of life and human nature. On the basis of the four levels of Tinbergen ( 1963 ), we developed a theoretical framework that describes the characteristics of photos and photography-related behaviors, including potential adaptative values related to the evolutionary functional domains of coping, well-being, social connectedness, courtship, and mating. We hypothesized that people take or use photos in contexts in which a pleasant event is coupled with uncertainty or with the processing of threatening information, and that people generally use photography as a coping strategy. Based on our analysis, we propose the Mental Utilization Hypothesis that explains the success of photography by its match with core human mental mechanisms, which characterize human nature.

The proposed hypothesis provides a novel conceptual framework, potentially useful in devising future experimental studies of photography. Despite the global ubiquity of photos, there is still almost no research into the cognitive mechanisms underlying photo taking. Investigations into the courtship or mating functions of photography are still limited to the explicit use of photos in online dating, but these functions are more fundamental and embracing. Studies regarding evolutionary functions of photography are particularly desirable. Important findings could be gained through comparisons between cultures, subcultures and sociological strata, gender and age classes. Important questions in such comparisons could be whether social prestige and social, occupational, or reproductive success can be linked with photography. Is photography an addition to existing social and sexual behavior or is it part of a socio-sexual change which compensates for or replaces previous behaviors or customs? Does it have “emergent properties” not found in its constituent elements? Last but not least, our description of taking and using photos as a coping strategy provides a comprehensive theoretical basis for new experimental research into the application of photography in psychotherapeutic contexts. With photography, people developed a new means of representing experiences and ideas through pictures with special characteristics, the understanding of which requires a minimum of effort and cultural knowledge. We are creatures in an increasingly complex social world for whom and in which these pictures open up powerful possibilities for action, but also for feeling at home and safe.

Data Availability Statement

Author contributions.

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the reviewers for their valuable comments and suggestions. They also thank Jenna Hicken for personal assistance in translating the manuscript.

Funding. This project was supported by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF): W1262-B29.

  • Alcaro A., Huber R., Panksepp J. (2007). Behavioral functions of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system: an affective neuroethological perspective . Brain Res. Rev . 56 , 283–321. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.014 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Allen M. L., Bloom P., Hodgson E. (2010). Do young children know what makes a picture useful to other people? J. Cogn. Cult . 10 , 27–37. 10.1163/156853710X497158 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Almeida I., Soares S. C., Castelo-Branco M. (2015). The distinct role of the amygdala, superior colliculus and pulvinar in processing of central and peripheral snakes . PLoS ONE 10 :e0129949. 10.1371/journal.pone.0129949 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anderson S. W., Damasio H., Damasio A. R. (2005). A neural basis for collecting behaviour in humans . Brain 128 , 201–212. 10.1093/brain/awh329 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Andolina D., Maran D., Valazania A., Conversi D., Puglisi-Allegra S. (2013). Prefrontal/amygdalar system determines stress coping behavior through 5-HT/GABA connection . Neuropsychopharmacology 38 , 2057–2067. 10.1038/npp.2013.107 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Antweiler C. (2016). Our Common Denominator: Human Universals Revisited . New York, NY: Berghahn Books. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Appleton J. (1975). The Experience of Landscape . Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Arbib M. A., Liebal K., Pika S. (2008). Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language . Curr. Anthropol . 49 , 1053–1076. 10.1086/593015 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Arendt H. (1958). The Human Condition . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Arndt J., Greenberg J., Solomon S., Pyszczynski T., Simon L. (1997). Suppression, accessibility of death-related thoughts, and cultural worldview defense: exploring the psychodynamics of terror management . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol . 73 , 5–18. 10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aust U., Huber L. (2010). Representational insight in pigeons: comparing subjects with and without real-life experience . Anim. Cogn . 13 , 207–218. 10.1007/s10071-009-0258-4 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baddeley A. D., Hitch G. (1974). Working memory, in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Advances in Research and Theory , Vol. 8 , ed Bower G. H. (Amsterdam: Academic Press; ), 47–89). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bakhshi S., Shamma D. A., Kennedy L., Gilbert E. (2015). Why we filter our photos and how it impacts engagement, in Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Weblogs and Social Media, ICWSM 2015 (Palo Alto, CA: The AAAI Press; ), 12–21. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control . New York, NY: W. H. Freeman. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barasch A., Diehl K., Silverman J., Zauberman G. (2017). Photographic memory: the effects of volitional photo taking on memory for visual and auditory aspects of an experience . Psychol. Sci . 28 , 1056–1066. 10.1177/0956797617694868 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barasch A., Zauberman G., Diehl K. (2014). When happiness shared is happiness halved: How taking photos to share with others affects experiences and memories . Adv. Consumer Res . 42, 101–105. Available online at: https://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/v42/acr_v42_16917.pdf
  • Bartels A., Zeki S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love . Neuroimage 21 , 1155–1166. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.003 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bartsch A., Mares M.-L. (2014). Making sense of violence: perceived meaningfulness as a predictor of audience Interest in violent media content . J. Commun . 64 , 956–976. 10.1111/jcom.12112 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bateson M., Nettle D., Roberts G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting . Biol. Lett . 2 , 412–414. 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0509 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bateson P., Laland K. N. (2013). Tinbergen's four questions: an appreciation and an update . Trends Ecol. Evol . 28 , 712–718. 10.1016/j.tree.2013.09.013 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baxter M. G., Murray E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward . Nat. Rev. Neurosci . 3 , 563–573. 10.1038/nrn875 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berger C. R., Calabrese R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond: toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication . Hum. Commun. Res . 1 , 99–112. 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1975.tb00258.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berridge K. C., Robinson T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res. Rev . 28 , 309–369. 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berto R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity . J. Environ. Psychol . 25 , 249–259. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.07.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bird-David N. (1999). “Animism” revisited: personhood, environment, and relational epistemology . Curr. Anthropol . 40 , S67–S91. 10.1086/200061 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blitch J. G. A. (2017). Naturalistic neurophysiological assessment of photographer cognitive state in the vicinity of Mount Everest, in Proceedings of the AHFE 2016 International Conference on Human Factors in Sports and Outdoor Recreation , eds Salmon P., Macquet A.-C. (Cham: Springer; ), 17–24. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bourdieu P. (1965). Culte de l'unité et différences cultivées [The cult of unity and cultivated differences], in Un art moyen: essai sur les usages sociaux de la photographie [A Middle-Brow Art] , eds Bourdieu P., Boltansky L., Castel R., Chamboredon J.-C. (Paris: Les Éditions de Minuit; ), 31–106. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bovet D., Vauclair J. (2000). Picture recognition in animals and humans . Behav. Brain Res . 109 , 143–165. 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00146-7 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bowlby J. (1974). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment . London: The Hogarth Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bradley M. M., Lang P. J. (2007). The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention, in Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment , eds Coan J. A., Allen J. J. B. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 29–46. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Broekhuijsen M., van den Hoven E., Markopoulos P. (2017). From photowork to photouse: exploring personal digital photo activities . Behav. Inform. Technol . 36 , 754–767. 10.1080/0144929X.2017.1288266 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown D. (1991). Human Universals . New York, NY: McGraw Hill. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Burgess M., Enzle M. E., Morry M. (2000). The social psychological power of photography: can the image-freezing machine make something of nothing? Eur. J. Soc. Psychol . 30 , 613–630. 10.1002/1099-0992(200009/10)30:5andlt;613::AID-EJSP11andgt;3.0.CO;2-S [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Burrow A. L., Rainone N. (2017). How many likes did I get?: Purpose moderates links between positive social media feedback and self-esteem . J. Exp. Soc. Psychol . 69 , 232–236. 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.09.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bush G., Luu P., Posner M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex . Trends Cogn. Sci . 4 , 215–222. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Buss D. M., Duntley J. D. (2011). The evolution of intimate partner violence . Aggress. Violent Behav . 16 , 411–419. 10.1016/j.avb.2011.04.015 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Buss D. M., Schmitt D. P. (1993). Sexual Strategies Theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating . Psychol. Rev . 100 , 204–232. 10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Butler A. B., Hodos W. (2005). Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation . New York, NY: Wiley-Liss. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Butler D. L., Mattingley J. B., Cunnington R., Suddendorf T. (2012). Mirror, mirror on the wall, how does my brain recognize my image at all? PLoS ONE 7 :e3145. 10.1371/journal.pone.0031452 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell D. (2014). The Integrity of the Image: Current Practices and Accepted Standards Relating to the Manipulation of Still Images in Photojournalism and Documentary Photography. World Press Photo Academy . Available online at: https://www.worldpressphoto.org/sites/default/files/upload/Integrity%20of%20the%20Image_2014%20Campbell%20report.pdf
  • Canon S. A. (2018). InfoTrends' Road Map 2018: Digital Photography Trends . Available online at: http://www.bizcommunity.com/Article/196/17/176366.html
  • Carrington D. (2020). How Many Photos Will be Taken in 2020 ? Mylio Development, LCC . Available online at: https://focus.mylio.com/tech-today/how-many-photos-will-be-taken-in-2020 (accessed June 25, 2020).
  • Carter C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love . Psychoneuroendocrinology 23 , 779–818. 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-9 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chalfen R. (1987). Snapshot Versions of Life . Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green State University Popular Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cherney I. D., Seiwert C. S., Dickey T. M., Flichtbeil J. D. (2006). Children's drawings: a mirror to their minds . Educ. Psychol . 26 , 127–142. 10.1080/01443410500344167 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Christakis N. A. (2019). Blueprint: The Evolutionary Origins of a Good Society . New York, NY: Little, Brown Spark. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen J. D., Braver T. S., Brown J. W. (2002). Computational perspectives on dopamine function in prefrontal cortex . Curr. Opin. Neurobiol . 12 , 223–229. 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00314-8 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conard N. J. (2009). A female figurine from the basal Aurignacian of Hohle Fels Cave in southwestern Germany . Nature 459 , 248–252. 10.1038/nature07995 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cox S. (2005). Intention and meaning in young children's drawing . J. Art Design Educ . 24 , 115–125. 10.1111/j.1476-8070.2005.00432.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crandall D., Backstrom L., Huttenlocher D., Kleinberg J. (2009). Mapping the world's photos, in Proceedings of the 18th International Conference on World Wide Web (WWW'09) (New York, NY: ACM; ), 761–770. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Csikszentmihalyi M., Rochberg-Halton E. (1981). The Meaning of Things: Domestic Symbols and the Self . New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Custers R., Aarts H. (2010). The unconscious will: how the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness . Science 329 , 47–50. 10.1126/science.1188595 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Damasio A. R. (2010). Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain . London: William Heinemann. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Darwin C. (1859). On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection or the Preservation of Favored Races in the Struggle for Live . London: John Murray. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Darwin C. (1879/2004). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex , 2nd Edn. London: Penguin. [ Google Scholar ]
  • De Kloet E. R., Joëls M., Holsboer F. (2005). Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease . Nat. Rev. Neurosci . 6 , 463–475. 10.1038/nrn1683 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • De Looper C. (2016). Vast Majority of Consumers Consider Their Photography Skills to Be Excellent. Digital Trends . Available online at: https://www.digitaltrends.com/photography/canon-photography-trends-survey/
  • De Pisapia N., Bacci F., Parrott D., Melcher D. (2016). Brain networks for visual creativity: a functional connectivity study of planning a visual artwork . Sci. Rep . 6 :39185. 10.1038/srep39185 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • De Waal F. B., Brosnan S. F. (2006). Simple and complex reciprocity in primates, in Cooperation in Primates and Humans: Mechanisms and Evolution , eds Kappeler P. M., van Schaik C. P. (Berlin: Springer; ), 85–105. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deacon T. (2006). The aesthetic faculty, in The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity , ed Turner M. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 21–53. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deacon T. W. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain . New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Co. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dehaene S., Naccache L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework . Cognition 79 , 1–37. 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DeLoache J. S., Pierroutsakos S. L., Uttal D. H., Rosengren K. S., Gottlieb A. (1998). Grasping the nature of pictures . Psychol. Sci . 9 , 205–210. 10.1111/1467-9280.00039 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deregowski J. B., Muldrow E. S., Muldrow W. F. (1972). Pictorial recognition in a remote Ethiopian population . Perception 1 , 417–425. 10.1068/p010417 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Devue C., Collette F., Balteau E., Degueldre C., Luxen A., Maquet P., et al.. (2007). Here I am: the cortical correlates of visual self-recognition . Brain Res . 1143 , 169–182. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.055 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diefenbach S., Christoforakos L. (2017). The selfie paradox: nobody seems to like them yet everyone has reasons to take them. An exploration of psychological functions of selfies in self-presentation . Front. Psychol . 8 :7. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00007 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diehl K., Zauberman G., Barasch A. (2016). How taking photos increases enjoyment of experiences . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol . 111 , 119–140. 10.1037/pspa0000055 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dommett E., Coizet V., Blaha C. D., Martindale J., Lefebvre V., Walton N., et al.. (2005). How visual stimuli activate dopaminergic neurons at short latency . Science 307 , 1476–1479. 10.1126/science.1107026 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Donald M. (2006). Art and cognitive evolution, in The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity , ed Turner M. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 3–20. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Donaldson Z. R., Young L. J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality . Science 322 , 900–904. 10.1126/science.1158668 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dutton D. (2009). The Art Instinct: Beauty, Pleasure, and Human Evolution . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisenberger N. I., Master S. L., Inagaki T. K., Taylor S. E., Shirinyan D., Lieberman M. D., et al.. (2011). Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related neural region and reduce pain experience . Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci . 108 , 11721–11726. 10.1073/pnas.1108239108 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ellamil M., Dobson C., Beeman M., Christoff K. (2012). Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process . Neuroimage 59 , 1783–1794. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.008 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Evans J. S. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition . Annu. Rev. Psychol . 59 , 255–278. 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Felleman D. J., Van Essen D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex . Cerebral Cortex 1 , 1–47. 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fisher H. E., Aron A., Mashek D., Li H., Brown L. L. (2002). Defining the brain systems of lust, romantic attraction, and attachment . Arch. Sex. Behav . 31 , 413–419. 10.1023/A:1019888024255 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Foley M. A. (2020). Effects of photographic reviews on recollections of the personal past: a new perspective on benefits and costs . Rev. Gen. Psychol. 24 , 369–381. 10.1177/1089268020958686 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Frazer J. G. (1911). The Golden Bough: A Study in Magic and Religion, Vol. 3. Taboo and the Perils of the Soul , 3rd Edn. London: MacMillan. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Frohlich D. M., Wall S., Kiddle G. (2013). Rediscovery of forgotten images in domestic photo collections . Pers. Ubiquitous Comput . 17 , 729–740. 10.1007/s00779-012-0612-4 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gale A., Lewis M. B. (2020). When the camera does lie: selfies are dishonest indicators of dominance . Psychol. Popular Media 9 , 447–455. 10.1037/ppm0000260 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ghazanfar A. A., Santos L. R. (2004). Primate brains in the wild: The sensory bases for social interactions . Nature Reviews Neuroscience . 5 , 603–616. 10.1038/nrn1473 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gillet S., Schmitz P., Mitas O. (2016). The snap-happy tourist: the effects of photographing behavior on tourists' happiness . J. Hosp. Touri. Res . 40 , 37–57. 10.1177/1096348013491606 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gobbini M. I., Leibenluft E., Santiago N., Haxby J. V. (2004). Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces . Neuroimage 22 , 1628–1635. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.049 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gollisch T., Meister M. (2010). Eye smarter than scientists believed: neural computations in circuits of the retina . Neuron 65 , 150–164. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.009 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goodson J. L. (2005). The vertebrate social behavior network: evolutionary themes and variations . Horm. Behav . 48 , 11–22. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.02.003 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gregory T. R. (2008). The evolution of complex organs . Evol. Educ. Outreach 1 , 358–389. 10.1007/s12052-008-0076-1 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • GSMA NTT DOCOMO (2014). Children's Use of Mobile Phones: An International Comparison 2013 . Available online at: https://www.gsma.com/publicpolicy/wp-content/uploads/2016/09/GSMA2013_Report_ChildrensUseOfMobilePhones.pdf
  • Gu X., Han S. (2007). Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of empathy for pain . Neuroimage 36 , 256–267. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.025 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guthrie D. R. (2005). The Nature of Paleolithic Art . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hafri A., Papafragou A., Trueswell J. C. (2013). Getting the gist of events: recognition of two-participant actions from brief displays . J. Exp. Psychol. Gen . 142 , 880–905. 10.1037/a0030045 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Harrison B. (2002). Photographic visions and narrative inquiry . Narr. Inquiry 12 , 87–111. 10.1075/ni.12.1.14har [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Harvey G. (2005). Animism: Respecting the Living World . London: C. Hurst and Co. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hazan C., Shaver P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol . 52 , 511–524. 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Henkel L. A. (2014). Point-and-shoot memories: the influence of taking photos on memory for a museum tour . Psychol. Sci . 25 , 396–402. 10.1177/0956797613504438 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hetherwick A. (1902). Some animistic beliefs among the Yaos of British Central Africa . J. Anthropol. Inst. Great Britain Ireland 32 , 89–95. 10.2307/2842905 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hobbs M., Owen S., Gerber L. (2017). Liquid love? Dating apps, sex, relationships and the digital transformation of intimacy . J. Sociol . 53 , 271–284. 10.1177/1440783316662718 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hocart A. M. (1922). The Cult of the dead in Eddystone of the Solomons (Part 1) . J. R. Anthropol. Inst . 52 , 71–112. 10.2307/2843772 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hostinar C. E., Sullivan R. M., Gunnar M. R. (2014). Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis: a review of animal models and human studies across development . Psychol. Bull . 140 , 256–282. 10.1037/a0032671 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hsu D. T., Sanford B. J., Meyers K. K., Love T. M., Hazlett K. E., Wang H., et al.. (2013). Response of the μ-opioid system to social rejection and acceptance . Mol. Psychiatry 18 , 1211–1217. 10.1038/mp.2013.96 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hsu M., Bhatt M., Adolphs R., Tranel D., Camerer C. F. (2005). Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making . Science 310 , 1680–1683. 10.1126/science.1115327 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hu Y., Manikonda L., Kambhampati S. (2014). What we Instagram: a first analysis of Instagram photo content and user types, in Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Weblogs and Social Media, ICWSM 2014 (Palo Alto, CA: The AAAI Press; ), 595–598. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jablonka E., Lamb M. J. (2014). Evolution in Four Dimensions: Genetic, Epigenetic, Behavioral, and Symbolic Variation in the History of Life (Revised ed.) . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jacobs R. H., Renken R., Cornelissen F. W. (2012). Neural correlates of visual aesthetics - Beauty as the coalescence of stimulus and internal state . PLoS ONE 7 :e31248. 10.1371/journal.pone.0031248 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jain M. J., Mavani K. J. (2017). A comprehensive study of worldwide selfie-related accidental mortality: a growing problem of the modern society . Int. J. Inj. Contr. Saf. Promot . 24 , 544–549. 10.1080/17457300.2016.1278240 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Joinson A. N. (2008). Looking at, looking up or keeping up with people? Motives and uses of Facebook, in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ‘08) (New York, NY: ACM; ), 1027–1036. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kahneman D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow . New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kano F., Tomonaga M. (2009). How chimpanzees look at pictures: a comparative eye-tracking study . Proc. R. Soc. Ser. B 276 , 1949–1955. 10.1098/rspb.2008.1811 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kaplan R., Kaplan S. (1989). The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kawabata H., Zeki S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty . J. Neurophysiol . 91 , 1699–1705. 10.1152/jn.00696.2003 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kemp S. (2021). Digital 2021 Global Overview Report [Web Log Message]. We Are Social, and Hootsuite . Retrieved from: https://datareportal.com/reports/digital-2021-global-overview-report
  • Kindberg T., Spasojevic M., Fleck R., Sellen A. (2005). The ubiquitous camera: an in-depth study of camera phone use . IEEE Pervasive Comput . 4 , 42–50. 10.1109/MPRV.2005.42 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kirk D., Sellen A., Rother C., Wood K. (2006). Understanding photowork, in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ‘06) (New York, NY: Association for Computing Machinery; ), 761–770. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kislinger L. (2021). Photographs beyond concepts: access to actions and sensations . Rev. Gen. Psychol . 25 , 44–59. 10.1177/1089268020969113 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kittredge G. L. (1929). Witchcraft in Old and New England . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knobloch L. K., Solomon D. H. (1999). Measuring the sources and content of relational uncertainty . Commun. Stud . 50 , 261–278. 10.1080/10510979909388499 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kotrschal K. (2019). Mensch: Woher wir kommen, wer wir sind, wohin wir gehen . Vienna: Brandstätter. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kowalski R. M., Giumetti G. W., Schroeder A. N., Lattanner M. R. (2014). Bullying in the digital age: a critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth . Psychol. Bull . 140 , 1073–1137. 10.1037/a0035618 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krämer N. C., Winter S. (2008). Impression management 2.0: the relationship of self-esteem, extraversion, self-efficacy, and self-presentation within social networking sites . J. Media Psychol . 20 , 106–116. 10.1027/1864-1105.20.3.106 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kringelbach M. L., Berridge K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness . Trends Cogn. Sci . 13 , 479–487. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.006 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee E. (2018). Evolving Photographer Types Portend the Future of Camera Ownership. InfoTrends, Inc . Retrieved from: https://blog.infotrends.com/tag/photography/ (accessed June 25, 2020).
  • Lee E., Lee J.-A., Moon J. H., Sung Y. (2015). Pictures speak louder than words: motivations for using instagram . Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network . 18 , 552–556. 10.1089/cyber.2015.0157 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee P., Stewart D. (2016). Photo Sharing: Trillions and Rising. Deloitte TMT Predictions 2016 . Available online at: https://www2.deloitte.com/content/dam/Deloitte/global/Documents/Technology-Media-Telecommunications/gx-tmt-prediction-online-photo-sharing.pdf
  • Leibenluft E., Gobbini M. I., Harrison T., Haxby J. V. (2004). Mothers' neural activation in response to pictures of their children and other children . Biol. Psychiatry 56 , 225–232. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.017 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lutter T., Meinecke C., Tropf T. (2017). Connected consumer technology, in Zukunft der Consumer Technology – 2017 [Future of Consumer Technology – 2017] , ed Bitkom E. V. (Bitkom), 12–49. Available online at: https://www.bitkom.org/noindex/Publikationen/2017/Studien/2017/CT-Studie/170901-CT-Studie-online.pdf
  • Magnusson L. O. (2018). Photographic agency and agency of photographs: three-year-olds and digital cameras . Aust. J. Early Child . 43 , 34–42. 10.23965/AJEC.43.3.04 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mäkelä A., Giller V., Tscheligi M., Selefin R. (2000). Joking, storytelling, artsharing, expressing affection: A field trial of how children and their social network communicate with digital images in leisure time, in CHI'00: Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (New York, NY: ACM; ), 548–555. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Malik A., Dhir A., Nieminen M. (2016). Uses and gratifications of digital photo sharing on Facebook . Telemat. Informat . 33 , 129–138. 10.1016/j.tele.2015.06.009 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Master S. L., Eisenberger N. I., Taylor S. E., Naliboff B. D., Shirinyan D., Lieberman M. D. (2009). A picture's worth: partner photographs reduce experimentally induced pain . Psychol. Sci . 20 , 1316–1318. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02444.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Milgram S. (1976). The image-freezing machine . Society 14 , 7–12. 10.1007/BF02694642 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miller E. K., Cohen J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function . Annu. Rev. Neurosci . 24 , 167–202. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miller G. (2001). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of the Human mind. New York, NY: Anchor Books. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miller R. J. (1973). Cross-cultural research in the perception of pictorial materials . Psychol. Bull . 80 , 135–150. 10.1037/h0034739 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mols I., Broekhuijsen M., van den Hoven E., Makropoulos P., Eggen B. (2015). Do we ruin the moment? Exploring the design of novel capturing technologies, in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Australian Special Interest Group for Computer Human Interaction, OzCHI' 15 (New York, NY: ACM; ), 653–661. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morris J. S., Öhman A., Dolan R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear . Proc. Natl. Acad. Sc . 96 , 1680–1685. 10.1073/pnas.96.4.1680 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morris T. (2021). Dating in 2021: Swiping Left on COVID-19 . Retrieved from: https://blog.globalwebindex.com/chart-of-the-week/online-dating/ (accessed March 13, 2021).
  • Murdock G. P. (1945). The common denominator of cultures, in The Science of Man in the World Crisis , ed Linton R. (Columbia University Press; ), 123–142. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nelson E. E., Panksepp J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev . 22 , 437–452. 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00052-3 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nesse R. M. (2013). Tinbergen's four questions, organized: a response to Bateson and Laland . Trends Ecol. Evol . 28 , 681–682. 10.1016/j.tree.2013.10.008 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Newton J. (2019). Why do People Take Pictures at Crash Scenes? BBC News. Retrieved from: https://www.bbc.com/news/uk-england-45839351 (accessed November 19, 2020).
  • Newtson D. (1973). Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol . 28 , 28–38. 10.1037/h0035584 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nightingale S. J., Wade K. A., Watson D. G. (2017). Can people identify original and manipulated photos of real-world scenes? Cogn. Res. Principles Implications 2 :30. 10.1186/s41235-017-0067-2 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Northcutt R. G. (2011). Evolving large and complex brains . Science 332 , 926–927. 10.1126/science.1206915 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nowak M. A. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation . Science 314 , 1560–1563. 10.1126/science.1133755 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nowak M. A., Sigmund K. (2005). Evolution of indirect reciprocity . Nature 437 , 1291–1298. 10.1038/nature,04131 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O'Connell L. A., Hofmann H. A. (2011). The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis . J. Comp. Neurol . 519 , 3599–3639. 10.1002/cne.22735 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oda R., Kato Y., Hiraishi K. (2015). The watching-eye effect on prosocial lying . Evol. Psychol . 13 , 1–5. 10.1177/1474704915594959 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Panksepp J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions . New York; NY: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perrett D. I., Oram M. W., Harries M. H., Bevan R., Hietamen J. K., Benson P. J., et al.. (1991). Viewer-centred and object-centred coding of heads in the macaque temporal cortex . Exp. Brain Res . 86 , 159–173. 10.1007/BF00231050 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Petrelli D., Whittaker S. (2010). Family memories in the home: contrasting physical and digital mementos . Pers. Ubiquitous Comput . 14 , 153–169. 10.1007/s00779-009-0279-7 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pettitt P. (2011). The Paleolithic Origins of Human Burial . Abingdon: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Piazza J., Bering J. M. (2009). Evolutionary cyber-psychology: applying an evolutionary framework to Internet behavior . Comput. Human Behav . 25 , 1258–1269. 10.1016/j.chb.2009.07.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pittman M., Reich B. (2016). Social media and loneliness: why an Instagram picture may be worth more than a thousand Twitter words . Comput. Human Behav . 62 , 155–167. 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.084 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Posner M. I., Nissen M. J., Klein R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance . Psychol. Rev . 83 , 157–171. 10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.157 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pourtois G., Schettino A., Vuilleumier P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: what is magic and what is not . Biol. Psychol . 92 , 492–512. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Preckel K., Scheele D., Eckstein M., Maier W., Hurelmann R. (2015). The influence of oxytocin on volitional and emotional ambivalence . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci . 10 , 987–993. 10.1093/scan/nsu147 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Preissler M. A., Bloom P. (2008). Two-year-olds use artist intention to understand drawings . Cognition 106 , 512–518. 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.02.002 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Regulation (EU) (2016). Regulation (EU) 2016/679 of the European Parliament and of the Council of 27 April 2016 on the Protection of Natural Persons With Regard to the Processing of Personal Data and on the Free Movement of Such Data, and Repealing Directive 95/46/EC (General Data Protection Regulation) . Available online at: http://data.europa.eu/eli/reg/2016/679/oj
  • Richerson P. J., Boyd R., Henrich J. (2010). Gene-culture coevolution in the age of genomics . Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci . 107 , 8985–8992. 10.1073/pnas.0914631107 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • RSPH YHM (2017). Status of Mind: Social Media and Young People's Mental Health and wellbeing. Royal Society for Public Health (RSPH) and the Young Health Movement (YHM) . Available online at: https://www.rsph.org.uk/uploads/assets/uploaded/d125b27c-0b62-41c5-a2c0155a8887cd01.pdf
  • Ryan M. J. (1990). Sexual selection, sensory systems and sensory exploitation . Oxford Surveys Evol. Biol . 7 , 157–195. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scheele D., Wille A., Kendrick K. M., Stoffel-Wagner B., Becker B., Güntürkün O., et al.. (2013). Oxytocin enhances brain reward system responses in men viewing the face of their female partner . Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci . 110 , 20308–20313. 10.1073/pnas.1314190110 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schiano D. J., Chen C. P., Issacs E. (2002). How teens take, view, share, and store photos [Interactive poster], in Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW 2002) . Available online at: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Diane_Schiano/publication/265533076_How_Teens_Take_View_Share_and_Store_Photos/links/54b7cfa10cf269d8cbf543a7/How-Teens-Take-View-Share-and-Store-Photos.pdf
  • Schiller D., Levy I., Niv Y., LeDoux J. E., Phelps E. A. (2008). From fear to safety and back: reversal of fear in the human brain . J. Neurosci . 28 , 11517–11525. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2265-08.2008 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schultz W. (2006). Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward . Annu. Rev. Psychol . 57 , 87–115. 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070229 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sedgewick J. R., Flath M. E., Elias L. J. (2017). Presenting your best self(ie): the influence of gender on vertical orientation of selfies on Tinder . Front. Psychol . 8 :604. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00604 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sellen A. J., Fogg A., Aitken M., Hodges S., Rother C., Wood K. (2007). Do life-logging technologies support memory for the past? An experimental study using sensecam, in CHI'07: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (ACM; ), 81–90. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seyfarth R. M., Cheney D. L., Bergman T. J. (2005). Primate social cognition and the origins of language . Trends Cogn. Sci. 9 , 264–266. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.001 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sharples M., Davison L., Thomas G. V., Rudman P. D. (2003). Children as photographers: an analysis of children's photographic behaviour and intentions at three age levels . Vis. Commun . 2 , 303–330. 10.1177/14703572030023004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shenhav A., Botvinick M. M., Cohen J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function . Neuron 79 , 217–240. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith A. (2011). Americans and Their Cell Phones. Pew Research Center . Available online at: http://www.pewinternet.org/2016/02/11/15-percent-of-american-adults-have-used-online-dating-sites-or-mobile-dating-apps/#fnref-15504-1
  • Smith A. (2016). 15% of American Adults Have Used Online Dating Sites or Mobile Dating Apps . Retrieved from: http://www.pewinternet.org/2016/02/11/15-percent-of-american-adults-have-used-online-dating-sites-or-mobile-dating-apps/#fnref-15504–1 (accessed March 20, 2021).
  • Smith P. K., Mahdavi J., Carvalho M., Fisher S., Russell S., Tippett N. (2008). Cyberbullying: its nature and impact in secondary school pupils . J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49 , 376–385. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01846.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sontag S. (1978). On Photography . New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Soon C. S., Brass M., Heinze H.-J., Haynes J.-D. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain . Nat. Neurosci . 11 , 543–545. 10.1038/nn.2112 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stake J. E. (2004). The property ‘instinct’ . Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 359 , 1763–1774. 10.1098/rstb.2004.1551 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Statista (2019). Number of Smartphone Users Worldwide From 2016 to 2021 . Available online at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/330695/number-of-smartphone-users-worldwide/
  • St. Jacques P., Conway M. A., Lowder M. W., Cabeza R. (2011). Watching my mind unfold versus yours: an fMRI study using a novel camera technology to examine neural differences in self-projection of self versus other perspectives . J. Cogn. Neurosci . 23 , 1275–1284. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21518 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stöckl H., Devries K., Rotstein A., Abrahams N., Campbell J., Watts C., et al.. (2013). The global prevalence of intimate partner homicide: a systematic review . The Lancet . 382 , 859–865. 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61030-2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Striedter G. F. (2020). A history of ideas in evolutionary neuroscience, in Evolutionary Neuroscience , 2nd Edn, ed J. Kaas H. (Academic Press; ), 3–16. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Talbot H. F. (1844/2011). The Pencil of Nature [Reproduction] . Bradford: KWS Publishers, in association with National Media Museum. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thagard P., Stewart T. C. (2011). The AHA! experience: Creativity through emergent binding in neural networks . Cogn. Sci . 35 , 1–33. 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01142.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • The Family of Man (1955). Museum of Modern Art New York . [ Google Scholar ]
  • The Post-Standard (1911). Speakers Give Sound Advice . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thomas G. V., Silk A. M. (1990). An Introduction to the Psychology of Children's Drawings . New York, NY: Harvester Wheatsheaf. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tinbergen N. (1963). On aims and methods of Ethology . Zeitschr. Tierpsychol . 20 , 410–433. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tomasello M. (2014). A Natural History of Human Thinking . Harvard University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tonegawa S., Liu X., Ramirez S., Redondo R. (2015). Memory engram cells have come of age . Neuron 87 , 918–931. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.002 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tulving E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human?, in The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self-Reflective Consciousness , eds Terrace H. S., Metcalfe J. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 3–56. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Turk D. J., van Bussel K., Waiter G. D., Macrae C. N. (2011). Mine and me: exploring the neural basis of object ownership . J. Cogn. Neurosci . 23 , 3657–3668. 10.1162/jocn_a_00042 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ulrich R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment, in Behavior and the Natural Environment , eds Altman I., Wohlwill J. F. (New York, NY: Plenum Press; ), 85–125. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ulrich-Lai Y., Herman J. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses . Nat. Rev. Neurosci . 10 , 397–409. 10.1038/nrn2647 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valtchanov D., Ellard C. G. (2015). Cognitive and affective responses to natural scenes: effects of low level visual properties on preference, cognitive load and eye-movements . J. Environ. Psychol . 43 , 184–195. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.07.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Le Q., Isbell L. A., Matsumoto J., Nguyen M., Hori E., Maior R. S., et al.. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes . Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci . 110 , 19000–19005. 10.1073/pnas.1312648110 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Vugt M., Tybur J. M. (2016). The evolutionary foundations of status hierarchy, in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology , 2nd Edn, Vol. 2 : Integrations, ed Buss D. M. (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons; ), 788–809. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vollmuth H. (2017). Unfälle: Sind Wir Alle Gaffer? [Accidents: Are We All Gawkers?] . Süddeutsche Zeitung. Retrieved from: https://www.sueddeutsche.de/panorama/unfaelle-sind-wir-alle-gaffer-1.3682243 (accessed November 19, 2020).
  • Wade K. A., Garry M., Read J. D., Lindsay D. S. (2002). A picture is worth a thousand lies: using false photographs to create false childhood memories . Psychon. Bull. Rev . 9 , 597–603. 10.3758/BF03196318 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wehrum S., Klucken T., Kagerer S., Walter B., Hermann A., Vaitl D., et al.. (2013). Gender commonalities and differences in the neural processing of visual sexual stimuli . J. Sex. Med . 10 , 1328–1342. 10.1111/jsm.12096 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • White R., Bourrillon R., Mensan R., Clark A., Chiotti L., Higham T., et al.. (2018). Newly discovered Aurignacian engraved blocks from Abri Cellier: history, context and dating . Q. Int . 498 , 99–125. 10.1016/j.quaint.2017.02.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Whiten A. (2011). The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes . Philos. Transac. R. SocB 366 , 997–1007. 10.1098/rstb.2010.0334 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Whittaker S., Bergman O., Clough P. (2010). Easy on that trigger dad: a study of long term familiy photo retrieval . Pers. Ubiquitous Comput . 14 , 31–43. 10.1007/s00779-009-0218-7 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Williams S. M., Goldman-Rakic P. S. (1993). Characterization of the dopaminergic innervation of the primate frontal cortex using a dopamine-specific antibody . Cerebral Cortex 3 , 199–222. 10.1093/cercor/3.3.199 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilson E. O. (1984). Biophilia . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilson E. O. (2012). The Social Conquest of Earth . New York, NY: Liveright. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Woltereck R. (1909). Weitere experimentelle Untersuchungen über Artveränderung, speziell über das Wesen quantitativer Artenunterschiede bei Daphniden [Further experimental studies on species change, especially on the nature of quantitative species differences in daphnia] . Verhandlungen Deutsch. Zoologischen Gesellschaft . 19 , 110–172. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wright S. (2010). Understanding Creativity in Early Childhood: Meaning-Making and Children's Drawing . Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zacks J. M., Tversky B., Iyer G. (2001). Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events . J. Exp. Psychol. Gen . 130 , 29–58. 10.1037/0096-3445.130.1.29 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zimmermann M. (1989). The nervous system in the context of information theory, in Human Physiology , eds Schmidt R. F., Thews G. (Berlin: Springer; ), 166–173. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zuboff S. (2015). Big other: surveillance capitalism and the prospects of an information civilization . J. Inform. Technol . 30 , 75–89. 10.1057/jit.2015.5 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Photography, Literature, and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Approach for the Representation of the Truth and the Reality

