6.2 Classical Conditioning

Learning objectives.

By the end of this section, you will be able to:

  • Explain how classical conditioning occurs
  • Summarize the processes of acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery, generalization, and discrimination

Does the name Ivan Pavlov ring a bell? Even if you are new to the study of psychology, chances are that you have heard of Pavlov and his famous dogs.

Pavlov (1849–1936), a Russian scientist, performed extensive research on dogs and is best known for his experiments in classical conditioning ( Figure 6.3 ). As we discussed briefly in the previous section, classical conditioning is a process by which we learn to associate stimuli and, consequently, to anticipate events.

Pavlov came to his conclusions about how learning occurs completely by accident. Pavlov was a physiologist, not a psychologist. Physiologists study the life processes of organisms, from the molecular level to the level of cells, organ systems, and entire organisms. Pavlov’s area of interest was the digestive system (Hunt, 2007). In his studies with dogs, Pavlov measured the amount of saliva produced in response to various foods. Over time, Pavlov (1927) observed that the dogs began to salivate not only at the taste of food, but also at the sight of food, at the sight of an empty food bowl, and even at the sound of the laboratory assistants' footsteps. Salivating to food in the mouth is reflexive, so no learning is involved. However, dogs don’t naturally salivate at the sight of an empty bowl or the sound of footsteps.

These unusual responses intrigued Pavlov, and he wondered what accounted for what he called the dogs' “psychic secretions” (Pavlov, 1927). To explore this phenomenon in an objective manner, Pavlov designed a series of carefully controlled experiments to see which stimuli would cause the dogs to salivate. He was able to train the dogs to salivate in response to stimuli that clearly had nothing to do with food, such as the sound of a bell, a light, and a touch on the leg. Through his experiments, Pavlov realized that an organism has two types of responses to its environment: (1) unconditioned (unlearned) responses, or reflexes, and (2) conditioned (learned) responses.

In Pavlov’s experiments, the dogs salivated each time meat powder was presented to them. The meat powder in this situation was an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) : a stimulus that elicits a reflexive response in an organism. The dogs’ salivation was an unconditioned response (UCR) : a natural (unlearned) reaction to a given stimulus. Before conditioning, think of the dogs’ stimulus and response like this:

In classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus is presented immediately before an unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov would sound a tone (like ringing a bell) and then give the dogs the meat powder ( Figure 6.4 ). The tone was the neutral stimulus (NS) , which is a stimulus that does not naturally elicit a response. Prior to conditioning, the dogs did not salivate when they just heard the tone because the tone had no association for the dogs.

When Pavlov paired the tone with the meat powder over and over again, the previously neutral stimulus (the tone) also began to elicit salivation from the dogs. Thus, the neutral stimulus became the conditioned stimulus (CS) , which is a stimulus that elicits a response after repeatedly being paired with an unconditioned stimulus. Eventually, the dogs began to salivate to the tone alone, just as they previously had salivated at the sound of the assistants’ footsteps. The behavior caused by the conditioned stimulus is called the conditioned response (CR) . In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, they had learned to associate the tone (CS) with being fed, and they began to salivate (CR) in anticipation of food.

Link to Learning

View this video about Pavlov and his dogs to learn more.

Real World Application of Classical Conditioning

How does classical conditioning work in the real world? Consider the case of Moisha, who was diagnosed with cancer. When she received her first chemotherapy treatment, she vomited shortly after the chemicals were injected. In fact, every trip to the doctor for chemotherapy treatment shortly after the drugs were injected, she vomited. Moisha’s treatment was a success and her cancer went into remission. Now, when she visits her oncologist's office every 6 months for a check-up, she becomes nauseous. In this case, the chemotherapy drugs are the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), vomiting is the unconditioned response (UCR), the doctor’s office is the conditioned stimulus (CS) after being paired with the UCS, and nausea is the conditioned response (CR). Let's assume that the chemotherapy drugs that Moisha takes are given through a syringe injection. After entering the doctor's office, Moisha sees a syringe, and then gets her medication. In addition to the doctor's office, Moisha will learn to associate the syringe with the medication and will respond to syringes with nausea. This is an example of higher-order (or second-order) conditioning, when the conditioned stimulus (the doctor's office) serves to condition another stimulus (the syringe). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if someone rang a bell every time Moisha received a syringe injection of chemotherapy drugs in the doctor's office, Moisha likely will never get sick in response to the bell.

Consider another example of classical conditioning. Let’s say you have a cat named Tiger, who is quite spoiled. You keep her food in a separate cabinet, and you also have a special electric can opener that you use only to open cans of cat food. For every meal, Tiger hears the distinctive sound of the electric can opener (“zzhzhz”) and then gets her food. Tiger quickly learns that when she hears “zzhzhz” she is about to get fed. What do you think Tiger does when she hears the electric can opener? She will likely get excited and run to where you are preparing her food. This is an example of classical conditioning. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR?