Profile image of Raihan Shafiq

The verisimilitude of truth of photography has placed it as an unequalled medium representing and expressing the reality. Yet, its widespread use as an art, the selection and exclusion of certain subjects that may be related to the ideology held by the photographer problematize such assertion. This essay will critically engage, through a few examples, with this difficulty in twentieth century's context. The problem of photographic representation, say of poverty, or war or labour exploitation will be addressed as in many cases they end up becoming fashionable clichés. While so doing, the essay will show how the use of literary narrative and photographic representation together may address the problem, and that may again confront difficulties in an age ubiquity of images.

Related Papers

CoSMo - Comparative Studies in Modernism

research essay on photography

Luca Bandirali

SATURDAY 18 November 8B - Photography and Fiction (II) Rettorato, Biblioteca Arturo Graf - 17, Via Po Chair: Teresa Prudente (Università di Torino) 3) Luca Bandirali (Università del Salento), Shooting in the Storyworld - In this talk I will try to place the figure of photographer in film’s narrative structure. Drama’s hero or heroine, according to Aristotle, is a twofold construction: he or she is both an agent and a character. As an agent, she carries out acts to modify her own storyworld and to achieve her goal; as a character, her actions align with her moral values. The photographer as an agent is self-defined, as she takes pictures, usually for a living. The questions are: what values are usually linked to these acts? Who is the photographer as a character?

irisjournal.org

Martyna Markowska

The Photocaptionist

silvia albertazzi

The Photocaptionist is delighted to present for the first time in English an excerpt of Silvia Albertazzi's book Literature and Photography, published by Carocci editore in 2017. We selected the first section of chapter 4, entitled 'From Text to Photo, from Photo to Text'. Enjoy the photo-literary feast!

Romi Mikulinsky

Taisuke Edamura

Photographic representation is commonly viewed as partial and fragmented. In today's extreme overflow of images photography increasingly emerges as formally deceptive and ideologically manipulative in the ways in which it serves the construction, circulation, and validation of chosen discourses (e.g. colonialism, social violence, and scientific truth). Further challenges to the notion of photographic representation lie in recent history: after World War II, the ethical implications of representation became a primary concern, while the very possibility of representation of traumatic events was questioned by theorists and artists alike. More recently, writings by Georges Didi-Huberman, Jacques Rancière, and Jean-Luc Nancy have sought to question the impossibility (or taboo) of representation, opening a discussion on how the links between photography, trauma and historical memory can be re-examined. Photography perhaps best functions as a discursive site in which either the unrepresentable emerges as self-evident or its fictitious nature simultaneously manifests, hides, and collapses. In the light of this hypothesis, the conference intends to address the following questions: What does the notion of the unrepresentable do to assumptions of photographic truth? What might the unrepresentable look like? Is there a representational impossibility specific to photography? When photography is requested to perform "adequate representation," how and in what context does the request become justifiable? How do today's image-making technologies affect the understanding of the unrepresentable?

Alice Morin , Anaïs Mauuarin , Véra Léon

Anke Coumans

In the world around us we see what the images have taught us to see. If people go on holiday, tempted by photographs in advertisements, they look for the images the travelling agent promised. That is what they take a picture of and this picture they post on Facebook. According to Flusser - and he thought of this way before the internet era – photographs are not representations, but programmes which ask reality to move towards the image. This becomes most painfully clear in plastic surgery, where women are trying to look like photo-shopped depictions of women. In war journalism, quite often photographers are facing a reenacted reality in front of their cameras, either or not created by themselves. They and the press officers in war zones know only too well what kind of images newspaper readers in the west want to see. We are caught in a web of images, on which reality has slowly lost its grip. This is the basic problem professional photography and each self-respecting photographer is facing.

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED PAPERS

Daniel A Novak

Michael S Roth

Eleni Papargyriou

Media Education

Peter Lančarič , Petra Cepkova

Angela Cristina Salgueiro Marques

Image [&] Narrative

Giorgia Alù

Ayelet Zohar

Suneel Mehmi

Michel Hardy-Vallée

Configurations

FICTION AND THE REACTION TO PHOTOGRAPHY: LITERALISM, THE LAW AND THE CONDITIONS AND THE CONTROL OF READING FROM THE INVENTION OF PHOTOGRAPHY TO THE 1920S

PhotoResearcher

Celio Barreto

Christopher Pinney

Edith Lázar

Larne Abse Gogarty

Thomas Royberghs

Ingkharat Chanaphai

Escritura e Imagen

Ana Laguna Martínez

Sofia Caldeira

Joanna Madloch

christina ljungberg

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Photography as an Art-Based Research Method

  • First Online: 14 December 2017

Cite this chapter

research essay on photography

  • Sten Langmann 3 &
  • David Pick 4  
  • An erratum to this publication is available online at https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-7279-6_7

The aim of this chapter is to examine the potential of photography as a means of art and to introduce the different genres of photo-narratives, documentary photography and portraiture that researchers can employ to collect data. While the chapter helps to inform social researchers about the choices they make when doing photographic research, it is also a reminder that when researchers use photographs to collect data, they simultaneously engage in acts of producing art.

The original version of this chapter was revised: Placement of the figures have been changed. The erratum to this chapter is available at https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-7279-6_7

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Arima, M. (2016). ‘Career’or ‘family’? Increasing work–life balance satisfaction among Japanese physicians with and without children. Diversity and Equality in Health Care, 13 (4), 290–291.

Article   Google Scholar  

Aubert, D. (2009). The doorstep portrait: Intrusion and performance in mainstream American documentary photography 1. Visual Studies, 24 (1), 3–18.

Babbie, E. R. (2000). The practice of social research . Belmont, CA: Cengage Learning.

Google Scholar  

Bach, H. (2007). Composing a visual narrative inquiry. Handbook of narrative inquiry: Mapping a methodology (pp. 280–307). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Becker, H. S. (1974). Photography and sociology. Studies in the Anthropology of Visual Communication, 1 (1), 3–26.

Becker, H. S. (1986). Doing things together: Selected papers . Northwestern University Press: Evanston.

Becker, H. S. (1995). Visual sociology, documentary photography, and photojournalism: It’s (almost) all a matter of context. Visual Studies, 10 (1–2), 5–14.

Bright, S. (2015). Art photography now: Revised and expanded edition . London: Thames & Hudson.

Chase, S. (2005). Narrative inquiry: Multiple lenses, approaches, voices. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 651–679). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Cheroux, C. (2008). Henri Cartier-Bresson: Ein Schnappschuss und sein Meister (trans: Steinitz, C.). Mosel: Schirmer.

Chilton, G., & Leavy, P. (2014). Arts-based research practice: Merging social research and the creative arts. The Oxford handbook of qualitative research (pp. 403–422) New York, NY: Oxford University.

Cope, V., Jones, B., & Hendricks, J. (2015). Portraiture: A methodology through which success and positivity can be explored and reflected. Nurse Researcher, 22 (3), 6–12.

Costello, D., & Iversen, M. (2012). Introduction: Photography between art history and philosophy. Critical Inquiry, 38 (4), 679–693.

Davis, J. H. (2003). Balancing the whole: Portraiture as methodology. In P. M. Camic, H. R. Rhodes, & L. Yardley (Eds.), Qualitative research in psychology: Expanding perspectives in methodology and design (pp. 199–2017). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Free, M. (2009). Can a ‘portrait’paint a thousand scholarly words . PhD Candidate, School of Education and Professional Studies, Brisbane and Logan, Griffith University.

Gaudelli, C. (2017). Women Boxers in Argentina. Retrieved from https://www.lensculture.com/articles/claudia-gaudelli-women-boxers-in-argentina .

Goldberg, J. (1985). Rich and poor: Photographs . New York: Random House Inc.

Gregos, K. (2006). Rochelle Costi. In T. J. Demos (Ed.), New perspectives in photography (pp. 068–069). London: Phaidon Press Limited.

Grierson, J. (1926). Flaherty’s Poetic Moana. The documentary tradition (p. 25). New York: Norton and Co.

Ishiyama, F. I., & Kitayama, A. (1994). Overwork and career-centered self-validation among the Japanese: Psychosocial issues and counselling implications. International Journal for the Advancement of Counselling, 17 (3), 167–182.

Kawanishi, Y. (2008). On Karo-Jisatsu (suicide by overwork): Why do Japanese workers work themselves to death? International Journal of Mental Health, 37 (1), 61–74.

Ketelle, D. (2010). The ground they walk on: Photography and narrative inquiry. The Qualitative Report, 15 (3), 547.

Krieger, S. (1991). Social science and the self: Personal essays on an art form . New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.

Marin, R., & Roldan, J. (2010). Photo essays and photographs in visual arts-based educational research. International Journal of Education through Art, 6 (1), 7–23.

Murphy, F. (2015). Filipino boy receives scholarship after photograph of him studying on the street goes viral. (Jiuly 10, 2015). The Telegraph .

North, S., & Morioka, R. (2016). Hope found in lives lost: Karoshi and the pursuit of worker rights in Japan. Contemporary Japan, 28 (1), 59–80.

Pelton, M. R., & van Manen, F. T. (1996). Benefits and pitfalls of long-term research: A case study of black bears in Great Smoky Mountains National Park. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 443–450.

Richardson, B. (1994). I etcetera: On the poetics and ideology of multipersoned narratives. Style, 28 , 312–328.

Richardson, L., & Pierre, E. (2000). Writing: A method of inquiry. In N. Denzin & Y. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 923–948). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Roberts, B. (2011). Photographic portraits: Narrative and memory. In Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 2011 (Vol. 12).

Sander, A. (1986). Citizens of the twentieth century . Cambridge: MIT Press.

Smith, N., & Rock, J. (2014). Documentary as a statement: Defining old genre in a new age. Journal of Media Practice, 15 (1), 58–62.

Snyder, J. (1980). Picturing vision. Critical Inquiry, 6 (3), 499–526.

Suchar, C. S. (1997). Grounding visual sociology research in shooting scripts. Qualitative Sociology, 20 (1), 33–55.

Szto, P., Furman, R., & Langer, C. (2005). Poetry and photography: An exploration into expressive/creative qualitative research. Qualitative Social Work, 4 (2), 135–156.

Wagner, J. (2004). Constructing credible images documentary studies, social research, and visual studies. American Behavioral Scientist, 47 (12), 1477–1506.

Willis, P. (2002). Poetry and poetics in phenomenological research. Indo-Pacific Journal of Phenomenology, 3 (1), 1–19.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia

Sten Langmann

Curtin Business School, Perth, WA, Australia

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sten Langmann .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Langmann, S., Pick, D. (2018). Photography as an Art-Based Research Method. In: Photography as a Social Research Method. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-7279-6_2

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-7279-6_2

Published : 14 December 2017

Publisher Name : Springer, Singapore

Print ISBN : 978-981-10-7277-2

Online ISBN : 978-981-10-7279-6

eBook Packages : Social Sciences Social Sciences (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Photography in Social Science Research

  • Journal of Educational and Social Research 10(4):62
  • This person is not on ResearchGate, or hasn't claimed this research yet.

Sandro Serpa at University of the Azores

  • University of the Azores

Abstract and Figures

with the various photographs that explicitly show order, cleanliness, religiosity and improvements in the facilities -depicts the importance of conveying a positive external image of the organisation in the promotion of a good relationship with the surrounding environment, in the promotion of internal cohesion and, finally, in the published institutional image. In this case, it is safe to conclude that this celebration worked as a celebration of the success of this organization, materialized in its institutional identity, making memory a fundamental resource in this process. These results led to the recognition of the importance of the mobilization of memory in the process of legitimizing an organization (Serpa, 2017). All photography is, by its very nature, manufactured, more or less intentionally, either considering the purposes ascribed to it or the level of technical procedures in the photographer's own framing and relationship with those photographed (Vanti, 2006; Langmann & Pick, 2013; Cruz & Salazar, 2016; Roberts, 2011). Vanti (2006) argues that photography

Discover the world's research

  • 25+ million members
  • 160+ million publication pages
  • 2.3+ billion citations

Josefina Amanda Suyo Vega

  • Carlos Miguel Ferreira
  • Ana Isabel da Silva ASantos
  • Fuzhou Wang

Ana Isabel Santos

  • Carla Beatriz Marques Felipe

Fabio Assis Pinho

  • Ricardo Teixeira

Bernadette Buckley

  • Jacques Hamel
  • Rizzia Rocha
  • Agbeyewornu Kofi Kemevor

Joana Essuman

  • Rev Mexic Soc
  • Ernest Greenwood
  • Oscar Uribe Villegas
  • Recruit researchers
  • Join for free
  • Login Email Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google Welcome back! Please log in. Email · Hint Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google No account? Sign up

Logo

Brill | Nijhoff

Brill | Wageningen Academic

Brill Germany / Austria

Böhlau

Brill | Fink

Brill | mentis

Brill | Schöningh

Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht

V&R unipress

Open Access

Open Access for Authors

Transformative Agreements

Open Access and Research Funding

Open Access for Librarians

Open Access for Academic Societies

Discover Brill’s Open Access Content

Organization

Stay updated

Corporate Social Responsiblity

Investor Relations

Policies, rights & permissions

Review a Brill Book

 
 
 

Author Portal

How to publish with Brill: Files & Guides

Fonts, Scripts and Unicode

Publication Ethics & COPE Compliance

Data Sharing Policy

Brill MyBook

Ordering from Brill

Author Newsletter

Piracy Reporting Form

Sales Managers and Sales Contacts

Ordering From Brill

Titles No Longer Published by Brill

Catalogs, Flyers and Price Lists

E-Book Collections Title Lists and MARC Records

How to Manage your Online Holdings

LibLynx Access Management

Discovery Services

KBART Files

MARC Records

Online User and Order Help

Rights and Permissions

Latest Key Figures

Latest Financial Press Releases and Reports

Annual General Meeting of Shareholders

Share Information

Specialty Products

Press and Reviews

 
   
   
   
   

Share link with colleague or librarian

Stay informed about this journal!

  • Get New Issue Alerts
  • Get Advance Article alerts
  • Get Citation Alerts

Photography as a Research Method with Learners in Compulsory Education: A Research Review

This article offers a review of thirty-one research articles from 2001–2019 on the use of photography as a research method with learners in compulsory education. Understood within the scope of ‘visual’, ‘participatory’ and ‘arts-based’ research methods, many scholars have linked the increased use of the photographic method to greater awareness of the rights of the child and changing understandings of children as full ‘human beings’ with agency rather than simply vulnerable ‘human becomings’. Nevertheless, photography is still a relatively under-utilised approach in research with learners in school-based compulsory education and its use is not widespread globally. Against the background of the history of visual and photographic methods in general and in education in particular, this article highlights two key themes in the empirical research literature: why the photographic method is used (dealing with representation, participation and emancipation); and how the photographic method and the photos themselves are used (pre-generated and participant-generated photographs). It closes with a reflection on what may be holding back its expansion, including key ethical concerns, and a proposal for encouraging its use in education.

  • 1 Introduction

It has been argued that the research methods that social researchers use should be applied creatively so that they can be made ‘fit for purpose’ ( Kara, 2015 ). With this responsibility in mind, a plethora of visual ( Banks, 2001 ; Pink, 2001 ; Rose, 2001 ), arts-based ( Leavy, 2008 ; Knowles & Cole, 2008 ) and participatory ( Reason & Bradbury, 2008 ; Chevalier & Buckles, 2013 ) methods have been evolving, particularly over the last four decades. Scholars have linked this development to a number of shifts, two of which can be related to children and young people and school-based research. The first shift is in understandings of children and young people that came around the time of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child in 1989, and particularly the legal obligation for the ‘best interests’ of children to be taken into consideration, and the views of children to be taken into account on anything that affects them ( Lundy, 2007 ; Lansdowne, 2011 ; Hanna, 2019 ). The second shift relates to the movement within the sociology of childhood that began to more vocally advocate the appreciation of different childhoods where children were increasingly seen not as a vulnerable collective who needed protection, but as individuals holding agency to act on their world and the capacity for independent thought: as fully-fledged ‘human beings’ rather than simply ‘human becomings’ ( James & James, 2004 ). These two shifts, it may be argued, lead to the conclusion that children and young people should be allowed to ‘represent’ themselves to the world ( Prout, 2001 ). Therefore, it is sometimes argued that these, more creative methods hold the potential to offer a more holistic, inclusive and flexible approach to exploring social realities – and a more enjoyable and engaging research experience – particularly with children and young people as research participants ( Thomson, 2008 ; Stirling & Yamada-Rice, 2015).

One method that may be included within this shifting methodological landscape is photography. Used widely within anthropology from the late 19th century, it now holds a firm place in 21st century research methods literature ( Banks, 2001 ). However, while photography has been used fairly frequently as a method in some other fields, as will become clear from the limited number of research articles that were available for this review, it is still a relatively under-utilised approach in school-based compulsory education. This is despite the enduring and perhaps growing salience of visual culture due to the widespread use of social media among younger people in particular ( Woodfield, 2014 ), a conundrum that this Special Issue seeks to take some steps towards addressing.

Against such a background, this article offers a review of thirty-one empirical research articles from the past eighteen years (2001–2019) on the use of photography as a research method with learners in compulsory education. It considers the development of visual and photographic methods in research in general and within educational research in particular. Then it presents the scope of this research review and the search strategy employed to find the articles included within it. Following this, it moves on to the two main themes that emerged from reviewing the research papers, namely: why the photographic method is used (dealing with the key motivations of representation, participation and emancipation); and how the photographic method and the photographs themselves are used (distinguishing between pre-generated and participant-generated photographs). It closes with a reflection on what may be holding back its expansion, including key ethical concerns, and a proposal for encouraging its use in education.

  • 2 Visual and Photographic Methods in Social Research

Photography has variously been described as a ‘visual’, ‘visual ethnographic’, ‘participatory’ and ‘arts-based’ method, depending on how and with whom it is used. Photography first began to be used as a research method within anthropology and ethnography in the early 20th century, when photographic equipment became accessible to researchers ( Banks, 2001 ). In this sense the camera was usually used by the researcher-photographer as a way of capturing an aspect of a community: as ‘photo-documentation’ ( Rose, 2001 ). Since then, the method has seen various evolutions, and particularly a significant movement towards being used as a way of involving participants in the research process itself, either through using ‘found’, researcher-produced or pre-existing photographs, or through participants producing photographs themselves.

Key scholars who have been active in the field of visual ethnography over the past two decades include Sarah Pink (2001) , Gillian Rose (2001) , and Marcus Banks (2001) , who have explored the various debates and dilemmas that have arisen as the method has evolved. For example, in her monograph, Pink (2001) proposes that images are everywhere, ‘inextricably interwoven with our personal identities, narratives, lifestyle, cultures and societies, as well as with definitions of history, space and truth.’ (p. 17) She therefore concludes that visual ethnographic researchers must appreciate the interlinkage between the oral/aural and the visual, for ‘[j]ust as images inspire conversations, conversations may invoke images…images are as inevitable as sounds, words or any other aspect of culture or society.’ (p. 17) Rose (2001) highlights issues of representation in terms of the extent to which an image can represent an object, person, place, time or concept; related to this is the issue of ‘audiencing’ which can influence how a particular image is understood and interpreted, a huge challenge for researchers in any qualitative research but perhaps particularly in visual research. Banks (2001) proposes that visual research is a ‘collaborative project between image maker and image subjects’ and so, social researchers ‘[can]not pretend that they can somehow transcend their humanity and stand outside, merely observing’. (p. 112) There have also been notable contributions from Claudia Mitchell (2011) in her monograph Doing Visual Research and Pat Thomson’s (2008) edited collection Doing Visual Research with Children and Young People . Many of the key debates explored in these monographs and volumes emerge also from the articles selected for this research review and will be returned to later.

In terms of photography in particular, it may be said that its use as a research method has expanded significantly, being used in very different ways to cover different topics and work with different groups of people, to serve different ends and to address or challenge some ethical issues. There have been notable edited collections published from the 1960s onward, such as John Collier’s collaboration with Malcolm Collier in their book Visual Anthropology: Photography as a Research Method . ( Collier & Collier, 1967 ) One of the best-known pioneers in this area has been Caroline Wang (along with Burris in the early days) who from 1997 began to publish on ‘photo-voice’ or ‘participatory photography’ in health and community contexts (see, for example, Wang, 1999 ). ‘Photo-voice’ is a method that involves participants documenting their experiences through photography and then discussing them, with a view to bringing about criticality, empowerment and change, all within a participant-led environment. ( Wang, 1999 ) It is an approach that has been used quite extensively within community settings, where a research participant will often have the use of a disposable camera and will therefore be able to take photographs within their daily lives. In this way, Wang has inspired a whole generation of researchers who wish to use a participatory approach to research, where participant-researchers are fully informed and leading and moulding the research in some way. It is also strongly associated with ‘photo-elicitation’ (Collier & Collier, 1986; Harper, 2002 ) whereby participants describe the photographs and sometimes write short inscriptions for them, that may or may not be shared publicly. However, ‘found’ or researcher-generated photographs have also been used as a way of sparking discussion or debate or tapping into a memory. Both of these approaches will feature in the themes of this research review below.

What may be apparent so far is that the best-known scholars in photography as a research method appear to be located outside of the field of education. While its use within social science research in general may be traced to the 1960s, as Kaplan, Lewis and Mumba (2007) noted, it is very difficult to trace the origin of the photographic method within compulsory education; in 1998 Wetton and McWhirter wrote on health education, in the same year, Prosser (1998) discussed the fact that text is normally more highly valued than images in educational research, and the earliest research paper that could be found for inclusion in this review was from 2001, which investigated sociability and cooperation among 4–5 year olds in England, using researcher-generated photographs ( Broadhead, 2001 ). There are some notable, more recent contributions specific to education that will interest the reader. There is extensive treatment of photographic methods in Miles and Howes’ (2015) edited collection Photography in Educational Research: Critical Reflections from Diverse Contexts . There is also the Wylie Handbook of Ethnography in Education from 2018 which includes a chapter on visual ethnography in education that refers to photography and covers such aspects as ‘participatory photography’ and ‘photo-elicitation’ interviews as well as the challenges of getting access to research sites due to institutional review boards ( Holm, 2018 ). However, while compiling this research review, what became clear was that, although photography as a research method is quite common within the early years of education, as well as in community (non-school) settings and other anthropological or sociological research ( Barker and Smith, 2012 ), it appears to be used less often with learners within compulsory education. Returning to Kaplan et al., they noted this in their work over a decade ago (2007) but it could be argued that the field has not expanded significantly since then. This is despite the fact that the method is clearly and continually being developed in other fields, while appreciation and understanding of visual culture grows. This is a curious point that will be explored more fully in the concluding section of this article, where ethical concerns and understandings of children will feature.

  • 3 Scope of this Review and Search Strategy

This research review is a qualitative, narrative review (Efrat et al., 2019) focused solely on the use of photography as a research method with learners in compulsory education. This type of review aims to ‘survey the state of knowledge in a particular subject area and offers a comprehensive background for understanding that topic.’ (p. 21) It is based on articles in English language journals that report empirical research as this is the only language in which I am fluent. Therefore, it is unsurprising that many of the articles have been written by English native speakers in English-speaking countries.