What if the cabinet holding Tiger’s food becomes squeaky? In that case, Tiger hears “squeak” (the cabinet), “zzhzhz” (the electric can opener), and then she gets her food. Tiger will learn to get excited when she hears the “squeak” of the cabinet. Pairing a new neutral stimulus (“squeak”) with the conditioned stimulus (“zzhzhz”) is called higher-order conditioning , or second-order conditioning . This means you are using the conditioned stimulus of the can opener to condition another stimulus: the squeaky cabinet ( Figure 6.5 ). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if you ring a bell, open the cabinet (“squeak”), use the can opener (“zzhzhz”), and then feed Tiger, Tiger will likely never get excited when hearing the bell alone.

Everyday Connection

Classical conditioning at stingray city.

Kate and her spouse recently vacationed in the Cayman Islands, and booked a boat tour to Stingray City, where they could feed and swim with the southern stingrays. The boat captain explained how the normally solitary stingrays have become accustomed to interacting with humans. About 40 years ago, people began to clean fish and conch (unconditioned stimulus) at a particular sandbar near a barrier reef, and large numbers of stingrays would swim in to eat (unconditioned response) what the people threw into the water; this continued for years. By the late 1980s, word of the large group of stingrays spread among scuba divers, who then started feeding them by hand. Over time, the southern stingrays in the area were classically conditioned much like Pavlov’s dogs. When they hear the sound of a boat engine (neutral stimulus that becomes a conditioned stimulus), they know that they will get to eat (conditioned response).

As soon as they reached Stingray City, over two dozen stingrays surrounded their tour boat. The couple slipped into the water with bags of squid, the stingrays’ favorite treat. The swarm of stingrays bumped and rubbed up against their legs like hungry cats ( Figure 6.6 ). Kate was able to feed, pet, and even kiss (for luck) these amazing creatures. Then all the squid was gone, and so were the stingrays.

Classical conditioning also applies to humans, even babies. For example, Elan buys formula in blue canisters for their six-month-old daughter, Angelina. Whenever Elan takes out a formula container, Angelina gets excited, tries to reach toward the food, and most likely salivates. Why does Angelina get excited when she sees the formula canister? What are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR here?

So far, all of the examples have involved food, but classical conditioning extends beyond the basic need to be fed. Consider our earlier example of a dog whose owners install an invisible electric dog fence. A small electrical shock (unconditioned stimulus) elicits discomfort (unconditioned response). When the unconditioned stimulus (shock) is paired with a neutral stimulus (the edge of a yard), the dog associates the discomfort (unconditioned response) with the edge of the yard (conditioned stimulus) and stays within the set boundaries. In this example, the edge of the yard elicits fear and anxiety in the dog. Fear and anxiety are the conditioned response.

Watch this video clip from the television show, The Office , for a humorous look at conditioning in which Jim conditions Dwight to expect a breath mint every time Jim’s computer makes a specific sound.

General Processes in Classical Conditioning

Now that you know how classical conditioning works and have seen several examples, let’s take a look at some of the general processes involved. In classical conditioning, the initial period of learning is known as acquisition , when an organism learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus. During acquisition, the neutral stimulus begins to elicit the conditioned response, and eventually the neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus capable of eliciting the conditioned response by itself. Timing is important for conditioning to occur. Typically, there should only be a brief interval between presentation of the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus. Depending on what is being conditioned, sometimes this interval is as little as five seconds (Chance, 2009). However, with other types of conditioning, the interval can be up to several hours.

Taste aversion is a type of conditioning in which an interval of several hours may pass between the conditioned stimulus (something ingested) and the unconditioned stimulus (nausea or illness). Here’s an example. Harry went to the carnival. He ate a lot of cotton candy and later that night was very sick and threw up. The next day, his friend offered him a piece of candy. He put it into his mouth and started to feel sick and had to spit it out. The unconditioned stimulus is eating too much cotton candy. The unconditioned response is getting sick and throwing up. The conditioned stimulus is the sugary flavor and the conditioned response is Harry feeling nauseous at the taste of sugar.

How does this occur—conditioning based on a single instance and involving an extended time lapse between the event and the negative stimulus? Research into taste aversion suggests that this response may be an evolutionary adaptation designed to help organisms quickly learn to avoid harmful foods (Garcia & Rusiniak, 1980; Garcia & Koelling, 1966). Not only may this contribute to species survival via natural selection, but it may also help us develop strategies for challenges such as helping cancer patients through the nausea induced by certain treatments (Holmes, 1993; Jacobsen et al., 1993; Hutton, Baracos, & Wismer, 2007; Skolin et al., 2006). Garcia and Koelling (1966) showed not only that taste aversions could be conditioned, but also that there were biological constraints to learning. In their study, separate groups of rats were conditioned to associate either a flavor with illness, or lights and sounds with illness. Results showed that all rats exposed to flavor-illness pairings learned to avoid the flavor, but none of the rats exposed to lights and sounds with illness learned to avoid lights or sounds. This added evidence to the idea that classical conditioning could contribute to species survival by helping organisms learn to avoid stimuli that posed real dangers to health and welfare.