In terms of search strategy, I followed Reed’s stages (2017). I did an initial general search based on Google and Google Scholar, looking for ‘photography as research method’ to check on terminology. After reading generally about visual and photographic methods, I then turned to academic databases, beginning with Scopus. I did a Boolean search of titles, key words and abstracts, with my search terms refined to ‘photography’, ‘education’/’school’/’learning’ and ‘research’ and ‘method’. I limited the disciplines to ‘arts and humanities’ and ‘social sciences.’ I also limited the search to research articles and excluded books and book chapters. I did not limit the time period at this stage, although I was aware from my initial reading that very little was available from before the early 2000s. I also did not limit the country focus. This brought up 135 research articles. I also used the references in the selected relevant papers that were more broadly on ‘children’ and ‘photography’ to plug gaps, particularly of papers that did not appear on Scopus. Some of these used the term ‘participant photography’ or ‘photovoice’ or ‘visual’ or ‘arts-based’. Finally, I searched the Taylor and Francis journal website to fill in any remaining gaps. After a brief review of titles and abstracts, I excluded papers that did not relate to compulsory education (a significant number). Once I had briefly scanned these articles, I also excluded those that did not focus on the use of the method with learners specifically.

In total the review is based on 31 articles that report empirical research studies. The findings focus on key themes that emerge from this research review. The list of papers included in this review can be found in Table 1 and in list form at the end of this article.

While all of the thirty-one articles reviewed for this paper focused on learners in compulsory education, they covered a wide range of topics within education in a variety of countries. These included gender and bullying in schools in England ( ), inclusion of learners with special educational needs and disabilities in UK, Zambia and Indonesia ( ), the school experiences of Black middle-class male youth in high school in usa ( ), the views of learners in Nordic countries on healthy and unhealthy eating ( ), transition of learners from primary to secondary schools in Bangladesh ( ), the understandings of inclusion held by migrant learners in schools in England ( ) and friendship and meaning in young people’s lives in Australia ( ). The themes explored are necessarily selective and not intended to be exhaustive. However, this thematic section is offered as a summary of some of the key focal points, highlighting significant diversities and commonalities, and with these, some gaps and critical questions that will be discussed and suggested to be taken forward in future research. The two themes are: why photography is used; and how photography and photographs are used. Each of these themes will be treated individually below. Key points from the themes are noted in table .

As referred to earlier, has stated that research methods in social research should be ‘fit for purpose’. This means that a researcher should think carefully about what method would best fit the research topic and the research participants with whom they wish to engage.

It appears that the photographic method is viewed within the selected articles as appropriate for use with a range of different learners in education, and this raises a question about motivation in choosing this method from the vast array of options that are available to any researcher. Despite some critical discussion of the use of the photographic method, most authors were drawn to this approach as an alternative to traditional qualitative methods that focus on the spoken word or observable phenomena, due to its potential to uncover issues that are not often explored, with groups who aren’t often asked, thus adding a richness that may otherwise be absent. Subsequently, three dominant and, in some cases, interlinked terms that emerged were ‘representation’, ‘participation’ and ‘emancipation’. In many cases the researchers discussed more than one of these terms as the focus of the research.

A number of the papers stated that they used photographic methods in order to better ‘represent’ the views or experiences of a particular group of research participants. These were often used in conjunction with other methods, such as in study with teenage female Aboriginal and Maori learners in Australia and New Zealand, that innovated to create the method of ‘photoyarn’ that combined photography with indigenous yarning and yarning circles to represent these students’ experiences. study on spatial mobility of learners viewed representation of children’s ‘real’ experiences as key, as did work with Traveller children and research with learners with special educational needs and disabilities. study on sexual cultures within secondary schools viewed photography as offering participants ‘alternative means of recounting their stories’ (p. 550). Taking representation further, work with children (as part of families) wished to ‘disrupt normative representations of families’ (p. 48). Therefore it was not just about offering a standardized representation of experience, but also challenging current or common representations. Nevertheless, the question remains, and is often raised by these and other authors, about the extent to which a photograph can be described as truly representative of a person, place, time or concept. Reflecting the background debates of many of the studies included in this review, Newman et al., (2006) describe representation as a debate between ‘realist’ and ‘critical’ approaches, between the idea that a photograph can be accurately representative, and the idea that any image invites interpretation and a construction of meaning among a number of actors. There are also significant issues of power, given that it is normally the adult researchers who ultimately get to decide what gets shared from the research work with learners ( ). In this way, it is a deeply ethical issue and researchers need to very carefully consider whether what they are offering is a representation that is in line with what the photographer and/or the photographed intended, in addition to be cognizant of the fact that what the image represents may be viewed differently by the audience (particularly salient in projects where the images are displayed, such as ) and also that this may change over time.

A large number of papers foregrounded the ‘participatory’ side of the photography, where it was chosen in order to promote participation and ‘voice’ in educational research on a particular topic. In paper that focused on the inclusion of learners with special educational needs in education and research, they describe visual methods as appropriate to their study as they ‘gave ‘voice’ and agency to those less able in number, language and textual skills which have systematically biased accounts in past qualitative research.’ (p. 59) work on understandings of learning stated their view that photography ‘offers a very accessible way for young people to become active in the research process themselves, to reverse the normal role of having research done to them, and allow them to participate more in this process’ (p. 366). Similarly, used photography as a way of promoting participation of migrant learners, some non-native speakers of English, in research, due to the lack of competence in English language required to engage with visual methods. , in her extensive research in South Africa, suggested that using such methods forms part of a researcher’s social responsibility to encourage participation in educational research of a variety of groups. Several studies went further to suggest that photographic methods were specifically child-friendly (e.g., ; ; ). However, a question does arise as to the particular ways in which participants might be participating or not participating – such as whether lack of enthusiasm for taking photographs might represent a withdrawal of consent ( ) – as well as whether that creates a false sense of agency to effect change ( ). Furthermore, it must not be assumed that photography is automatically a more ‘child-friendly’ method than any other; returning to , all methods should be ‘fit for purpose’ and adapted to the particular context of the research.

Finally, another tranche of papers in this review focused more on the ‘emancipatory’ potential of using photography as a method in educational research. used photography to highlight the lack of representation for women of colour in the media, and thereby ‘provoked critical conversations about race and gender in their school community.’ (p. 299) viewed photography by Latino/a youth on challenging racism in schools as a way of ‘unsilencing’ them, allowing their voices to be heard as a way of empowering them. used the term ‘counter storytelling’ (Stovall, 2006) to describe how she viewed photography, offering young people of color in usa high schools the chance to counter the dominant narratives about their lives, through not only taking but also displaying their photos. stated that Black male youth participants in his study ‘expressed excitement over getting to share their stories and were hopeful that schoolteachers and administrators would understand them better.’ (p. 452) It is interesting to note that the distinctly emancipatory tone appears to come out most strongly from studies based in the usa where the research participants were secondary school age. This raises questions about whether the perception of agency is absent in many other countries, particularly among primary-age learners, perhaps showing that in compulsory education there remains the sense, introduced earlier, that younger learners are ‘human becomings’ in need of development and adult protection rather than fully-fledged ‘human beings’ with valid opinions and agency ( ).

In addition to the motivation behind using photography, another key theme was how photography and the photographs themselves were used. This is the second theme and is considered below.

Many of the studies in this review paid tribute to the development of specific sub-methods within photography-as-method, such as ‘photo-voice’ (Wang & Burris, 1997) and ‘photo-elicitation’ (Collier & Collier, 1986). However, even where these tried-and-tested methods were employed, researchers used photography and photographs in a variety of ways. There was a clear distinction between those studies that used ‘found’ or pre-generated photographs and those where the participants generated the photographs themselves.

There were only a small number of papers that used pre-generated and/or researcher-generated photographs. These were normally shown to the participants as a way of prompting discussion. study on learners’ perceptions of un/healthy food used pre-generated photographs that were shared with focus groups of children who looked at and then discussed them. Pang et al.’s, (2016) research on subjectivities of un/healthy bodies among Chinese Australian youth used photographs of different bodies from magazines to elicit their views. In part of study, researcher-generated photographs of one school (in Zambia) were shown to learners in another school (in Tanzania) to prompt discussion. study on primary-secondary transition for learners in Bangladesh (2012) used researcher-generated photographs of the learners’ primary school to bring back memories for the learners. Unusually for the selected studies, two papers described researcher-generated photography: uses the photographs taken by herself of a school, and then analyses them herself to explore the school experiences of learners in socioeconomically divergent schools; and uses researcher-generated photographs of shifts in stages of play among young primary school learners. While such studies may raise questions about representation and interpretation (see Rose, 2011), the apparent effectiveness that authors report does pose a challenge to the popular idea that photographs must be generated by participants in order to promote participation in research. It may be that pre-generated photographs are more suitable than participant-generated photographs for exploring particular topics, especially where the issues might be deemed too sensitive to warrant the production of photographs that might be attributed to particular individuals. Indeed, confidentiality is another ethical concern that was mentioned frequently in the articles under review (see, for example, ).

Much more popular than the use of pre-generated photographs was the approach whereby research participants themselves generated photographs. Unsurprisingly, this appears to overlap with studies that saw themselves as overtly ‘representative’, ‘participatory’ or ‘emancipatory’. Many of these studies stated that photo-voice was used, such as Mitchell et al.,’s (2006) work on school absenteeism in rural South Africa, study with Latino/a youth in usa, research on literacy with high school students in usa, and study on wellbeing among adolescents in usa. Usually, research participants were provided with a disposable (e.g. ) or digital camera (e.g. ) and were given the task of taking photographs relating to a particular topic, either within school, with the researcher being present (e.g., ) or outside school, independently and without the researcher being present (e.g. ). Then these cameras were returned to the researcher and at least some of the photographs were developed/printed for the participants to view. The majority of studies that involved participants taking photographs themselves used a version of ‘photo-elicitation’ following the photograph-taking in order to find out participants’ meanings and intentions in taking the photographs, and/or to promote discussion or debate on a particular topic. In O’Donaghue’s (2007) study on gender and masculinities in Irish primary schools, participants added written captions to their images, based also on notes that they had taken down as they took their photographs. paper describes two different approaches: one where a written inscription was added to the photos and another where the photographs were used as a stimulus for oral discussion during individual interviews rather than pinpointing a caption. In their study on deaf children’s experiences of school, used computers to allow participants to edit the images and to add their own interpretations to them. In these kinds of studies, then, the issue of representation and interpretation again came to the fore, with many selecting photo-elicitation as a way of trying to foreground the interpretations of learners rather than those of the adult researchers.

It is worth adding that a small number of studies combined the use of pre-generated and participant-generated photographs. For example, Kirby, Levin and Inchley (2013) investigated the environmental factors that influence physical activity participation among young people in Scotland, where primary school participants created blogs using some of their own and ‘found’ images as a way of capturing the full extent of what they wanted to say, or to keep their own images private. It may be that this offers a more inclusive approach as learner-participants have the choice of creating or finding images that satisfy them, rather than experiencing discomfort in viewing their own ‘inadequate’ photographs or sharing their own images without fully informed consent. Indeed, given that most studies stated that their aim was to be inclusive, then this approach may go some way towards achieving this.

Springing from this, then, is the issue of involvement of research participants in data analysis or in ‘member checking’ once the findings have been drafted. As with the issue of representation, this is an ethical issue, thus showing how ethical dilemmas are threaded through the entire research process, and pertinent often long after the formal research period has come to an end. While it may be argued that photo elicitation goes some way towards involving participants in data analysis, the reality remains that the opportunity to return to the schools and learners and get their input into the analysis of the data was rarely offered or taken (with the exception of computer-based analysis with deaf children). The child rights-based approach developed most extensively by Laura Lundy (see ) would argue that children should be involved at every stage of research, including design, fieldwork, data analysis and dissemination, and so this last stage would be invaluable.

However, it may be argued that the sharing of these photographs (often with captions) is another way of involving participants in data analysis, given that they have some control over what gets shared. This was the case in a number of the studies, where a school- or community-based exhibition was set up (e.g., ; ; ; ; ; ). Nevertheless, it remains the case that the audience has the freedom to interpret the images on display in their own way, which may be entirely different from the intentions of the learner-photographers, and therefore may not promote the kind of ‘counter storytelling’ that scholars such as or might wish for.

This article reviewed 31 research articles from 2001–2019 on the use of photography as a research method with learners in compulsory education. It was set against the background of discussions around ‘visual’, ‘participatory’ and ‘arts-based’ research methods, and the growth of photography as a method in general was attributed to shifts in understandings of children and young people that came around the time of the adoption by the UN of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child in 1989, and the movement within the sociology of childhood that began to more vocally advocate the appreciation of different, agentic childhoods and children. It was highlighted that while photography is popular in some disciplines, it remains a relatively under-utilised research method with learners in school-based compulsory education. Nevertheless, 31 research articles were selected for the qualitative, narrative review, and two themes were drawn out. The first theme related to the motivation behind the authors’ use of photography as a research method. Motivations were classified in three ways: representation, participation and emancipation. While the analysis broadly agreed that these were laudable aims, it also raised issues relating to the extent to which a photograph can represent reality and the issue of interpretation by the photographer, the photographed, and the audience, as well as whether participation always indicates consent and whether participation in research can create a false sense of agency to effect change. Another point that was considered was that it appeared to be the studies based in secondary education in the usa that had an overtly emancipatory agenda, and reflected on whether this indicated that, in other countries and in primary education, there is still the sense that learners cannot effect real change.

The second theme considered how photography and the photographs themselves were used in the studies related in these articles. This was broadly divided into two aspects: the use of pre-generated (or ‘found’) photographs; and the use of participant-generated photographs. While a small number of studies used pre-generated photographs, the overwhelming majority involved participants in generating and sometimes editing photographs, and further sometimes selecting photographs for dissemination. This led to a discussion around involving participants in the interpretation of the photographs, where ‘photo-voice’ and ‘photo-elicitation’ techniques were found to be very popular. It was found that only very few studies managed to return to the participants at a later stage for a final ‘member-check’ on the analysis, which for some authors was regrettable, and perhaps not fully in line with the ‘child rights-based approach’ to research.

Given the relatively small uptake of the photographic research method for use with learners in compulsory education (considering both the articles contained in this review as well as a number of monographs and edited volumes mentioned earlier), it is worth pondering, for a moment, why that is. Its limited use may come as a surprise, given that it feels like we are surrounded (bombarded?) by visual culture on a constant basis in the 21st century. This is not least because of the rapid rise and now pervasiveness of social media, particularly among young people ( ). As , p. 361) notes, there are ‘contradictions between the current widespread practice of visual recordings in public and private spheres and the cautious approach adopted in educational research’. This is a conundrum that this Special Issue on ‘Visual Methods in Educational Research’ seeks, in part, to address, and I would like to make my contribution here too.

One of the biggest concerns that has emerged with the rise of photographic research methods is that of ethics, and certainly ethical concerns were something that many of the authors of the articles in this review very clearly grappled with. It appears to be a particularly thorny issue due to the (perceived or real) higher chance of participants being identified through images of their faces than via text-based methods. There are also ethical considerations around the potential for mis-representation of participants’ images (as mentioned above by ), whether children and young people, unfamiliar with research and researchers, can be said to have given ‘fully informed consent’ and who, ultimately, owns the images. These are challenges over which researchers are not agreed, and different researchers have chosen to use the images in different ways, with some using them in public displays, some using software to turn photographs into cartoons or line drawings, others blurring faces in published work, and still others publishing or even holding on to no images at all. In addition to ethical concerns, allusion was made earlier to understandings of children and young people as ‘human becomings’ rather than ‘human beings’ as it was noted that there has been some shifting in this area, not least thanks to the uncrc 1989. However, it may be that the idea of young people as ‘human becomings’ still prevails and may go some way towards explaining the low uptake of the photographic method in the school environment. While full exploration of these issues is beyond the scope of this study (see , for a good summary), it is important to be aware that the rise of university ethics committees and schools’ increasing concerns about being legally challenged over misuse of images, juxtaposed with the ever-increasing popularity of image-based social media, mean that this debate is likely to continue to evolve. ( ; )

However, I would like to posit that it is not only such important ethical and age-related concerns that may be contributing to the lack of use of photography in school-based research; it may also be that access to schools is restricted as research using photography is not strictly related to the ‘core’ subjects of literacy or numeracy. Therefore, it is a challenge for researchers to convince schools of the benefits of their research and may explain why so many of the studies included in this review were very clear about their intentions in using photography and photographs: many of them had to engage in a process of negotiation with schools in order to gain access ( , is a case in point). This is certainly a challenge that I, as a user of photographic research methods, have faced (see ).

A final conundrum that arises from this research review is the fact that the photographic research method is not widely used in compulsory education outside the ‘West’. This is of particular significance to a journal like this one – the – as there is so little published work on the use of photography as a research method in education in China – in English or, I am reliably informed by my local colleagues, in Chinese. The usa and UK show most often in searches for research papers. It would be useful for future research to consider more extensively why that might be, so that all methods might be considered, and so that we can all work towards using methods that are ‘fit for purpose’ and inclusive of all groups in education.

To close, it is important to return to the motivation behind using photography, and why at least some researchers continue to forge ahead in developing the method. I am convinced that photography can be a useful research method in education as long as one remembers that it is imperfect and partial, and should be understood within and adapted to a context, where the researcher is always ready to abandon the method if it is not effective or appropriate. It can contribute to answering some of the most important questions within education, but it cannot answer any one question in full. The final word is left for the author of one of the articles in this review:

, p. 366)

As I have written elsewhere ( ) as have many others, this need to reflect the narratives of those within the education systems that we, as social researchers, hope to benefit, continues to be a moral imperative and one that requires our continual engagement.

, P. , & Morrow, V. (2004). . Barnardo’s.

, , & , ( ). .

Barnardo’s.)| false , M. (2001). . Sage.

, ( ). .

Sage.)| false , J.M. , & Buckles, D.J. (2013). . Routledge.

, , & , ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , J. , & Collier, M. (1967). . University of New Mexico Press.

, , & , ( ). .

University of New Mexico Press.)| false , S. , & Ravid, R. (2019). . The Guilford Press.

, , & , ( ). .

The Guilford Press.)| false , H. (2019). Children’s Right to Participation and Consultation: Involving them in creative visual research. In C. Capewell and A. Fox , , Research Intelligence, Issue 39, Summer 2019 (pp. 2930). BERA. Available at: .

, ( ). . In

and , , Research Intelligence, Issue 39, Summer 2019 (pp. – ).

BERA. Available at: .)| false , D. (2002). Talking about pictures: A case for photo elicitation. 17(1), 1326.

, ( ). . ( ), – .)| false , G. (2018). Visual ethnography in education. In D. Beach , C. Bagley and S. Marques da Silva (Eds.), (pp. 325–354). Wiley.

, ( ). . In

, and (Eds.), (pp. 325–354).

Wiley.)| false , A. and James, A. (2004). . Palgrave Macmillan.

, and , ( ). .

Palgrave Macmillan.)| false , H. (2015). . Policy Press.

, ( ). .

Policy Press.)| false , J. G. , & Cole, A. L. (Eds.). (2008). . SAGE.

, , & , (Eds.). ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , G. (2011). . Save the Children Fund/unicef.

, ( ). . Save the Children Fund/ .)| false , P. (2008). . Guilford Publications.

, ( ). .

Guilford Publications.)| false , L. (2007). ‘Voice’ is Not Enough: Conceptualising Article 12 of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child. , 33(6), 927942.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , S. , & Howes, A. (Eds.). (2015). . Routledge.

, , & , (Eds.). ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , C. (2016). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , S. (2001). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , H. , & Frankham, J. (2007). Seeing voices and hearing pictures: Image as discourse and the framing of image-based research. , 28(3), 373387.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. (1998). The status of image-based research. In J. Prosser (Ed.), . (pp. 97–112) Routledge Falmer.

, ( ). . In

(Ed.), . (pp. 97–112) Routledge Falmer.)| false , A. (2001). Representing children: reflections on the children 5–16 programme, , 15, 193201.

, ( ). , , , – .)| false , P. , & Bradbury, H. (Eds). (2008). . SAGE.