Robert Rescorla demonstrated how powerfully an organism can learn to predict the UCS from the CS. Take, for example, the following two situations. Ari’s dad always has dinner on the table every day at 6:00. Soraya’s mom switches it up so that some days they eat dinner at 6:00, some days they eat at 5:00, and other days they eat at 7:00. For Ari, 6:00 reliably and consistently predicts dinner, so Ari will likely start feeling hungry every day right before 6:00, even if he's had a late snack. Soraya, on the other hand, will be less likely to associate 6:00 with dinner, since 6:00 does not always predict that dinner is coming. Rescorla, along with his colleague at Yale University, Allan Wagner, developed a mathematical formula that could be used to calculate the probability that an association would be learned given the ability of a conditioned stimulus to predict the occurrence of an unconditioned stimulus and other factors; today this is known as the Rescorla-Wagner model (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972)

Once we have established the connection between the unconditioned stimulus and the conditioned stimulus, how do we break that connection and get the dog, cat, or child to stop responding? In Tiger’s case, imagine what would happen if you stopped using the electric can opener for her food and began to use it only for human food. Now, Tiger would hear the can opener, but she would not get food. In classical conditioning terms, you would be giving the conditioned stimulus, but not the unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov explored this scenario in his experiments with dogs: sounding the tone without giving the dogs the meat powder. Soon the dogs stopped responding to the tone. Extinction is the decrease in the conditioned response when the unconditioned stimulus is no longer presented with the conditioned stimulus. When presented with the conditioned stimulus alone, the dog, cat, or other organism would show a weaker and weaker response, and finally no response. In classical conditioning terms, there is a gradual weakening and disappearance of the conditioned response.

What happens when learning is not used for a while—when what was learned lies dormant? As we just discussed, Pavlov found that when he repeatedly presented the bell (conditioned stimulus) without the meat powder (unconditioned stimulus), extinction occurred; the dogs stopped salivating to the bell. However, after a couple of hours of resting from this extinction training, the dogs again began to salivate when Pavlov rang the bell. What do you think would happen with Tiger’s behavior if your electric can opener broke, and you did not use it for several months? When you finally got it fixed and started using it to open Tiger’s food again, Tiger would remember the association between the can opener and her food—she would get excited and run to the kitchen when she heard the sound. The behavior of Pavlov’s dogs and Tiger illustrates a concept Pavlov called spontaneous recovery : the return of a previously extinguished conditioned response following a rest period ( Figure 6.7 ).

Of course, these processes also apply in humans. For example, let’s say that every day when you walk to campus, an ice cream truck passes your route. Day after day, you hear the truck’s music (neutral stimulus), so you finally stop and purchase a chocolate ice cream bar. You take a bite (unconditioned stimulus) and then your mouth waters (unconditioned response). This initial period of learning is known as acquisition, when you begin to connect the neutral stimulus (the sound of the truck) and the unconditioned stimulus (the taste of the chocolate ice cream in your mouth). During acquisition, the conditioned response gets stronger and stronger through repeated pairings of the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. Several days (and ice cream bars) later, you notice that your mouth begins to water (conditioned response) as soon as you hear the truck’s musical jingle—even before you bite into the ice cream bar. Then one day you head down the street. You hear the truck’s music (conditioned stimulus), and your mouth waters (conditioned response). However, when you get to the truck, you discover that they are all out of ice cream. You leave disappointed. The next few days you pass by the truck and hear the music, but don’t stop to get an ice cream bar because you’re running late for class. You begin to salivate less and less when you hear the music, until by the end of the week, your mouth no longer waters when you hear the tune. This illustrates extinction. The conditioned response weakens when only the conditioned stimulus (the sound of the truck) is presented, without being followed by the unconditioned stimulus (chocolate ice cream in the mouth). Then the weekend comes. You don’t have to go to class, so you don’t pass the truck. Monday morning arrives and you take your usual route to campus. You round the corner and hear the truck again. What do you think happens? Your mouth begins to water again. Why? After a break from conditioning, the conditioned response reappears, which indicates spontaneous recovery.