, , & , (Eds). ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , M. (2017). . Fast Track Impact.

, ( ). .

Fast Track Impact.)| false , G. (2001). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , E. , & Yamada-Rice, D. (Eds.). (2015). . Palgrave Macmillan.

, , & ( ). .

Palgrave Macmillan.)| false , P. (Ed.). (2008). . Routledge.

, (Ed.). ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , C. (1999). Photovoice: A participatory action research strategy applied to women’s health. , 8(2), 185192.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. , & Redwood-Jones Y. (2001). Photovoice ethics: perspectives from flint photovoice. 28, 560572.

, , & ( ). . , – .)| false , N.N. , & McWhirter, J. (1998). Images and curriculum development in health education, in J. Prosser (Ed.), (pp. 263–283). Falmer Press.

, , & , ( ). , in

(Ed.), (pp. 263–283).

Falmer Press.)| false , K. (2014). . NatCen Social Research.

, ( ). . NatCen Social Research.)| false , L. (2009). ‘Snapped’: researching the sexual cultures of schools. using visual methods. , 22(5), 549561.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , Q. (2012). Photographs and stories: ethics, benefits and dilemmas of using participant photography with Black middle-class male youth. , 12(4), 443458.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Weller, S. (2003). “Is it Fun?” Developing Children Centred Research Methods. , 23(1/2), 3358.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Smith, F. (2012). What’s in focus? A critical discussion of photography, children and young people. , 15(2), 91103.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , L. , Talvia, S. , Fossgard, E. , Arnfjörð, U. B. , Hörnell, A. , Ólafsdóttir, A.S. , Gunnarsdóttir, I. , Wergedahl, H. , Lagström, H. , Waling, M. , & Olsson, C. (2017). Nordic children’s conceptualizations of healthy eating in relation to school lunch. , 117(2), 130147.

, , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , P. (2001). Investigating Sociability and Cooperation in Four and Five Year Olds in Reception Class Settings, , 9(1), 2335.

, ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false , M. , & Martinez, S. (2016). Consciousness-raising or unintentionally oppressive? Potential negative consequences of photovoice. , 21(5), 798810.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , D. (2019). Re-imagining research partnerships: Thinking through “co-research” and ethical practice with children and youth. , 13(1), 4058.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , T. , & Hess, E. (2007). What the camera sees and from whose perspective: Fun methodologies for engaging children in enlightening adults. , 14(1), 2945.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2007). Young Travellers and the children’s fund: Some practical notes on an experimental image-based research project. , 7(1), 1622.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , T. , & Curmi-Hall, C. (2019). Transforming School Hallways Through Critical Inquiry: Multimodal Literacies for Civic Engagement. , 63(3), 299309.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , K. (2018). Increasing the Depth of Field: Critical Race Theory and Photovoice as Counter Storytelling Praxis. , 50, 648674.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false , H. (2018). ‘Migrant learners as experts: Exploring their perspectives on inclusion in a primary school in England’. . .

, ( ). ‘ ’. . .)| false , I. , Lewis, I. , & Mumba, P. (2007). Picturing global educational inclusion? Looking and thinking across students’ photographs from the UK, Zambia and Indonesia. , 7(1), 2335.

, , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , Levin, K. , & Inchley, J. (2013). Socio-Environmental influences on physical activity among young people: A qualitative study. , 28(6), 954969.

, , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2009). About face: visual research involving children, , 37(4), 361370.

, ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false , S. (2011). A understanding of a case study: Analysis of a robust qualitative research methodology. , 17(3), 310328.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2008). Getting the picture and changing the picture: visual methodologies and educational research in South Africa. , 28, 365383.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false , C. , Stuart, J. , Moletsane, R. , & Nkwanyana, C.B. (2006). Why We Don’t Go to School on Fridays: On Youth Participation through Photo Voice in Rural KwaZulu-Natal. , 41(3), 267282.

, , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , M. , Woodcock, A. , & Dunham, P. (2006). ‘Playtime in the Borderlands’: Children’s Representations of School, Gender and Bullying through Photographs and Interviews, , 4(3), 289302.

, , , , & , ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false Donoghue , D. (2007). ‘James always hangs out here’: making space for place in studying masculinities at school. , 22(1), 6273.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , Harm, C. , & Cohen, L. (2019). They’re always there for me! Friendship and meaning in young people’s lives? , 60, 596608.

, , , , & , ( ). , , – .)| false , B. , Alfrey, L. , & Varea, V. (2016). Young Chinese Australians’ subjectivities of ‘health’ and ‘(un)healthy bodies’. , 21(7), 10911108.

, , , , &

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Loxley, A. (2007). Enhancing the contribution of visual methods to inclusive education. , 7(1), 5568.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. (2017). Photoyarn: Aboriginal and Mãori girls’ researching contemporary boarding school experiences. , 1, 313.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false T. , Shdaimah, C. , de Tablan, D. , & Sharpe, T. (2016). Exploring wellbeing and agency among urban youth through photovoice. , 67, 114122.

, , , , , & , ( ). . , , – .)| false , M.M.C. (2012). Nostalgia, transition and the school: an innovative approach of using photographic images as a visual method in educational research. , 35(3), 269292.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , H. , & Young, A. (2014). Research with deaf children and not on them: A study of method and process. , 28, 366379.

, , & , ( ). . , , – .)| false , E. (2011). Off to school: A comparative study of schools in the U.S. , 1(2), 4558.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , A. , Bushin, N. , Carpena-Méndez, F. , & Ní Laoire, C. (2010). Using visual methodologies to explore contemporary Irish childhoods. , 10(2), 143158.

, , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , K. , Pellegrino, A. , Harmon, J. , Ewaida, M. , Bell, A. , Lynch, M. , & Sell, C. (2013). Picturing Culturally Relevant Literacy Practices: Using Photography to See How Literacy Curricula and Pedagogies Matter to Urban Youth. , 15(2), 120.

, , , , , , , , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false ; ; ; ; ; , , P. , & Morrow, V. (2004). . Barnardo’s.

, , & , ( ). .

Barnardo’s.)| false , M. (2001). . Sage.

, ( ). .

Sage.)| false , J.M. , & Buckles, D.J. (2013). . Routledge.

, , & , ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , J. , & Collier, M. (1967). . University of New Mexico Press.

, , & , ( ). .

University of New Mexico Press.)| false , S. , & Ravid, R. (2019). . The Guilford Press.

, , & , ( ). .

The Guilford Press.)| false , H. (2019). Children’s Right to Participation and Consultation: Involving them in creative visual research. In C. Capewell and A. Fox , , Research Intelligence, Issue 39, Summer 2019 (pp. 2930). BERA. Available at: .

, ( ). . In

and , , Research Intelligence, Issue 39, Summer 2019 (pp. – ).

BERA. Available at: .)| false , D. (2002). Talking about pictures: A case for photo elicitation. 17(1), 1326.

, ( ). . ( ), – .)| false , G. (2018). Visual ethnography in education. In D. Beach , C. Bagley and S. Marques da Silva (Eds.), (pp. 325–354). Wiley.

, ( ). . In

, and (Eds.), (pp. 325–354).

Wiley.)| false , A. and James, A. (2004). . Palgrave Macmillan.

, and , ( ). .

Palgrave Macmillan.)| false , H. (2015). . Policy Press.

, ( ). .

Policy Press.)| false , J. G. , & Cole, A. L. (Eds.). (2008). . SAGE.

, , & , (Eds.). ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , G. (2011). . Save the Children Fund/unicef.

, ( ). . Save the Children Fund/ .)| false , P. (2008). . Guilford Publications.

, ( ). .

Guilford Publications.)| false , L. (2007). ‘Voice’ is Not Enough: Conceptualising Article 12 of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child. , 33(6), 927942.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , S. , & Howes, A. (Eds.). (2015). . Routledge.

, , & , (Eds.). ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , C. (2016). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , S. (2001). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , H. , & Frankham, J. (2007). Seeing voices and hearing pictures: Image as discourse and the framing of image-based research. , 28(3), 373387.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. (1998). The status of image-based research. In J. Prosser (Ed.), . (pp. 97–112) Routledge Falmer.

, ( ). . In

(Ed.), . (pp. 97–112) Routledge Falmer.)| false , A. (2001). Representing children: reflections on the children 5–16 programme, , 15, 193201.

, ( ). , , , – .)| false , P. , & Bradbury, H. (Eds). (2008). . SAGE.

, , & , (Eds). ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , M. (2017). . Fast Track Impact.

, ( ). .

Fast Track Impact.)| false , G. (2001). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , E. , & Yamada-Rice, D. (Eds.). (2015). . Palgrave Macmillan.

, , & ( ). .

Palgrave Macmillan.)| false , P. (Ed.). (2008). . Routledge.

, (Ed.). ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , C. (1999). Photovoice: A participatory action research strategy applied to women’s health. , 8(2), 185192.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. , & Redwood-Jones Y. (2001). Photovoice ethics: perspectives from flint photovoice. 28, 560572.

, , & ( ). . , – .)| false , N.N. , & McWhirter, J. (1998). Images and curriculum development in health education, in J. Prosser (Ed.), (pp. 263–283). Falmer Press.

, , & , ( ). , in

(Ed.), (pp. 263–283).

Falmer Press.)| false , K. (2014). . NatCen Social Research.

, ( ). . NatCen Social Research.)| false , L. (2009). ‘Snapped’: researching the sexual cultures of schools. using visual methods. , 22(5), 549561.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , Q. (2012). Photographs and stories: ethics, benefits and dilemmas of using participant photography with Black middle-class male youth. , 12(4), 443458.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Weller, S. (2003). “Is it Fun?” Developing Children Centred Research Methods. , 23(1/2), 3358.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Smith, F. (2012). What’s in focus? A critical discussion of photography, children and young people. , 15(2), 91103.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , L. , Talvia, S. , Fossgard, E. , Arnfjörð, U. B. , Hörnell, A. , Ólafsdóttir, A.S. , Gunnarsdóttir, I. , Wergedahl, H. , Lagström, H. , Waling, M. , & Olsson, C. (2017). Nordic children’s conceptualizations of healthy eating in relation to school lunch. , 117(2), 130147.

, , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , P. (2001). Investigating Sociability and Cooperation in Four and Five Year Olds in Reception Class Settings, , 9(1), 2335.

, ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false , M. , & Martinez, S. (2016). Consciousness-raising or unintentionally oppressive? Potential negative consequences of photovoice. , 21(5), 798810.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , D. (2019). Re-imagining research partnerships: Thinking through “co-research” and ethical practice with children and youth. , 13(1), 4058.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , T. , & Hess, E. (2007). What the camera sees and from whose perspective: Fun methodologies for engaging children in enlightening adults. , 14(1), 2945.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2007). Young Travellers and the children’s fund: Some practical notes on an experimental image-based research project. , 7(1), 1622.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , T. , & Curmi-Hall, C. (2019). Transforming School Hallways Through Critical Inquiry: Multimodal Literacies for Civic Engagement. , 63(3), 299309.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , K. (2018). Increasing the Depth of Field: Critical Race Theory and Photovoice as Counter Storytelling Praxis. , 50, 648674.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false , H. (2018). ‘Migrant learners as experts: Exploring their perspectives on inclusion in a primary school in England’. . .

, ( ). ‘ ’. . .)| false , I. , Lewis, I. , & Mumba, P. (2007). Picturing global educational inclusion? Looking and thinking across students’ photographs from the UK, Zambia and Indonesia. , 7(1), 2335.

, , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , Levin, K. , & Inchley, J. (2013). Socio-Environmental influences on physical activity among young people: A qualitative study. , 28(6), 954969.

, , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2009). About face: visual research involving children, , 37(4), 361370.

, ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false , S. (2011). A understanding of a case study: Analysis of a robust qualitative research methodology. , 17(3), 310328.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2008). Getting the picture and changing the picture: visual methodologies and educational research in South Africa. , 28, 365383.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false , C. , Stuart, J. , Moletsane, R. , & Nkwanyana, C.B. (2006). Why We Don’t Go to School on Fridays: On Youth Participation through Photo Voice in Rural KwaZulu-Natal. , 41(3), 267282.

, , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , M. , Woodcock, A. , & Dunham, P. (2006). ‘Playtime in the Borderlands’: Children’s Representations of School, Gender and Bullying through Photographs and Interviews, , 4(3), 289302.

, , , , & , ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false Donoghue , D. (2007). ‘James always hangs out here’: making space for place in studying masculinities at school. , 22(1), 6273.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , Harm, C. , & Cohen, L. (2019). They’re always there for me! Friendship and meaning in young people’s lives? , 60, 596608.

, , , , & , ( ). , , – .)| false , B. , Alfrey, L. , & Varea, V. (2016). Young Chinese Australians’ subjectivities of ‘health’ and ‘(un)healthy bodies’. , 21(7), 10911108.

, , , , &

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Loxley, A. (2007). Enhancing the contribution of visual methods to inclusive education. , 7(1), 5568.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. (2017). Photoyarn: Aboriginal and Mãori girls’ researching contemporary boarding school experiences. , 1, 313.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false T. , Shdaimah, C. , de Tablan, D. , & Sharpe, T. (2016). Exploring wellbeing and agency among urban youth through photovoice. , 67, 114122.

, , , , , & , ( ). . , , – .)| false , M.M.C. (2012). Nostalgia, transition and the school: an innovative approach of using photographic images as a visual method in educational research. , 35(3), 269292.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , H. , & Young, A. (2014). Research with deaf children and not on them: A study of method and process. , 28, 366379.

, , & , ( ). . , , – .)| false , E. (2011). Off to school: A comparative study of schools in the U.S. , 1(2), 4558.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , A. , Bushin, N. , Carpena-Méndez, F. , & Ní Laoire, C. (2010). Using visual methodologies to explore contemporary Irish childhoods. , 10(2), 143158.

, , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , K. , Pellegrino, A. , Harmon, J. , Ewaida, M. , Bell, A. , Lynch, M. , & Sell, C. (2013). Picturing Culturally Relevant Literacy Practices: Using Photography to See How Literacy Curricula and Pedagogies Matter to Urban Youth. , 15(2), 120.

, , , , , , , , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false All Time Past Year Past 30 Days Abstract Views 221 0 0 Full Text Views 7532 1851 59 PDF Views & Downloads 9357 2127 89

While all of the thirty-one articles reviewed for this paper focused on learners in compulsory education, they covered a wide range of topics within education in a variety of countries. These included gender and bullying in schools in England ( ), inclusion of learners with special educational needs and disabilities in UK, Zambia and Indonesia ( ), the school experiences of Black middle-class male youth in high school in usa ( ), the views of learners in Nordic countries on healthy and unhealthy eating ( ), transition of learners from primary to secondary schools in Bangladesh ( ), the understandings of inclusion held by migrant learners in schools in England ( ) and friendship and meaning in young people’s lives in Australia ( ). The themes explored are necessarily selective and not intended to be exhaustive. However, this thematic section is offered as a summary of some of the key focal points, highlighting significant diversities and commonalities, and with these, some gaps and critical questions that will be discussed and suggested to be taken forward in future research. The two themes are: why photography is used; and how photography and photographs are used. Each of these themes will be treated individually below. Key points from the themes are noted in table .

As referred to earlier, has stated that research methods in social research should be ‘fit for purpose’. This means that a researcher should think carefully about what method would best fit the research topic and the research participants with whom they wish to engage.

It appears that the photographic method is viewed within the selected articles as appropriate for use with a range of different learners in education, and this raises a question about motivation in choosing this method from the vast array of options that are available to any researcher. Despite some critical discussion of the use of the photographic method, most authors were drawn to this approach as an alternative to traditional qualitative methods that focus on the spoken word or observable phenomena, due to its potential to uncover issues that are not often explored, with groups who aren’t often asked, thus adding a richness that may otherwise be absent. Subsequently, three dominant and, in some cases, interlinked terms that emerged were ‘representation’, ‘participation’ and ‘emancipation’. In many cases the researchers discussed more than one of these terms as the focus of the research.

A number of the papers stated that they used photographic methods in order to better ‘represent’ the views or experiences of a particular group of research participants. These were often used in conjunction with other methods, such as in study with teenage female Aboriginal and Maori learners in Australia and New Zealand, that innovated to create the method of ‘photoyarn’ that combined photography with indigenous yarning and yarning circles to represent these students’ experiences. study on spatial mobility of learners viewed representation of children’s ‘real’ experiences as key, as did work with Traveller children and research with learners with special educational needs and disabilities. study on sexual cultures within secondary schools viewed photography as offering participants ‘alternative means of recounting their stories’ (p. 550). Taking representation further, work with children (as part of families) wished to ‘disrupt normative representations of families’ (p. 48). Therefore it was not just about offering a standardized representation of experience, but also challenging current or common representations. Nevertheless, the question remains, and is often raised by these and other authors, about the extent to which a photograph can be described as truly representative of a person, place, time or concept. Reflecting the background debates of many of the studies included in this review, Newman et al., (2006) describe representation as a debate between ‘realist’ and ‘critical’ approaches, between the idea that a photograph can be accurately representative, and the idea that any image invites interpretation and a construction of meaning among a number of actors. There are also significant issues of power, given that it is normally the adult researchers who ultimately get to decide what gets shared from the research work with learners ( ). In this way, it is a deeply ethical issue and researchers need to very carefully consider whether what they are offering is a representation that is in line with what the photographer and/or the photographed intended, in addition to be cognizant of the fact that what the image represents may be viewed differently by the audience (particularly salient in projects where the images are displayed, such as ) and also that this may change over time.

A large number of papers foregrounded the ‘participatory’ side of the photography, where it was chosen in order to promote participation and ‘voice’ in educational research on a particular topic. In paper that focused on the inclusion of learners with special educational needs in education and research, they describe visual methods as appropriate to their study as they ‘gave ‘voice’ and agency to those less able in number, language and textual skills which have systematically biased accounts in past qualitative research.’ (p. 59) work on understandings of learning stated their view that photography ‘offers a very accessible way for young people to become active in the research process themselves, to reverse the normal role of having research done to them, and allow them to participate more in this process’ (p. 366). Similarly, used photography as a way of promoting participation of migrant learners, some non-native speakers of English, in research, due to the lack of competence in English language required to engage with visual methods. , in her extensive research in South Africa, suggested that using such methods forms part of a researcher’s social responsibility to encourage participation in educational research of a variety of groups. Several studies went further to suggest that photographic methods were specifically child-friendly (e.g., ; ; ). However, a question does arise as to the particular ways in which participants might be participating or not participating – such as whether lack of enthusiasm for taking photographs might represent a withdrawal of consent ( ) – as well as whether that creates a false sense of agency to effect change ( ). Furthermore, it must not be assumed that photography is automatically a more ‘child-friendly’ method than any other; returning to , all methods should be ‘fit for purpose’ and adapted to the particular context of the research.

Finally, another tranche of papers in this review focused more on the ‘emancipatory’ potential of using photography as a method in educational research. used photography to highlight the lack of representation for women of colour in the media, and thereby ‘provoked critical conversations about race and gender in their school community.’ (p. 299) viewed photography by Latino/a youth on challenging racism in schools as a way of ‘unsilencing’ them, allowing their voices to be heard as a way of empowering them. used the term ‘counter storytelling’ (Stovall, 2006) to describe how she viewed photography, offering young people of color in usa high schools the chance to counter the dominant narratives about their lives, through not only taking but also displaying their photos. stated that Black male youth participants in his study ‘expressed excitement over getting to share their stories and were hopeful that schoolteachers and administrators would understand them better.’ (p. 452) It is interesting to note that the distinctly emancipatory tone appears to come out most strongly from studies based in the usa where the research participants were secondary school age. This raises questions about whether the perception of agency is absent in many other countries, particularly among primary-age learners, perhaps showing that in compulsory education there remains the sense, introduced earlier, that younger learners are ‘human becomings’ in need of development and adult protection rather than fully-fledged ‘human beings’ with valid opinions and agency ( ).

In addition to the motivation behind using photography, another key theme was how photography and the photographs themselves were used. This is the second theme and is considered below.

Many of the studies in this review paid tribute to the development of specific sub-methods within photography-as-method, such as ‘photo-voice’ (Wang & Burris, 1997) and ‘photo-elicitation’ (Collier & Collier, 1986). However, even where these tried-and-tested methods were employed, researchers used photography and photographs in a variety of ways. There was a clear distinction between those studies that used ‘found’ or pre-generated photographs and those where the participants generated the photographs themselves.

There were only a small number of papers that used pre-generated and/or researcher-generated photographs. These were normally shown to the participants as a way of prompting discussion. study on learners’ perceptions of un/healthy food used pre-generated photographs that were shared with focus groups of children who looked at and then discussed them. Pang et al.’s, (2016) research on subjectivities of un/healthy bodies among Chinese Australian youth used photographs of different bodies from magazines to elicit their views. In part of study, researcher-generated photographs of one school (in Zambia) were shown to learners in another school (in Tanzania) to prompt discussion. study on primary-secondary transition for learners in Bangladesh (2012) used researcher-generated photographs of the learners’ primary school to bring back memories for the learners. Unusually for the selected studies, two papers described researcher-generated photography: uses the photographs taken by herself of a school, and then analyses them herself to explore the school experiences of learners in socioeconomically divergent schools; and uses researcher-generated photographs of shifts in stages of play among young primary school learners. While such studies may raise questions about representation and interpretation (see Rose, 2011), the apparent effectiveness that authors report does pose a challenge to the popular idea that photographs must be generated by participants in order to promote participation in research. It may be that pre-generated photographs are more suitable than participant-generated photographs for exploring particular topics, especially where the issues might be deemed too sensitive to warrant the production of photographs that might be attributed to particular individuals. Indeed, confidentiality is another ethical concern that was mentioned frequently in the articles under review (see, for example, ).

Much more popular than the use of pre-generated photographs was the approach whereby research participants themselves generated photographs. Unsurprisingly, this appears to overlap with studies that saw themselves as overtly ‘representative’, ‘participatory’ or ‘emancipatory’. Many of these studies stated that photo-voice was used, such as Mitchell et al.,’s (2006) work on school absenteeism in rural South Africa, study with Latino/a youth in usa, research on literacy with high school students in usa, and study on wellbeing among adolescents in usa. Usually, research participants were provided with a disposable (e.g. ) or digital camera (e.g. ) and were given the task of taking photographs relating to a particular topic, either within school, with the researcher being present (e.g., ) or outside school, independently and without the researcher being present (e.g. ). Then these cameras were returned to the researcher and at least some of the photographs were developed/printed for the participants to view. The majority of studies that involved participants taking photographs themselves used a version of ‘photo-elicitation’ following the photograph-taking in order to find out participants’ meanings and intentions in taking the photographs, and/or to promote discussion or debate on a particular topic. In O’Donaghue’s (2007) study on gender and masculinities in Irish primary schools, participants added written captions to their images, based also on notes that they had taken down as they took their photographs. paper describes two different approaches: one where a written inscription was added to the photos and another where the photographs were used as a stimulus for oral discussion during individual interviews rather than pinpointing a caption. In their study on deaf children’s experiences of school, used computers to allow participants to edit the images and to add their own interpretations to them. In these kinds of studies, then, the issue of representation and interpretation again came to the fore, with many selecting photo-elicitation as a way of trying to foreground the interpretations of learners rather than those of the adult researchers.

It is worth adding that a small number of studies combined the use of pre-generated and participant-generated photographs. For example, Kirby, Levin and Inchley (2013) investigated the environmental factors that influence physical activity participation among young people in Scotland, where primary school participants created blogs using some of their own and ‘found’ images as a way of capturing the full extent of what they wanted to say, or to keep their own images private. It may be that this offers a more inclusive approach as learner-participants have the choice of creating or finding images that satisfy them, rather than experiencing discomfort in viewing their own ‘inadequate’ photographs or sharing their own images without fully informed consent. Indeed, given that most studies stated that their aim was to be inclusive, then this approach may go some way towards achieving this.

Springing from this, then, is the issue of involvement of research participants in data analysis or in ‘member checking’ once the findings have been drafted. As with the issue of representation, this is an ethical issue, thus showing how ethical dilemmas are threaded through the entire research process, and pertinent often long after the formal research period has come to an end. While it may be argued that photo elicitation goes some way towards involving participants in data analysis, the reality remains that the opportunity to return to the schools and learners and get their input into the analysis of the data was rarely offered or taken (with the exception of computer-based analysis with deaf children). The child rights-based approach developed most extensively by Laura Lundy (see ) would argue that children should be involved at every stage of research, including design, fieldwork, data analysis and dissemination, and so this last stage would be invaluable.

However, it may be argued that the sharing of these photographs (often with captions) is another way of involving participants in data analysis, given that they have some control over what gets shared. This was the case in a number of the studies, where a school- or community-based exhibition was set up (e.g., ; ; ; ; ; ). Nevertheless, it remains the case that the audience has the freedom to interpret the images on display in their own way, which may be entirely different from the intentions of the learner-photographers, and therefore may not promote the kind of ‘counter storytelling’ that scholars such as or might wish for.

This article reviewed 31 research articles from 2001–2019 on the use of photography as a research method with learners in compulsory education. It was set against the background of discussions around ‘visual’, ‘participatory’ and ‘arts-based’ research methods, and the growth of photography as a method in general was attributed to shifts in understandings of children and young people that came around the time of the adoption by the UN of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child in 1989, and the movement within the sociology of childhood that began to more vocally advocate the appreciation of different, agentic childhoods and children. It was highlighted that while photography is popular in some disciplines, it remains a relatively under-utilised research method with learners in school-based compulsory education. Nevertheless, 31 research articles were selected for the qualitative, narrative review, and two themes were drawn out. The first theme related to the motivation behind the authors’ use of photography as a research method. Motivations were classified in three ways: representation, participation and emancipation. While the analysis broadly agreed that these were laudable aims, it also raised issues relating to the extent to which a photograph can represent reality and the issue of interpretation by the photographer, the photographed, and the audience, as well as whether participation always indicates consent and whether participation in research can create a false sense of agency to effect change. Another point that was considered was that it appeared to be the studies based in secondary education in the usa that had an overtly emancipatory agenda, and reflected on whether this indicated that, in other countries and in primary education, there is still the sense that learners cannot effect real change.

The second theme considered how photography and the photographs themselves were used in the studies related in these articles. This was broadly divided into two aspects: the use of pre-generated (or ‘found’) photographs; and the use of participant-generated photographs. While a small number of studies used pre-generated photographs, the overwhelming majority involved participants in generating and sometimes editing photographs, and further sometimes selecting photographs for dissemination. This led to a discussion around involving participants in the interpretation of the photographs, where ‘photo-voice’ and ‘photo-elicitation’ techniques were found to be very popular. It was found that only very few studies managed to return to the participants at a later stage for a final ‘member-check’ on the analysis, which for some authors was regrettable, and perhaps not fully in line with the ‘child rights-based approach’ to research.

Given the relatively small uptake of the photographic research method for use with learners in compulsory education (considering both the articles contained in this review as well as a number of monographs and edited volumes mentioned earlier), it is worth pondering, for a moment, why that is. Its limited use may come as a surprise, given that it feels like we are surrounded (bombarded?) by visual culture on a constant basis in the 21st century. This is not least because of the rapid rise and now pervasiveness of social media, particularly among young people ( ). As , p. 361) notes, there are ‘contradictions between the current widespread practice of visual recordings in public and private spheres and the cautious approach adopted in educational research’. This is a conundrum that this Special Issue on ‘Visual Methods in Educational Research’ seeks, in part, to address, and I would like to make my contribution here too.

One of the biggest concerns that has emerged with the rise of photographic research methods is that of ethics, and certainly ethical concerns were something that many of the authors of the articles in this review very clearly grappled with. It appears to be a particularly thorny issue due to the (perceived or real) higher chance of participants being identified through images of their faces than via text-based methods. There are also ethical considerations around the potential for mis-representation of participants’ images (as mentioned above by ), whether children and young people, unfamiliar with research and researchers, can be said to have given ‘fully informed consent’ and who, ultimately, owns the images. These are challenges over which researchers are not agreed, and different researchers have chosen to use the images in different ways, with some using them in public displays, some using software to turn photographs into cartoons or line drawings, others blurring faces in published work, and still others publishing or even holding on to no images at all. In addition to ethical concerns, allusion was made earlier to understandings of children and young people as ‘human becomings’ rather than ‘human beings’ as it was noted that there has been some shifting in this area, not least thanks to the uncrc 1989. However, it may be that the idea of young people as ‘human becomings’ still prevails and may go some way towards explaining the low uptake of the photographic method in the school environment. While full exploration of these issues is beyond the scope of this study (see , for a good summary), it is important to be aware that the rise of university ethics committees and schools’ increasing concerns about being legally challenged over misuse of images, juxtaposed with the ever-increasing popularity of image-based social media, mean that this debate is likely to continue to evolve. ( ; )

However, I would like to posit that it is not only such important ethical and age-related concerns that may be contributing to the lack of use of photography in school-based research; it may also be that access to schools is restricted as research using photography is not strictly related to the ‘core’ subjects of literacy or numeracy. Therefore, it is a challenge for researchers to convince schools of the benefits of their research and may explain why so many of the studies included in this review were very clear about their intentions in using photography and photographs: many of them had to engage in a process of negotiation with schools in order to gain access ( , is a case in point). This is certainly a challenge that I, as a user of photographic research methods, have faced (see ).

A final conundrum that arises from this research review is the fact that the photographic research method is not widely used in compulsory education outside the ‘West’. This is of particular significance to a journal like this one – the – as there is so little published work on the use of photography as a research method in education in China – in English or, I am reliably informed by my local colleagues, in Chinese. The usa and UK show most often in searches for research papers. It would be useful for future research to consider more extensively why that might be, so that all methods might be considered, and so that we can all work towards using methods that are ‘fit for purpose’ and inclusive of all groups in education.

To close, it is important to return to the motivation behind using photography, and why at least some researchers continue to forge ahead in developing the method. I am convinced that photography can be a useful research method in education as long as one remembers that it is imperfect and partial, and should be understood within and adapted to a context, where the researcher is always ready to abandon the method if it is not effective or appropriate. It can contribute to answering some of the most important questions within education, but it cannot answer any one question in full. The final word is left for the author of one of the articles in this review:

, p. 366)

As I have written elsewhere ( ) as have many others, this need to reflect the narratives of those within the education systems that we, as social researchers, hope to benefit, continues to be a moral imperative and one that requires our continual engagement.

, P. , & Morrow, V. (2004). . Barnardo’s.

, , & , ( ). .

Barnardo’s.)| false , M. (2001). . Sage.

, ( ). .

Sage.)| false , J.M. , & Buckles, D.J. (2013). . Routledge.

, , & , ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , J. , & Collier, M. (1967). . University of New Mexico Press.

, , & , ( ). .

University of New Mexico Press.)| false , S. , & Ravid, R. (2019). . The Guilford Press.

, , & , ( ). .

The Guilford Press.)| false , H. (2019). Children’s Right to Participation and Consultation: Involving them in creative visual research. In C. Capewell and A. Fox , , Research Intelligence, Issue 39, Summer 2019 (pp. 2930). BERA. Available at: .

, ( ). . In

and , , Research Intelligence, Issue 39, Summer 2019 (pp. – ).

BERA. Available at: .)| false , D. (2002). Talking about pictures: A case for photo elicitation. 17(1), 1326.

, ( ). . ( ), – .)| false , G. (2018). Visual ethnography in education. In D. Beach , C. Bagley and S. Marques da Silva (Eds.), (pp. 325–354). Wiley.

, ( ). . In

, and (Eds.), (pp. 325–354).

Wiley.)| false , A. and James, A. (2004). . Palgrave Macmillan.

, and , ( ). .

Palgrave Macmillan.)| false , H. (2015). . Policy Press.

, ( ). .

Policy Press.)| false , J. G. , & Cole, A. L. (Eds.). (2008). . SAGE.

, , & , (Eds.). ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , G. (2011). . Save the Children Fund/unicef.

, ( ). . Save the Children Fund/ .)| false , P. (2008). . Guilford Publications.

, ( ). .

Guilford Publications.)| false , L. (2007). ‘Voice’ is Not Enough: Conceptualising Article 12 of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child. , 33(6), 927942.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , S. , & Howes, A. (Eds.). (2015). . Routledge.

, , & , (Eds.). ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , C. (2016). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , S. (2001). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , H. , & Frankham, J. (2007). Seeing voices and hearing pictures: Image as discourse and the framing of image-based research. , 28(3), 373387.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. (1998). The status of image-based research. In J. Prosser (Ed.), . (pp. 97–112) Routledge Falmer.

, ( ). . In

(Ed.), . (pp. 97–112) Routledge Falmer.)| false , A. (2001). Representing children: reflections on the children 5–16 programme, , 15, 193201.

, ( ). , , , – .)| false , P. , & Bradbury, H. (Eds). (2008). . SAGE.

, , & , (Eds). ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , M. (2017). . Fast Track Impact.

, ( ). .

Fast Track Impact.)| false , G. (2001). . SAGE.

, ( ). .

SAGE.)| false , E. , & Yamada-Rice, D. (Eds.). (2015). . Palgrave Macmillan.

, , & ( ). .

Palgrave Macmillan.)| false , P. (Ed.). (2008). . Routledge.

, (Ed.). ( ). .

Routledge.)| false , C. (1999). Photovoice: A participatory action research strategy applied to women’s health. , 8(2), 185192.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. , & Redwood-Jones Y. (2001). Photovoice ethics: perspectives from flint photovoice. 28, 560572.

, , & ( ). . , – .)| false , N.N. , & McWhirter, J. (1998). Images and curriculum development in health education, in J. Prosser (Ed.), (pp. 263–283). Falmer Press.

, , & , ( ). , in

(Ed.), (pp. 263–283).

Falmer Press.)| false , K. (2014). . NatCen Social Research.

, ( ). . NatCen Social Research.)| false , L. (2009). ‘Snapped’: researching the sexual cultures of schools. using visual methods. , 22(5), 549561.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , Q. (2012). Photographs and stories: ethics, benefits and dilemmas of using participant photography with Black middle-class male youth. , 12(4), 443458.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Weller, S. (2003). “Is it Fun?” Developing Children Centred Research Methods. , 23(1/2), 3358.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Smith, F. (2012). What’s in focus? A critical discussion of photography, children and young people. , 15(2), 91103.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , L. , Talvia, S. , Fossgard, E. , Arnfjörð, U. B. , Hörnell, A. , Ólafsdóttir, A.S. , Gunnarsdóttir, I. , Wergedahl, H. , Lagström, H. , Waling, M. , & Olsson, C. (2017). Nordic children’s conceptualizations of healthy eating in relation to school lunch. , 117(2), 130147.

, , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , P. (2001). Investigating Sociability and Cooperation in Four and Five Year Olds in Reception Class Settings, , 9(1), 2335.

, ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false , M. , & Martinez, S. (2016). Consciousness-raising or unintentionally oppressive? Potential negative consequences of photovoice. , 21(5), 798810.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , D. (2019). Re-imagining research partnerships: Thinking through “co-research” and ethical practice with children and youth. , 13(1), 4058.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , T. , & Hess, E. (2007). What the camera sees and from whose perspective: Fun methodologies for engaging children in enlightening adults. , 14(1), 2945.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2007). Young Travellers and the children’s fund: Some practical notes on an experimental image-based research project. , 7(1), 1622.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , T. , & Curmi-Hall, C. (2019). Transforming School Hallways Through Critical Inquiry: Multimodal Literacies for Civic Engagement. , 63(3), 299309.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , K. (2018). Increasing the Depth of Field: Critical Race Theory and Photovoice as Counter Storytelling Praxis. , 50, 648674.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false , H. (2018). ‘Migrant learners as experts: Exploring their perspectives on inclusion in a primary school in England’. . .

, ( ). ‘ ’. . .)| false , I. , Lewis, I. , & Mumba, P. (2007). Picturing global educational inclusion? Looking and thinking across students’ photographs from the UK, Zambia and Indonesia. , 7(1), 2335.

, , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , Levin, K. , & Inchley, J. (2013). Socio-Environmental influences on physical activity among young people: A qualitative study. , 28(6), 954969.

, , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2009). About face: visual research involving children, , 37(4), 361370.

, ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false , S. (2011). A understanding of a case study: Analysis of a robust qualitative research methodology. , 17(3), 310328.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , C. (2008). Getting the picture and changing the picture: visual methodologies and educational research in South Africa. , 28, 365383.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false , C. , Stuart, J. , Moletsane, R. , & Nkwanyana, C.B. (2006). Why We Don’t Go to School on Fridays: On Youth Participation through Photo Voice in Rural KwaZulu-Natal. , 41(3), 267282.

, , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , M. , Woodcock, A. , & Dunham, P. (2006). ‘Playtime in the Borderlands’: Children’s Representations of School, Gender and Bullying through Photographs and Interviews, , 4(3), 289302.

, , , , & , ( ). , , ( ), – .)| false Donoghue , D. (2007). ‘James always hangs out here’: making space for place in studying masculinities at school. , 22(1), 6273.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , Harm, C. , & Cohen, L. (2019). They’re always there for me! Friendship and meaning in young people’s lives? , 60, 596608.

, , , , & , ( ). , , – .)| false , B. , Alfrey, L. , & Varea, V. (2016). Young Chinese Australians’ subjectivities of ‘health’ and ‘(un)healthy bodies’. , 21(7), 10911108.

, , , , &

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. , & Loxley, A. (2007). Enhancing the contribution of visual methods to inclusive education. , 7(1), 5568.

, , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , J. (2017). Photoyarn: Aboriginal and Mãori girls’ researching contemporary boarding school experiences. , 1, 313.

, ( ). . , , – .)| false T. , Shdaimah, C. , de Tablan, D. , & Sharpe, T. (2016). Exploring wellbeing and agency among urban youth through photovoice. , 67, 114122.

, , , , , & , ( ). . , , – .)| false , M.M.C. (2012). Nostalgia, transition and the school: an innovative approach of using photographic images as a visual method in educational research. , 35(3), 269292.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , H. , & Young, A. (2014). Research with deaf children and not on them: A study of method and process. , 28, 366379.

, , & , ( ). . , , – .)| false , E. (2011). Off to school: A comparative study of schools in the U.S. , 1(2), 4558.

, ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , A. , Bushin, N. , Carpena-Méndez, F. , & Ní Laoire, C. (2010). Using visual methodologies to explore contemporary Irish childhoods. , 10(2), 143158.

, , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false , K. , Pellegrino, A. , Harmon, J. , Ewaida, M. , Bell, A. , Lynch, M. , & Sell, C. (2013). Picturing Culturally Relevant Literacy Practices: Using Photography to See How Literacy Curricula and Pedagogies Matter to Urban Youth. , 15(2), 120.

, , , , , , , , , , , , & , ( ). . , ( ), – .)| false ; ; ; ; ; , All Time Past Year Past 30 Days Abstract Views 221 0 0 Full Text Views 7532 1851 59 PDF Views & Downloads 9357 2127 89

Cover Beijing International Review of Education

Reference Works

Primary source collections

COVID-19 Collection

How to publish with Brill

Open Access Content

Contact & Info

Sales contacts

Publishing contacts

Stay Updated

Newsletters

Social Media Overview

Terms and Conditions  

Privacy Statement  

Cookie Settings  

Accessibility

Legal Notice

Terms and Conditions   |   Privacy Statement   |  Cookie Settings   |   Accessibility   |  Legal Notice   |  Copyright © 2016-2024

Copyright © 2016-2024

Character limit 500 /500

FLEET LIBRARY | Research Guides

Rhode island school of design, photography.

Essential Readings

Contemporary commentary, exploring further.

research essay on photography

Banner

Photography: researching its history, processes, and photographers: Reference Sources and Bibliography

Jan Saudek.  "Life," 1966.  (Olomouc Museum of Art, New York.)   In:   Czech Photography of the 20th Century ,   by Vladimir Birgus and Jan Mlcoch, p. 193.  Prague: KANT, 2010.