Acquisition and extinction involve the strengthening and weakening, respectively, of a learned association. Two other learning processes—stimulus discrimination and stimulus generalization—are involved in determining which stimuli will trigger learned responses. Animals (including humans) need to distinguish between stimuli—for example, between sounds that predict a threatening event and sounds that do not—so that they can respond appropriately (such as running away if the sound is threatening). When an organism learns to respond differently to various stimuli that are similar, it is called stimulus discrimination . In classical conditioning terms, the organism demonstrates the conditioned response only to the conditioned stimulus. Pavlov’s dogs discriminated between the basic tone that sounded before they were fed and other tones (e.g., the doorbell), because the other sounds did not predict the arrival of food. Similarly, Tiger, the cat, discriminated between the sound of the can opener and the sound of the electric mixer. When the electric mixer is going, Tiger is not about to be fed, so she does not come running to the kitchen looking for food. In our other example, Moisha, the cancer patient, discriminated between oncologists and other types of doctors. She learned not to feel ill when visiting doctors for other types of appointments, such as her annual physical.

On the other hand, when an organism demonstrates the conditioned response to stimuli that are similar to the condition stimulus, it is called stimulus generalization , the opposite of stimulus discrimination. The more similar a stimulus is to the condition stimulus, the more likely the organism is to give the conditioned response. For instance, if the electric mixer sounds very similar to the electric can opener, Tiger may come running after hearing its sound. But if you do not feed her following the electric mixer sound, and you continue to feed her consistently after the electric can opener sound, she will quickly learn to discriminate between the two sounds (provided they are sufficiently dissimilar that she can tell them apart). In our other example, Moisha continued to feel ill whenever visiting other oncologists or other doctors in the same building as her oncologist.

Behaviorism

John B. Watson , shown in Figure 6.8 , is considered the founder of behaviorism. Behaviorism is a school of thought that arose during the first part of the 20th century, which incorporates elements of Pavlov’s classical conditioning (Hunt, 2007). In stark contrast with Freud, who considered the reasons for behavior to be hidden in the unconscious, Watson championed the idea that all behavior can be studied as a simple stimulus-response reaction, without regard for internal processes. Watson argued that in order for psychology to become a legitimate science, it must shift its concern away from internal mental processes because mental processes cannot be seen or measured. Instead, he asserted that psychology must focus on outward observable behavior that can be measured.

Watson’s ideas were influenced by Pavlov’s work. According to Watson, human behavior, just like animal behavior, is primarily the result of conditioned responses. Whereas Pavlov’s work with dogs involved the conditioning of reflexes, Watson believed the same principles could be extended to the conditioning of human emotions (Watson, 1919).

In 1920, while chair of the psychology department at Johns Hopkins University, Watson and his graduate student, Rosalie Rayner, conducted research on a baby nicknamed Little Albert. Rayner and Watson’s experiments with Little Albert demonstrated how fears can be conditioned using classical conditioning. Through these experiments, Little Albert was exposed to and conditioned to fear certain things. Initially he was presented with various neutral stimuli, including a rabbit, a dog, a monkey, masks, cotton wool, and a white rat. He was not afraid of any of these things. Then Watson, with the help of Rayner, conditioned Little Albert to associate these stimuli with an emotion—fear. For example, Watson handed Little Albert the white rat, and Little Albert enjoyed playing with it. Then Watson made a loud sound, by striking a hammer against a metal bar hanging behind Little Albert’s head, each time Little Albert touched the rat. Little Albert was frightened by the sound—demonstrating a reflexive fear of sudden loud noises—and began to cry. Watson repeatedly paired the loud sound with the white rat. Soon Little Albert became frightened by the white rat alone. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR? Days later, Little Albert demonstrated stimulus generalization—he became afraid of other furry things: a rabbit, a furry coat, and even a Santa Claus mask ( Figure 6.9 ). Watson had succeeded in conditioning a fear response in Little Albert, thus demonstrating that emotions could become conditioned responses. It had been Watson’s intention to produce a phobia—a persistent, excessive fear of a specific object or situation— through conditioning alone, thus countering Freud’s view that phobias are caused by deep, hidden conflicts in the mind. However, there is no evidence that Little Albert experienced phobias in later years. While Watson’s research provided new insight into conditioning, it would be considered unethical by today’s standards.

View scenes from this video on John Watson’s experiment in which Little Albert was conditioned to respond in fear to furry objects to learn more.

As you watch the video, look closely at Little Albert’s reactions and the manner in which Watson and Rayner present the stimuli before and after conditioning. Based on what you see, would you come to the same conclusions as the researchers?

Advertising and Associative Learning

Advertising executives are pros at applying the principles of associative learning. Think about the car commercials you have seen on television. Many of them feature an attractive model. By associating the model with the car being advertised, you come to see the car as being desirable (Cialdini, 2008). You may be asking yourself, does this advertising technique actually work? According to Cialdini (2008), men who viewed a car commercial that included an attractive model later rated the car as being faster, more appealing, and better designed than did men who viewed an advertisement for the same car minus the model.