Pavel Stecha. From the series "Retirement Home at St. Thomas' in the Lesser Town, Prague," 1972.  (Museum of the Decorative Arts in Prague) In:  Czech Photography of the 20th Century ,  by Vladimir Birgus and Jan Mlcoch, p. 221.  Prague: KANT, 2010.

Richard Avedon. "Nadja Auermann and A Person Unknown, dress by Geoffrey Beene, suit and sweater by Jean Paul Gaultier, Montauk, New York," 1995.  In: Avedon Fashion 1944-2000 , by Carol Squiers and Vince Aletti, p. 398.  NY: Abrams, 2009.

Berenice Abbott. "Wall Street, Showing East River, from Roof of Irving Trust Co. Building," May, 1938.  In: New York in theThirties, as Photographed by Berenice Abbott , text by Elizabeth McCausland, plate 8. NY:  Dover Publications, 1939.

The suburbs

Philip-Lorca diCorcia.  "Oklahoma City," 1999.  In: Philip-Lorca diCorcia , curated by Benett Simpson, p. 69.  Boston:   The Institute of Contemporary Art/Boston, 2007.

Irving Penn.  "Chimney sweep (A), London," 1950.  In: Irving Penn: Small Trades , by Virginia A. Heckert and Anne Lacoste, p. 47. Los Angeles:  The J. Paul Getty Museum, 2009. 

Love and friendship

Grace Robertson. From "Mother's Day Off," 1954.  In: The Oxford Companion to the Photograph , edited by Robin Lenman, p. 540.  NY: Oxford University Press, 2005.

David Miller.  "Lightning from thunderhead at Hall's Creek, Australia," 1995.  In: The Oxford Companion to the Photograph , edited by Robin Lenman, p. 258. NY:  Oxford University Press, 2005.

Dictionaries and encyclopedias

Auer, Michèle, and Michel Auer.    Encyclopédie internationale des photographes de 1839 à nos jours = Photographers encyclopaedia international 1839 to the present .   Hermance, Switzerland : Editions Camera obscura, c1985.

Bibliographical and biographical information on photographers of the 19th and 20th centuries, an alphabetical listing of 1600+ artists from 44 countries.  Vol. II includes a list of photographers by country and a chronological summary of the important events in the history of photography.

Clark Reference NE2600 A1 A84 (Volumes 1-2)

Browne, Turner, and Elaine Partnow.   Macmillan Biographical Encyclopedia of Photographic Artists and Innovators.   New York:  Macmillan; London:  Collier Macmillan, 1983.

Provides basic biographical information on 2,000+ photographers and innovators in the field, including roughly 500 from the 19th and early 20th centuries; in addition, each entry lists publications, portfolios, collections, dealers/representatives, and address.  Photographers were chosen on the basis of “dedication” (photography had to be “a major passion”) and “visibility” (work had to be available in photographic books, journals, museum collections, or through grants and awards, though being well known was not a criterion).  Includes inventors, photographic curators, museum directors, gallery owners, photo-historians, and photography critics in addition to photographers.

Clark Reference  NE2600 A1 B76

Edwards, Gary.   International Guide to Nineteenth-Century Photographers and Their Works: Based on Catalogues of Auction Houses and Dealers.   Boston, Mass: G.K. Hall, 1988.

This unique work "constitutes a census of the [19th-century]  photographic material that has passed through the hands of booksellers, galleries, and auction houses out into the world" during the period of roughly 1970 - 2000, with a few auction catalogs from the 1950s and 1960s.   Organized by photographer, the Guide lists photographic auction catalogs from about 50 major auction houses of America and Britain and a few from Europe (Paris, Munich, Geneva).  An invaluable resource for tracing the sales of work by several hundred 19th-century photographers from around the world.

Clark Reference  NE2609 E38

Hannavy, John, editor.   Encyclopedia of Nineteenth-Century Photography .  New York:  Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group, 2008.

Aims to be “an essential reference work for anyone interested in the medium of photography,” based on scholarly research that has “established the role of many hitherto unrecorded figures, measured the achievements of many of the leading practitioners against contemporary appraisal of their work, and placed the history of photography’s first century within a social and economic context.”  Includes articles on major inventors, manufacturers, organizations, and supporters of the medium as well as key photographers.  Access points are provided by an alphabetical list of entries, a thematic list of entries, and a thorough index.  Each article includes cross-references and bibliographies.

Clark Reference  NE2600 A1 E635  (Volumes 1–2)

*Lenman, Robyn.   The Oxford Companion to the Photograph .   Oxford; New York:  Oxford University Press, 2005.

Includes numerous technical entries (e.g. substantial articles on lens development and three-dimensional photography) as well as articles on photographic theory and techniques.  Contains many national and regional entries not available in other reference books (e.g. Africa and Scandinavia).  Strong in Japanese and Chinese photography.  Strong emphasis on photography as a social and political practice, with articles on tourism, advertising, propaganda, military photography, and photojournalism.  Art photography is also well represented.  Includes a limited number of biographical articles.  Most entries include bibliographies.

Clark Reference  NE2600 A1 O94

Stroebel, Leslie, and Richard Zakia.   The Focal Encyclopedia of Photography, 3rd Edition .  Boston:  Focal Press, 1993.

Especially useful for technical definitions; also includes biographical entries and entries on theory, history, and antiquated processes.  Technical details and definitions are well illustrated with black-and-white line drawings.  Each alphabetical section begins with a list of abbreviations that begin with that letter (e.g. LED: light-emitting diode).

Clark Reference  NE2600 A1 F6

Warren, Lynn, editor.   Encyclopedia of Twentieth-Century Photography.   New York:  Routledge, 2006.

Scholarly discussion of 20 th -century photography history introduces the reader to a history of the technical issues that have changed over the century, defines the concepts, terms, and themes that evolved over 100 years, describes the role of institutions and publications in the shaping of that history, and explains the development of photography in specific countries and regions around the world.  Gathers information on “the most often cited names, terms, concepts, processes, and countries,” and provides the historical and theoretical contexts for understanding each entry.  Includes extensive cross-referencing, substantive bibliographies, analytical index, and 600+ illustrations.

Clark Reference  NE2610 A1 W37 (Volumes 1–3)

Bibliographies

NOTE: check the "Periodical literature" tab, above, for more bibliographies covering periodical literature on photography and photographers.

Boni, Albert, editor.   Photographic Literature, 1960–1970: An International Bibliographic Guide to General & Specialized Literature on Photographic Processes, Techniques, Theory, Chemistry, Physics, Apparatus, Materials & Applications, Industry, History, Biography, Aesthetics, etc.   Hastings-on-Hudson, NY:  Morgan & Morgan, 1972.

Essential reference work that includes publications of all types, dating from 1727; many entries are annotated.  Primarily technical, but includes useful sections on individual photographers, bibliography, history, aesthetics, and illustrated books as well as photographic processes.  Author indexes.

Clark Reference  ZNE2606 B6

Burns, Stanley B.   Early Medical Photography in America (1839–1883) .  NY:  Burns Archive, 1983.

Reprinted from: New York State Journal of Medicine , selected volumes from v. 79, no. 5-v. 81, no. 8.   Burns' monograph, written and published as a seven-part series for the NYSJM , describes 70+ American medical texts.  Texts are categorized into seven levels of illustration, ranging from books with original photographs (e.g. albumens and salt prints) to books with less notable illustrations (e.g. woodcuts copied from photographs); also included are books written by physicians but outside the medical canon.

Clark Stacks  NE2612 B87e

Heidtmann, Frank.   Bibliographie der Photographie Deutschsprachige Publikationen der Jahre 1839-1984 = Bibliography of German-Language Photographic Publications 1839-1984 .  Second revised and enlarged ed.  K.G. Saur:  Munchen, London, NY, Paris, 1989.

"A more or less complete national bibliography of photographic literature," intended as an aid for researchers, journalists, photo historians, collectors of photographs, professional and amateur photographers, and all others interested in photography and its literature.  Organization is a hierarchical classification scheme of five major sections - General, Technology, On Photography (theory and composition), Visual, and Company Publications (e.g. Busch, Canon, Zeiss) - with sub-classifications within these divisions.  Does not list individual periodical articles.  Indexes by personal names, classification terms, and title key words.

Clark Reference  Z NE2606 H45 (Volumes 1-2)

Johnson, William S.   Nineteenth-Century Photography:  An Annotated Bibliography, 1839–1879.   Boston:  G.K. Hall, 1990.

Selected bibliography of literature in English.  Includes references to books and periodical articles about photography from 1839 to 1879, published between 1839 and 1990 in a broad range of general-interest and specialist photographic journals, books, manuals, and pamphlets.  In addition to articles about photography, there are citations to materials that illustrate photographic practice and that are examples of the varied uses of photography in book publishing at that time.  Organization is by broad topic, with an author index.

Clark Reference  Z NE2609 J64

Koelzer, Walter, Editor.   Bibliographie der Photo-und-Film-Zeitschriften, 1840-1940 = Bibliography of photographic and cinematic journals, 1840-1940 = Bibliographie des periodiques photo-cinema, 1840-1940.  Dusseldorf, Foto Brell, 1992.

Alphabetical listing of 2,000+ international periodicals with information on language, country of origin, publication dates, frequency, holding libraries, and sources consulted.  Publications in the UK, US, Belgium, Germany, France, and Russia are well represented; publications of Southeastern Europe, Spain, Portugal, and Latin America less so due to lack of available information.  Annotations when included are trilingual.  Indexes include periodical country of origin, personal and corporate name, sources consulted, and holding library symbols.

Clark Reference  Z NE2606 K64

Newhall, Beaumont.   A Catalogue of the Epstean Collection on the History and Science of Photography: And its Applications Especially to the Graphic Arts: With an Appreciation and Bibliography of Edward Epstean .   Pawlet, VT:  Helios, 1972, c1937.

Catalog of an extraordinary collection, assembled by a leading expert in photomechanical printing.  Classified list of books emphasizes the technical history and practice of photography; also includes sections on bibliography, aesthetics, and periodical literature.  Author and title indexes.   The Clark library copy is part of the David A. Hanson Collection of the History of Photomechanical Reproduction.

Clark Rare Book Room  NE2606 E778c

Palmquist, Peter E.   A Bibliography of Writings By and About Women in Photography 1850–1990 , 2nd Edition .   Arcata, CA:  Published by Peter E. Palmquist, 1994.

Bibliography of articles from periodical sources and books “with at least some relevance to women photographers,” ranging from autobiography to technical treatises and including critical writings.  “At least ninety percent of the entries cannot be found in Readers’ Guide to Periodical Literature or any other traditional indices.”  Entries are arranged chronologically by year and then alphabetically by author.  Name index provides access to 3,000 women photographers and writers.  A selected bibliography serves as a basic starting point to aid researchers in studying “the entire field of women photographers on a global scale.”

Clark Reference  Z NE2606 P345

Photography.  Modern Art Bibliographical Series, volume 2.  Oxford; Santa Barbara, CA:  Clio Press, 1982.

Contains annotated entries from ArtBibliographies Modern for articles on photography published between 1972 and 1979.  Emphasis is on the artistic rather than the scientific side of photography, but much information can be found on cameras, equipment, and the technical development of photography, particularly during the 19th century.  No distinction is made between art photography and industrial, documentary, or applied photography.  A general section conains material on general or worldwide photography, followed by a section arranged alphabetically by country.

Clark Reference  Z NE2610 P36

Roosens, Laurent, and Luc Salu.   His tory of Photography:  A Bibliography of Books.   London; New York:  Mansell, 1989– .

Critically selective yet broadly conceived compilation of 25,000+ entries drawn from books, exhibition catalogs, dissertations, essays, brochures, offprints, and trade literature on photography and related areas (silhouettes, optics, holography, etc.)  Four volumes cover photographers born before 1914, 1936, 1950, and 1962, respectively.  Each volume updates but does not supercede the coverage of the preceding ones. 

Clark Reference  Z NE2606 R66 (Volumes 1–4)

Rowley, Mark.   Photo Illustrated Medical Literature: A Bibliography .  [S.l.]:  Cabinet of Art and Medicine, 2004.

Annotated list, arranged chronologically, of photographically illustrated medical texts.  Excluded are books illustrated by lithographs or woodcuts copied from photographs and photographically illustrated books written by doctors on non-medical subject matter (except for works on photomicrography).  Includes many fascinating illustrations.  See also Burns, above.

Clark Reference  Z NE2606 R684

Rudisill, Richard, et al .  Edited by Peter E. Palmquist.  Photographers:  A Sourcebook for Historical Research , revised 2 nd edition .  Nevada City, CA:  Carl Mautz, 2000.

Combines a selection of six essays on the experience of regional directory research with Richard Rudisill’s “Directories of Photographers: An Annotated World Bibliography.  Rudisill’s annotated bibliography is truly international, providing access to a wide range of general, national, regional, and area-specific histories, directories, and bibliographies; also contains a “works in progress” section that includes the compiler’s address.

Clark Stacks  Z NE2606 P56

Sennett, Robert S.   The Nineteenth-Century Photographic Press: A Study Guide .  NY; London:  Garland Publishing, 1987.

Provides historians and scholars of photography "some access to the wealth of material published in journals between 1840 and the end of the century."  Begins with a brief but useful essay on the birth of and the nature of the photographic press.  Lists in alphabetical order "every European and American journal which could be construed as being relevant to photography;" several have been studied issue by issue for several years and citations made to important contributions.

Clark Reference  Z NE2609 S45n

— — —.    Photography and Photographers to 1900:  An Annotated Bibliography .  NY; London:  Garland Publishing, 1985.

A select bibliography of "those titles which deserve recollection because they are groundbreaking, or particularly influential, or of literary merit."  Does not include periodical literature; focuses on books.  Includes sections on general works on photography, early technical treatises, early theoretical treatises, monographs on photographers, and early books of views and topographical surveys.  Index by subject and author.

Clark Reference  Z NE2609 S46p

Indexes and websites for photographs, photographic collections, and photograph exhibitions

Exhibitions of the Royal Photographic Society 1870-1915

Annual exhibition catalogues of the Photographic Society, London, dating from 1870 to 1915 have been scanned and digitized. Database contains detailed records of all the exhibitions, reproductions of all the catalogue pages, all of the pictures of the photographs that were printed in the catalogues, and reviews of the exhibitions. Visitors can perform a detailed search across all of the catalogues or browse them by date.

Online resource

*International Museum of Photography at George Eastman House. International Photography: George Eastman House Index to Photographers, Collections, and Exhibitions . Edited by Andrew H. Eskind, Greg Drake, Kirsti Ringger, and Lynne Rumney. Enlarged and expanded version of the Index to American photographic collections, 3rd enl. ed. New York: London: G.K. Hall, Prentice Hall International, 1998.

Essential reference work that gives collection and exhibition data on 78,000+  photographers from 615 U.S. and foreign collections. Photographers volume features an alphabetical listing and indicates for each photographer collections that hold work and major exhibitions that included work.   Collections volume gives the address and listing of photographers for each collection.   Exhibitions volume is arranged chronologically with location, dates, and list of photographers exhibited.

Clark Reference  N510 A1 1998

Kreisel, Martha. Photography Books Index: A Subject Guide to Photo Anthologies . Metuchen, N.J: Scarecrow Press, 1980.

Intended to provide librarians, artists, students and the general public with access by subject and by photographer to photographs in published sources; the books indexed are histories and anthologies of photography published between 1946 and 1977.

Clark Stacks  ZNE2606 M6

Parry, Pamela Jeffcott. Photography Index: A Guide to Reproductions . Westport, Conn: Greenwood Press, 1979.

Reference work used to locate reproductions of photographs, including those of documentary journalistic and artistic intent, by both known and anonymous photographers.  Entries are based on illustrations in 80 major books, photo anthologies, surveys, and exhibition catalogs.  Part I is a chronological index to anonymous photographs and Part II is an alphabetical index by photographer. Part III is a subject and title index.

Clark Reference   NE2600 A1 P37

Wall, John.    Directory of British photographic collections .    London: Heinemann, 1977.

A comprehensive directory of "all the photographic collections of note in the British Isles," including  collections  private and commercial, personal or open to public view, published and unpublished, historic and contemporary; in short, "every kind" of photographic collection of note has been included, and every kind of photography "from the discovery of photography to the present day."   Organized by subject, there are photographer, title, subject, location, and owner indexes.

Clark Reference  N1020 A1 W34

We Trust in Human Precision

20,000+ Professional Language Experts Ready to Help. Expertise in a variety of Niches.

API Solutions

Value-Driven Pricing

Unmatched expertise at affordable rates tailored for your needs. Our services empower you to boost your productivity.

PC editors choice

Trusted by Global Leaders

GoTranscript is the chosen service for top media organizations, universities, and Fortune 50 companies.

GoTranscript

One of the Largest Online Transcription and Translation Agencies in the World. Founded in 2005.

Speaker 1: Today we will discuss what is perhaps the most single important element of your research paper, the title. The title is the first thing that journal editors and reviewers see when they look at your paper. It is also the only piece of information that fellow researchers will see in a database or search engine query. Therefore, you want to make sure the title captures all of the relevant aspects of your study but does so in a way that is accessible and captivating to readers. Follow these steps to create a perfect title for your paper. First, ask yourself some questions about what your paper seeks to answer and what it accomplishes. What is my paper about? My paper studies how program volume affects outcomes for liver transplant patients on waiting lists. What methods or techniques did I use to perform my study? I employed a case study. What or who was the subject of my study? I studied 60 liver transplant patients on a waiting list in the U.S. aged 20 to 50 years. What were the results? Positive correlation between waiting list volume and poor outcome of transplant procedure. After answering these questions, move on to the second step, which is to identify and list key words and phrases from these responses. Program volume, outcomes, liver transplant patients, waiting lists, case study, 60 liver transplant patients, age 20 to 50 years, positive correlation. These keywords will form the foundation of your title. Once you have identified and listed these keywords, use them to create one long sentence. This study used a case study of 60 liver transplant patients around the U.S. aged 20 to 50 years to assess how the waiting list volume affects the outcome of liver transplantation in patients. These indicate a positive correlation between increased waiting list volume and a negative prognosis after transplant procedure. Next create a working title. Remove elements that make it a complete sentence, but keep everything that is most important to what the study is about. Delete all unnecessary or redundant words. Now let's shift some words around and rephrase it a bit to shorten the length and make it leaner and yet more natural. What you are left with is a case study of 60 liver transplant patients around the U.S. aged 20 to 50 years assessing the impact of waiting list volume on outcome of a transplantation and showing a positive correlation between increased waiting list volume and negative prognosis. This is getting closer to what we want in a title, which is just the most important information. But note that the word count for this working title is still 38 words, whereas the average published journal article title is 16 words or fewer. Therefore we need to eliminate some words and phrases that are not essential to the title. In step 5 you will delete all extra words and phrases and put key words at the beginning and end of your title. Since the number of patients studied and the exact outcome are not the most essential elements of this paper, remove these elements first. In addition, the methods used in a study are not usually the most searched for keywords in databases and represent additional details that you may want to remove to make your title a little leaner. So we are left with assessing the impact of waiting list volume on outcome and prognosis in liver transplantation patients. In this final version you can immediately see how much clearer the title is and what the study is about and what it aims to achieve. And note that the important terms are written at the beginning and the end of the title. Assessing, which is the main action of the study, is at the beginning and liver transplantation patients, the specific subject of the study, is given at the end. This will help a lot with search engine and database queries, meaning that a lot more researchers will find your article once it is published. And if you want to add a subtitle to give more detail about methodology, you can do this by putting this information after a colon. A case study of US adult patients ages 20 to 25. We abide strictly by our word count rule, this may be unnecessary, but every journal has its own standard formatting and style guidelines for titles, so it's a good idea to be aware of these both while writing your title and writing the study itself. So let's review these steps. First, answer some basic questions about your paper. Next, identify and list keywords and phrases from these responses. Third, turn these keywords into a long sentence. Out of this long sentence you can create a working title, deleting any unnecessary and redundant words. Last, delete any extra elements to meet a suitable word count for your title. Note key terms at the beginning and the end, and again you may add a subtitle if it seems necessary or important to this study. Keep these tips in mind when creating your research paper title. Write the title after you've written your paper in abstract. Include all of your paper's essential terms. Keep it short and to the point, about 16 words or fewer is best. Avoid using jargon or abbreviations that will not be understandable by the general reader. Use keywords that closely relate to the content of your paper, and never use a period at the end of your title. Remember, your title is not a sentence. For more tips on how to improve your writing, visit wordvice.com and check out our resources page where you will find a great many helpful articles and videos. And be sure to subscribe to our YouTube channel and social media pages to stay up to date with more excellent academic writing and journal publications content.

techradar

Research on the Inheritance and Innovation Path of Minority Culture from the Perspective of Rural Revitalization: A Case Study of Nanhua Yi Embroidery Culture

Related Papers

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

Short Wave

Your support helps make our show possible and unlocks access to our sponsor-free feed.