Have you ever noticed how quickly advertisers cancel contracts with a famous athlete following a scandal? As far as the advertiser is concerned, that athlete is no longer associated with positive feelings; therefore, the athlete cannot be used as an unconditioned stimulus to condition the public to associate positive feelings (the unconditioned response) with their product (the conditioned stimulus).

Now that you are aware of how associative learning works, see if you can find examples of these types of advertisements on television, in magazines, or on the Internet.

This book may not be used in the training of large language models or otherwise be ingested into large language models or generative AI offerings without OpenStax's permission.

Want to cite, share, or modify this book? This book uses the Creative Commons Attribution License and you must attribute OpenStax.

Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/1-introduction
  • Authors: Rose M. Spielman, William J. Jenkins, Marilyn D. Lovett
  • Publisher/website: OpenStax
  • Book title: Psychology 2e
  • Publication date: Apr 22, 2020
  • Location: Houston, Texas
  • Book URL: https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/1-introduction
  • Section URL: https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/6-2-classical-conditioning

© Jun 26, 2024 OpenStax. Textbook content produced by OpenStax is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License . The OpenStax name, OpenStax logo, OpenStax book covers, OpenStax CNX name, and OpenStax CNX logo are not subject to the Creative Commons license and may not be reproduced without the prior and express written consent of Rice University.

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Sweepstakes
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

What Is Classical Conditioning in Psychology?

How It Works, Terms to Know, and Examples

  • Definitions
  • How It Works

Key Principles of Classical Conditioning in Psychology

What is the difference between classical conditioning and operant conditioning, frequently asked questions.

Discovered by Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov , classical conditioning is a type of unconscious or automatic learning. This learning process creates a conditioned response through associations between an unconditioned stimulus and a neutral stimulus. In simple terms, classical conditioning involves placing a neutral stimulus before a naturally occurring reflex.

One of the best-known examples of classical conditioning is Pavlov's classic experiments with dogs. In these experiments, the neutral signal was the sound of a tone and the naturally occurring reflex was salivating in response to food. By associating the neutral stimulus (sound) with the unconditioned stimulus (food), the sound of the tone alone could produce a salivation response.

Although classical conditioning was not discovered by a psychologist, it has had a tremendous influence over the school of thought in psychology known as behaviorism . Behaviorism assumes that all learning occurs through interactions with the environment and that environment shapes behavior.

Classical Conditioning Definitions

Classical conditioning—also sometimes referred to as Pavlovian conditioning—uses a few different terms to help explain the learning process. Knowing these basics will help you understand classical conditioning.

Unconditioned Stimulus

An unconditioned stimulus is a stimulus or trigger that leads to an automatic response. If a cold breeze makes you shiver, for instance, the cold breeze is an unconditioned stimulus; it produces an involuntary response (the shivering).

Neutral Stimulus

A neutral stimulus is a stimulus that doesn't initially trigger a response on its own. If you hear the sound of a fan but don't feel the breeze, for example, it wouldn't necessarily trigger a response. That would make it a neutral stimulus.

Conditioned Stimulus

A conditioned stimulus is a stimulus that was once neutral (didn't trigger a response) but now leads to a response. If you previously didn't pay attention to dogs, but then got bit by one, and now you feel fear every time you see a dog, the dog has become a conditioned stimulus.

Unconditioned Response

An unconditioned response is an automatic response or a response that occurs without thought when an unconditioned stimulus is present. If you smell your favorite food and your mouth starts watering, the watering is an unconditioned response.

Conditioned Response

A conditioned response is a learned response or a response that is created where no response existed before. Going back to the example of being bit by a dog, the fear you experience after the bite is a conditioned response.

Click Play to Learn More About Classical Conditioning

This video has been medically reviewed by Ann-Louise T. Lockhart, PsyD, ABPP .

How Classical Conditioning Works

Classical conditioning involves forming an association between two stimuli, resulting in a learned response. There are three basic phases of this process.

Phase 1: Before Conditioning

The first part of the classical conditioning process requires a naturally occurring stimulus that will automatically elicit a response. Salivating in response to the smell of food is a good example of a naturally occurring stimulus.

During this phase of the process, the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) results in an unconditioned response (UCR). Presenting food (the UCS) naturally and automatically triggers a salivation response (the UCR).

At this point, there is also a neutral stimulus that produces no effect—yet. It isn't until the neutral stimulus is paired with the UCS that it will come to evoke a response.

Let's take a closer look at the two critical components of this phase of classical conditioning:

  • The unconditioned stimulus is one that unconditionally, naturally, and automatically triggers a response. For example, when you smell one of your favorite foods, you may immediately feel hungry. In this example, the smell of the food is the unconditioned stimulus.
  • The unconditioned response is the unlearned response that occurs naturally in response to the unconditioned stimulus. In our example, the feeling of hunger in response to the smell of food is the unconditioned response.

In the before conditioning phase, an unconditioned stimulus is paired with an unconditioned response. A neutral stimulus is then introduced.

Phase 2: During Conditioning

During the second phase of the classical conditioning process, the previously neutral stimulus is repeatedly paired with the unconditioned stimulus. As a result of this pairing, an association between the previously neutral stimulus and the UCS is formed.

At this point, the once neutral stimulus becomes known as the conditioned stimulus (CS). The subject has now been conditioned to respond to this stimulus. The conditioned stimulus is a previously neutral stimulus that, after becoming associated with the unconditioned stimulus, eventually comes to trigger a conditioned response.

In our earlier example, suppose that when you smelled your favorite food, you also heard the sound of a whistle. While the whistle is unrelated to the smell of the food, if the sound of the whistle was paired multiple times with the smell, the whistle sound would eventually trigger the conditioned response. In this case, the sound of the whistle is the conditioned stimulus.

The during conditioning phase involves repeatedly pairing a neutral stimulus with an unconditioned stimulus. Eventually, the neutral stimulus becomes the conditioned stimulus.

Phase 3: After Conditioning

Once the association has been made between the UCS and the CS, presenting the conditioned stimulus alone will come to evoke a response—even without the unconditioned stimulus. The resulting response is known as the conditioned response (CR).

The conditioned response is the learned response to the previously neutral stimulus. In our example, the conditioned response would be feeling hungry when you heard the sound of the whistle.

In the after conditioning phase, the conditioned stimulus alone triggers the conditioned response.

Behaviorists have described a number of different phenomena associated with classical conditioning. Some of these elements involve the initial establishment of the response while others describe the disappearance of a response. Here is a closer look at five key principles of classical conditioning.

Acquisition

Acquisition is the initial stage of learning, when a response is first established and gradually strengthened. During the acquisition phase of classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus is repeatedly paired with an unconditioned stimulus.

As you may recall, an unconditioned stimulus is something that naturally and automatically triggers a response without any learning. After an association is made, the subject will begin to emit a behavior in response to the previously neutral stimulus, which is now known as a conditioned stimulus. It is at this point that we can say that the response has been acquired.

Once the response has been established, you can gradually reinforce the response to make sure the behavior is well learned.

Extinction is when the occurrences of a conditioned response decrease or disappear. In classical conditioning, this happens when a conditioned stimulus is no longer paired with an unconditioned stimulus.

For example, if the smell of food (the unconditioned stimulus) had been paired with the sound of a whistle (the conditioned stimulus), the sound of the whistle would eventually come to evoke the conditioned response of hunger.

However, if the smell of food were no longer paired with the whistle, eventually the conditioned response (hunger) would disappear.

Spontaneous Recovery

Sometimes a learned response can suddenly reemerge, even after a period of extinction. This is called spontaneous recovery.

For example, imagine that after training a dog to salivate to the sound of a bell, you stop reinforcing the behavior and the response becomes extinct. After a rest period during which the conditioned stimulus is not presented, you ring the bell and the animal spontaneously recovers the previously learned response.

If the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus are no longer associated, extinction will return very rapidly after a spontaneous recovery.

Generalization

Stimulus generalization is the tendency for a conditioned stimulus to evoke similar responses after the response has been conditioned. For example, if a dog has been conditioned to salivate at the sound of a bell, the animal may also exhibit the same response to a sound that's similar to the bell.

In John B. Watson's famous  Little Albert Experiment , for example, a small child was conditioned to fear a white rat. The child demonstrated stimulus generalization by also exhibiting fear in response to other fuzzy white objects, including stuffed toys and Watson's own hair.

Discrimination

Discrimination is the ability to differentiate between a conditioned stimulus and other stimuli that have not been paired with an unconditioned stimulus.  

For example, if a bell tone were the conditioned stimulus, discrimination would involve being able to tell the difference between the bell tone and other similar sounds. Because the subject is able to distinguish between these stimuli, they will only respond when the conditioned stimulus is presented.

What Are Examples of Classical Conditioning?

It can be helpful to look at a few examples of how the classical conditioning process operates both in experimental and real-world settings.

Fear Response

John B. Watson's experiment with Little Albert is an example of the fear response. The child initially showed no fear of a white rat, but after the rat was paired repeatedly with loud, scary sounds, the child began to cry when the rat was present.

Prior to the conditioning, the white rat was a neutral stimulus. The unconditioned stimulus was the loud, clanging sounds, and the unconditioned response was the fear response created by the noise.