Titanic research dive reveals discovery, decay and new photos

Headshot of Scott Detrow, 2018

Scott Detrow

Avery Keatley

Tinbete Ermyas

Discovery and decay revealed in latest Titanic research dive

New images of the Titanic show part of the railing of the prow has fallen off.

New images of the Titanic show part of the railing of the prow has fallen off. RMS Titanic, Inc. hide caption

More than a century after the Titanic sank, the wreck is still offering up new discoveries to researchers.

This summer, RMS Titanic, Inc. — the salvor-in-possession of the wreck — made its first unmanned dive to the site in 14 years. The expedition was armed with high-resolution cameras and scanning equipment, and took more than two million photos of the Titanic.

The goal was to image the wreck in the highest quality possible, and also to seek out artifacts for future recovery expeditions.

A remarkable new view of the Titanic shipwreck is here, thanks to deep-sea mappers

A remarkable new view of the Titanic shipwreck is here, thanks to deep-sea mappers

James Penca is a researcher with the company, and he joined the nearly month-long expedition earlier this summer. He spoke with All Things Considered host Scott Detrow about the finds — and the losses — that the team discovered.

This interview has been lightly edited for length and clarity.

Interview highlights

Scott Detrow : Your group is the official company that explores and preserves artifacts from the Titanic. And this is not the group's first expedition, it's been down there several times. What was distinct about this particular recent mission?

James Penca: We haven't been to the wreck since 2010. It's been 14 years. So 14 years of technology, as we all know, is quite advanced from what we had back in 2010. What we were able to take down there, the cameras and the scanners, would make 2010’s expedition look like child's play in terms of what we were able to learn from the wreck.

Detrow: What did it feel like? What was that moment like for you the first time that you saw the wreck?

Penca: I have been a hopeless Titanic enthusiast since I was six years old, and it never ceased to amaze me that I was that close to the wreck. Seeing the first images of the ship on a screen, it was emotional. There were a lot of tears in the room for a lot of us, even the people who've been there before.

Detrow: Let's talk about some of the discoveries that were made on this trip. I understand there were some exciting ones and there were some bittersweet ones. Let's start with the exciting.

A virtual recreation of the Diana of Versailles statue on the mantle in a first class lounge.

A virtual recreation of the Diana of Versailles statue on the mantle in a first class lounge. Titanic: Honor and Glory hide caption

Penca: On this expedition, we weren't recovering, but we were looking out for artifacts for future recovery. And there was one artifact at the top of our list. It was a bronze statue about two feet tall, known as the Diana of Versailles. This statue sat in the first class lounge aboard Titanic, and this statue was the centerpiece of that elegant room. The trouble with this artifact is, it has been photographed only one time, in 1986. So, to find this statue to us would be one of the great achievements of this expedition, but the odds of finding it, it's truly a needle in a haystack that is two-and-a-half miles underwater in pitch black darkness. After many trials and errors, we did manage to find Diana and take her first photos in 38 years, and we found her with just hours remaining in the expedition.

A new photo of the Diana of Versailles statue aboard the Titanic.

A new photo of the Diana of Versailles statue aboard the Titanic. RMS Titanic, Inc hide caption

Detrow: So on one hand, high profile aspects of the wreck have been found and remain shockingly the same. But on the other hand, there was some evidence you gathered that some of the iconic aspects of the ship may be collapsing.

Penca: Yeah, so everyone knows the famous James Cameron film, the “I'm flying,” scene, you know, “I’m the king of the world.” [That] scene all took place on Titanic's iconic prow at the very, very point of the ship. We discovered when we reached the wreck last month that the port side railing has fallen off. This is a very natural thing, different parts of the railing are getting weaker. But for such a high-profile and highly photographed and iconic railing to now be resting on the ocean floor below, it's a significant change. Titanic will literally never look the same.

U.S. Coast Guard marine safety engineers inspect the aft titanium endcap from the Titan submersible on a boat.

A year after the Titan submersible implosion, investigators still don't have answers

Detrow: There have been many shipwrecks throughout history. There have been many high-profile tragedies. There have been many of those things that have become big blockbuster movies. But there's something different about Titanic. What do you think it is that attracts people's imaginations in this way, yours or others?

Penca: You know, we are storytellers. We love a good story. And I would put Titanic's life and loss up against any great story, any Hollywood film, any great novel, a war story. Every single element of it lined up as if it was written for a Hollywood film. It is truly too good to be true. But, the more you dive into Titanic, the more true it becomes.

Beyond Peers: Cross-Industry Competition  and Strategic Financing

Swiss Finance Institute Research Paper No. 24-45

62 Pages Posted:

Boris Nikolov

University of Lausanne; Swiss Finance Institute; European Corporate Governance Institute (ECGI)

Norman Schuerhoff

Swiss Finance Institute - HEC Lausanne

Zepeng Wang

University of Lausanne

Date Written: September 01, 2024

Corporate financial leverage within competition networks is determined by both direct and  indirect competitors. Using data on firms’ self reported competitors, we identify eleven  stable competition communities within the U.S. economy, where firms are grouped into  communities based on competitive interactions both within and across industries. We find  a strong complementarity between a firm’s leverage and that of its community members,  consistent with strategic interactions with both immediate peers and chain effects from  the propagation of shocks affecting indirect peers. To achieve identification, we employ a  granular instrumental variable approach. Our results highlight that firms’ financial strategies  are shaped not only by direct competition but also by the broader competitive environment.

Keywords: capital structure, strategic competition, financial complementarity, competitor networks

JEL Classification: G31, G32, L13

Suggested Citation: Suggested Citation

Boris Nikolov (Contact Author)

University of lausanne ( email ).

Lausanne, CH-1015 Switzerland

Swiss Finance Institute ( email )

c/o University of Geneva 40, Bd du Pont-d'Arve CH-1211 Geneva 4 Switzerland

European Corporate Governance Institute (ECGI) ( email )

c/o the Royal Academies of Belgium Rue Ducale 1 Hertogsstraat 1000 Brussels Belgium

Swiss Finance Institute - HEC Lausanne ( email )

Chavannes-près-Renens Switzerland

Do you have a job opening that you would like to promote on SSRN?

Paper statistics, related ejournals, swiss finance institute research paper series.

Subscribe to this free journal for more curated articles on this topic

Corporate Governance & Finance eJournal

Subscribe to this fee journal for more curated articles on this topic

Information

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

applsci-logo

Article Menu

research essay on photography

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Influence of additives on grinding performance of digital light processing-printed phenol bond grinding wheels.

research essay on photography

1. Introduction

2. materials and methods, 2.1. materials, 2.2. printing and post-processing process, 2.3. grinding process, 2.4. measurement, 3.1. mechanical properties of grinding wheels, 3.2. grinding experiment, 3.2.1. dressing operation, 3.2.2. grinding forces, 3.2.3. real depth of cut and tool wear, 3.2.4. surface roughness, 3.2.5. surface morphology and microtopography, 4. conclusions.

Author Contributions

Institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

Click here to enlarge figure

MaterialParticle Size [μm]Fiber Length [mm]Fiber Diameter [nm]
Silicon carbide (SiC)60–70__
Glass fiber (GF)_12100
Carbon fiber (CF)_3100
Copper (Cu)40__
Additive-Manufactured Grinding Wheels
Grinding Wheel No.Equal as Tensile SamplePhenol Resin w%Abrasives w% Additives w%
GW-1TS-158.8%SiC 41.2%_
GW-2TS-258.1%SiC 40.7%GF 1.2%
GW-3TS-358.5%SiC 40.9%CF 0.6%
GW-4TS-455.6%SiC 38.9%Cu 5.5%
Tensile Samples
Sample No.Phenol Resin w%Abrasives w% Additives w%
TS-0100%__
TS-158.8%SiC 41.2%_
TS-258.1%SiC 40.7%GF 1.2%
TS-358.5%SiC 40.9%CF 0.6%
TS-455.6%SiC 38.9Cu 5.5%
Parameter Set 1
Grinding wheelGW-1; GW-2; GW-3; GW-4;Workpiece materialAluminium 7075
Grinding parametersV = 30 m/s, V = 6000 mm/min, a = 10 µm
Dressing and sharpening parameterDressing: a = 4 µm, V = 10 m/s, U = 9CoolantEmulsion
Grinding wheelGW-1; GW-2; GW-3; GW-4;Workpiece materialAluminium 7075
Grinding parametersV = 30 m/s, V = 1200 mm/min, a = 50 µm
Dressing and sharpening parameterDressing: a = 4 µm, V = 10 m/s, U = 9CoolantEmulsion
Grinding wheelGW-1; GW-2; GW-3; GW-4;Workpiece materialAluminium 7075
Grinding parametersV = 30 m/s, V = 600 mm/min, a = 100 µm
Dressing and sharpening parameterDressing: a = 4 µm, V = 10 m/s, U = 9CoolantEmulsion
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Habel, A.; Barmouz, M.; Steinhäuser, F.; Azarhoushang, B. Influence of Additives on Grinding Performance of Digital Light Processing-Printed Phenol Bond Grinding Wheels. Appl. Sci. 2024 , 14 , 7711. https://doi.org/10.3390/app14177711

Habel A, Barmouz M, Steinhäuser F, Azarhoushang B. Influence of Additives on Grinding Performance of Digital Light Processing-Printed Phenol Bond Grinding Wheels. Applied Sciences . 2024; 14(17):7711. https://doi.org/10.3390/app14177711

Habel, Ammar, Mohsen Barmouz, Felix Steinhäuser, and Bahman Azarhoushang. 2024. "Influence of Additives on Grinding Performance of Digital Light Processing-Printed Phenol Bond Grinding Wheels" Applied Sciences 14, no. 17: 7711. https://doi.org/10.3390/app14177711

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

IMAGES

  1. Photography Research Paper

    research essay on photography

  2. Photographic Essay

    research essay on photography

  3. Photography Essay

    research essay on photography

  4. Photography Essay

    research essay on photography

  5. Essay photography

    research essay on photography

  6. Photo Essay

    research essay on photography

VIDEO

  1. Photo Essay Ep.1: Yosemite on 35mm Film (Portra 400)

  2. Nikon DSLR Camera Photography #youtubeshorts #romantic

  3. Short Essay on World Photography day

  4. Workshop 5: Strategies for a Strong Research Essay

  5. ART IS NOT FOR SALE

  6. Daily Dose of Nature

COMMENTS

  1. Hunters and Gatherers of Pictures: Why Photography Has Become a Human

    Introduction. Photography is ubiquitous around the world, with the number of people taking and using personal photographs steadily increasing (Lee and Stewart, 2016; Canon, 2018).More than 90 percent of all photographs (henceforth photos) are taken with smartphones (Carrington, 2020), and more than half of the world's population uses smartphones or mobile phones to take, view, and share photos ...

  2. Photography and Its Origins

    Edited by Tanya Sheehan and Andrés Mario Zervigón, this collection of 16 original essays, illustrated with 32 color images, showcases prominent and emerging voices in the field of photography studies. Their research cuts across disciplines and methodologies, shedding new light on old questions about histories and their writing.

  3. (PDF) A Brief History of Light & Photography

    The research includes a brief historical introduction to the progress of photography (diachronic approach) to study the (co)relationship between the development of photographic science and ...

  4. Social Theory, Photography and the Visual Aesthetic of Cultural

    Abstract. Social theory and photographic aesthetics both engage with issues of representation, realism and validity, having crossed paths in theoretical and methodological controversies. This discussion begins with reflections on the realism debate in photography, arguing that beyond the polar positions of realism and constructivism the ...

  5. (PDF) Photography: The Key Concepts

    PDF | On Sep 25, 2020, David Bate published Photography: The Key Concepts | Find, read and cite all the research you need on ResearchGate

  6. Full article: "Looking with intention": using photographic essays as

    For most of our students, writing a photographic essay on their Berlin fieldwork was a first time experience with using visual data as a research method, confirming Sanders (Citation 2007) observation that the pedagogical contributions of photography are still overlooked in geography. The assignment made our students enthusiastic, but also ...

  7. (PDF) Photography, Literature, and Writing: An Interdisciplinary

    This revelation of photography and literature, while addressing effectively photography and finding meanings hidden behind it refers the problems arise by the somewhat equivalent status of to the deceiving nature of co mmod ity and exchange value image and reality through such practise. as an attempt to hide the real production relation as Now ...

  8. Full article: INTRODUCTION: PHOTOGRAPHY AND LANDSCAPE

    The essay contributed by Danièle Méaux continues the theme of ordinary landscapes and the role of photography in bringing them to our attention. Méaux discusses such projects using works by Thierry Girard, Alain Ceccaroli and Laurent Gueneau, among others, as examples of "vernacular landscapes" that were "discovered" in the 1980s by ...

  9. The visual vernacular: embracing photographs in research

    A Qualitative Space highlights research approaches that push readers and scholars deeper into qualitative methods and methodologies. Contributors to A Qualitative Space may: advance new ideas about qualitative methodologies, methods, and/or techniques; debate current and historical trends in qualitative research; craft and share nuanced reflections on how data collection methods should be ...

  10. History of Photography

    Included in Thomson Reuters Arts and Humanities Citation Index® Founded in 1977, History of Photography is an international quarterly journal devoted to the history, practice and theory of photography in all its definitions - artistic, scientific, commercial, and domestic; in archives, art galleries, and museums; on the page, on the wall, or on the screen.

  11. PDF How Taking Photos Increases Enjoyment of Experiences

    One such factor is photo-taking, which has gone unexamined by prior research even as it has become ubiquitous. We identify engagement as a relevant process that influences whether photo-taking will increase or decrease enjoyment. Across 3 field and 6 lab experiments, we find that taking photos enhances enjoyment of positive experiences across a ...

  12. photography as a research method

    Abstract. This chapter discusses the development of photography as a research method in the social sciences. It describes the different types of photographs used, such as archival photographs and photographs taken by the researcher, and it focuses especially on photographs taken by participants. The uses of different approaches to obtain ...

  13. Photography as an Art-Based Research Method

    Photo-narratives form a storytelling approach to photography . Stories are distilled into one-off images or image series of multilayered information (Bright, 2015).Photo-narratives share a common ground with narrative inquiry, which is a type of qualitative inquiry, in that both focus on retrospective meaning (Chase, 2005).The term story forms an important component of narrative inquiry, as it ...

  14. Visual Methodologies in Qualitative Research: Autophotography and Photo

    Visual methodologies are used to understand and interpret images (Barbour, 2014) and include photography, film, video, painting, drawing, collage, sculpture, artwork, graffiti, advertising, and cartoons.Visual methodologies are a new and novel approach to qualitative research derived from traditional ethnography methods used in anthropology and sociology.

  15. (PDF) Photography in Social Science Research

    This paper aims to add to the analysis of the potentialities, limitations and challenges of the use of photography in social sciences research. For this purpose, the paper. study based on their ...

  16. PDF Moving toward Visual Literacy: Photography as a Language of ...

    municate emerging understandings with others and with self (p. 8). In other words, developing visual literacy through the language o. photography is a part of current initiatives on teaching inquiry.The study of photographs (and video) of children's learning and classroom experiences is fast becoming central to the work of many new and ...

  17. PDF On Photography

    On Photography iii. eForeword One of the most highly regarded books of its kind, On Photography first appeared in 1977 and is described by its author as "a progress of essays about the meaning and career of photographs." It begins with the famous "In Plato's Cave" essay, then offers five other

  18. Photography as a Research Method with Learners in Compulsory ...

    Abstract This article offers a review of thirty-one research articles from 2001-2019 on the use of photography as a research method with learners in compulsory education. Understood within the scope of 'visual', 'participatory' and 'arts-based' research methods, many scholars have linked the increased use of the photographic method to greater awareness of the rights of the child ...

  19. Research Guides: Photography: Critical Thinking & Theory

    One of the most highly regarded books of its kind, "On Photography" first appeared in 1977 and is described by its author as "a progress of essays about the meaning and career of photographs." It begins with the famous " In Plato' s Cave" essay, then offers five other prose meditations on this topic, and concludes with a fascinating and far ...

  20. Reference Sources and Bibliography

    Critically selective yet broadly conceived compilation of 25,000+ entries drawn from books, exhibition catalogs, dissertations, essays, brochures, offprints, and trade literature on photography and related areas (silhouettes, optics, holography, etc.) Four volumes cover photographers born before 1914, 1936, 1950, and 1962, respectively.

  21. Full article: The Memory of Photography

    Some may detect in the title of this paper an ambiguity between the memory of photography as a technology that is "outmoded", past its sell-by date, and photography as an "aide-memoire" machine to the human memory process of remembering. As "photography" mutates into its digital double (like a sister or brother) whose system operates in a different way and is still in the process ...

  22. Mastering Research Questions: A Guide to Focused and ...

    It's a clear, focused, concise, complex, and arguable question around which you center your research. Or it's just a thing that's meant to frustrate and confuse students throughout college writing. But hopefully after this video, research questions will be a little less confusing and frustrating. So why do I need a research question?

  23. Photography as a Research Method

    Abstract. This chapter discusses the development of photography as a research method in social sciences. It describes the different types of photographs used, such as archival photographs and photographs taken by the researcher, and it focuses especially on photographs taken by participants. The uses of different approaches to obtain ...

  24. Crafting Effective Introductions for Research Papers: Essential Tips

    Speaker 1: After the title page and abstract, the reader's first true interaction with your research paper is the introduction. Your introduction will establish the foundation upon which your readers approach your work, and if you use the tips we discuss in this video, these readers should be able to logically apply the rules set in your introduction to all parts of your paper, all the way ...

  25. Creating Effective Research Paper Titles: A Step-by-Step Guide for

    Keep these tips in mind when creating your research paper title. Write the title after you've written your paper in abstract. Include all of your paper's essential terms. Keep it short and to the point, about 16 words or fewer is best. Avoid using jargon or abbreviations that will not be understandable by the general reader.

  26. Research on the Inheritance and Innovation Path of Minority Culture

    As one of the traditional Chinese crafts, Yi embroidery carries rich cultural connotation and historical value. In recent years, with the implementation of rural revitalization strategy, the protection and inheritance of Yi embroidery culture has become an urgent problem to be solved. Through field research and literature research, this paper takes Nanhua County as an example to explore the ...

  27. Titanic research dive reveals discovery, decay and new photos

    This summer, RMS Titanic, Inc. — the salvor-in-possession of the wreck — made its first unmanned dive to the wreck in 14 years. The team uncovered some rare finds — and losses.

  28. Beyond Peers: Cross-Industry Competition and Strategic Financing

    Corporate financial leverage within competition networks is determined by both direct and indirect competitors. Using data on firms' self reported competitors, we identify eleven stable competition communities within the U.S. economy, where firms are grouped into communities based on competitive interactions both within and across industries. We find a strong complementarity between a firm ...

  29. Photography and Culture

    Photography & Culture is a peer-reviewed quarterly journal aimed at advancing the study of photography through direct considerations of its reciprocal relationship with culturally defined environments, conventions, heritage and politics. The journal embraces the prevalence of changing definitions for both "photography" and "culture", encouraging the development of knowledge about the ...

  30. Influence of Additives on Grinding Performance of Digital Light ...

    Resin bond grinding wheels are the most common grinding tools in the industry. Until now, all research on the additive manufacturing of resin bond grinding wheels has focused on commercially available acrylate resin. However, using a phenol-based bond to print resin-bond grinding wheels has been challenging for researchers and industries. In this study, a photo-curable phenol resin bond ...