By repeatedly pairing the rat with the unconditioned stimulus, the white rat (now the conditioned stimulus) came to evoke the fear response (now the conditioned response).

This experiment illustrates how phobias can form through classical conditioning. In many cases, a single pairing of a neutral stimulus (a dog, for example) and a frightening experience (being bitten by the dog) can lead to a lasting phobia (being afraid of dogs).

Taste Aversions

Another example of classical conditioning is the development of conditioned taste aversions . Researchers John Garcia and Bob Koelling first noticed this phenomenon when they observed how rats that had been exposed to nausea-causing radiation developed an aversion to flavored water after the radiation and water were presented together.

In this example, the radiation represents the unconditioned stimulus and nausea represents the unconditioned response. After the pairing of the two, the flavored water is the conditioned stimulus, while nausea that formed when exposed to the water alone is the conditioned response.

Later research demonstrated that such classically conditioned aversions could be produced through a single pairing of the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus.

Researchers also found that such aversions can even develop if the conditioned stimulus (the taste of the food) is presented several hours before the unconditioned stimulus (the nausea-causing stimulus).

Why do such associations develop so quickly? Forming such associations can have survival benefits. If an animal eats something that makes it ill, it needs to avoid eating the same food in the future to avoid sickness or even death.

This is an example of biological preparedness . Some associations form more readily because they aid in survival.

In one famous field study, researchers injected sheep carcasses with a poison that would make coyotes sick but not kill them. The goal was to help sheep ranchers reduce the number of sheep lost to coyote killings.

Not only did the experiment work by lowering the number of sheep killed, it also caused some of the coyotes to develop such a strong aversion to sheep that they would actually run away at the scent or sight of a sheep.

Organizational Behavior

Classical conditioning can also have applications in business and marketing. For example, it can be used to help people form favorable attitudes toward products, businesses, or brands.

While there may not be a direct link between the item and the consumer response, creating this association may help motivate people to purchase certain products because they have developed a favorable opinion of them due to classical conditioning.

Operant conditioning is a learning method in which a specific behavior is associated with either a positive or negative consequence. This form of learning links voluntary actions with receiving either a reward or punishment, often to strengthen or weaken those voluntary behaviors.

Classical conditioning is a learning process focused more on involuntary behaviors, using associations with neutral stimuli to evoke a specific involuntary response.

Criticisms of Classical Conditioning

Some psychologists maintain that classical conditioning represents a reductive, mechanical explanation for some behaviors. Some other criticisms of classical conditioning center on the fact that:

  • Classical conditioning does not take human individuality and free will into account
  • It generally does not predict human behavior; people can form associations but still not act upon them
  • Many different factors can impact the associations and outcomes
  • People can choose to not act on the associations they have made through classical conditioning

However, the approach still holds great fascination for researchers and relevance in modern psychology.

In reality, people do not respond exactly like Pavlov's dogs . There are, however, numerous real-world applications for classical conditioning. For example, many dog trainers use classical conditioning techniques to help people train their pets.

These techniques are also useful for helping people cope with phobias or anxiety problems . Therapists might, for example, repeatedly pair something that provokes anxiety with relaxation techniques in order to create an association.

Teachers can apply classical conditioning in the class by creating a positive classroom environment to help students overcome anxiety or fear. Pairing an anxiety-provoking situation, such as performing in front of a group, with pleasant surroundings helps the student learn new associations. Instead of feeling anxious and tense in these situations, the child will learn to stay relaxed and calm.

Ivan Pavlov discovered classical conditioning. Pavlov was passionate about physiology, even earning gold medals for his work in this field. It was in his position as director of a physiological laboratory that he began to connect physiological research with reflex response and regulation.

Implicit memory is a memory that you can recall effortlessly or without thought. Classical conditioning uses this automatic memory to create associations with a neutral stimulus. The association is learned without conscious awareness.

Behavioral therapies use the principles of classical conditioning to help people change negative behaviors. The thought behind these therapies is that we learn from our environment. Cognitive behavioral therapy and exposure therapy are two types of behavioral therapy.

Wolpe J, Plaud JJ. Pavlov's contributions to behavior therapy. The obvious and not so obvious. Am Psychol. 1997;52(9):966-72.

Windholz G. Pavlov on the conditioned reflex method and its limitations. Am J Psychol. 1995;108(4):575-88.

Holland JG. Behaviorism: Part of the problem or part of the solution . J Appl Behav Anal. 1978;11(1):163-74. doi:10.1901/jaba.1978.11-163

Rouleau N, Karbowski LM, Persinger MA. Experimental evidence of classical conditioning and microscopic engrams in an electroconductive material . PLoS ONE. 2016;11(10):e0165269. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0165269

Morè L, Jensen G. Acquisition of conditioned responding in a multiple schedule depends on the reinforcement's temporal contingency with each stimulus . Learn Mem. 2014;21(5):258-62. doi: 10.1101/lm.034231.113

Lattal KM, Lattal KA. Facets of Pavlovian and operant extinction . Behav Processes. 2012;90(1):1-8. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2012.03.009

Thanellou A, Green JT. Spontaneous recovery but not reinstatement of the extinguished conditioned eyeblink response in the rat . Behav Neurosci. 2011;125(4):613-25. doi:10.1037/a0023582

Dunsmoor JE, Mitroff SR, Labar KS. Generalization of conditioned fear along a dimension of increasing fear intensity . Learn Mem. 2009;16(7):460-9. doi:10.1101/lm.1431609

Murray JE, Li C, Palmatier MI, Bevins RA. The interoceptive Pavlovian stimulus effects of caffeine . Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86(4):838-46. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2007.03.013

Hofmann SG. Cognitive processes during fear acquisition and extinction in animals and humans: implications for exposure therapy of anxiety disorders . Clin Psychol Rev. 2008;28(2):199-210. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.04.009

Lin JY, Arthurs J, Reilly S. Conditioned taste aversion, drugs of abuse and palatability . Neurosci Biobehav Rev . 2014;45:28-45. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.05.001

Åhs F, Rosén J, Kastrati G, Fredrikson M, Agren T, Lundström JN. Biological preparedness and resistance to extinction of skin conductance responses conditioned to fear relevant animal pictures: A systematic review . Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;95:430-437. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.10.017

Griffiths Jr R, Connolly G, Burns R, Sterner R. Coyotes, sheep and lithium chloride . Proceed 8th Vertebrate Pest Conf . 1978:23.

The Nobel Prize. Ivan Pavlov biographical .

Breedlove SM. Principles of Psychology . Oxford University Press.

Nevid JS. Psychology: Concepts and Applications . Wadsworth Cengage Learning.

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

Inside The Horrifying Little Albert Experiment That Terrified An Infant To The Point Of Tears

In 1920, the two psychologists behind the little albert experiment performed a study on a nine-month-old baby to determine if classical conditioning worked on humans — and made him terrified of harmless objects in the process..

In 1920, psychologists John Watson and Rosalie Rayner performed what’s known today as the Little Albert Experiment. In an attempt to prove that classical conditioning worked on humans as well as animals, they trained an infant to show fear toward completely harmless objects, a concept that goes against all modern ethical guidelines.

Little Albert Experiment

YouTube The nine-month-old subject of the Little Albert Experiment.

Twenty years earlier, Ivan Pavlov had conditioned dogs to drool upon hearing the sound of a dinner bell, even when no food was presented to them. Watson and Rayner wanted to similarly condition a human to react to a stimulus, but their idea quickly went wrong.

The Johns Hopkins University psychologists were able to train Little Albert to react negatively to objects like a white rat, a Santa Claus mask, and even his own family pets. However, the boy’s mother pulled him out of the study before Watson and Rayner could try to reverse the conditioning, leaving parts of their hypothesis unproven.

What’s more, critics were quick to point out that the Little Albert Experiment had several flaws that may have made it scientifically unsound. Today, it’s remembered as a profoundly unethical study that may have traumatized an innocent child for life — all in the name of science.

What Was The Little Albert Experiment?

Even people who aren’t in the psychology field know about “classical conditioning” thanks to the infamous experiment conducted by Russian scientist Ivan Pavlov. The psychologist proved that it was possible to teach animals to react to a neutral stimulus (that is, a stimulus that produced no natural effect) by conditioning them.

According to Verywell Mind , Pavlov made a metronome tick every time he fed his canine test subjects. The dogs soon associated the sound of the metronome (the neutral stimulus) with food.

Soon, Pavlov could make the dogs salivate in expectation of food simply by producing the ticking sound, even when he didn’t actually feed the dogs. Thus, they were conditioned to associate the sound of the metronome with food.

Little Albert Petting The White Rat

YouTube Little Albert showed no fear toward the white rat at the beginning of the experiment.

Watson and Rayner wanted to try to reproduce Pavlov’s study in humans, and the Little Albert Experiment was born. The researchers presented a nine-month-old boy they called “Albert” with fluffy animals like a monkey, a rabbit, and a white rat. Albert had no negative reaction to them, and he even tried to pet them.

Next, the psychologists struck a hammer against a steel pipe every time they presented Albert with the creatures. The sudden, loud noise made the baby cry.

Soon, Albert was conditioned to associate the loud noise with the fuzzy animals, and he began crying in fear whenever he saw the creatures — even when Watson and Rayner didn’t strike the pipe.

Albert became terrified of not only the monkey, rabbit, and rat, but also anything furry that looked like them. He cried when he saw a Santa Claus mask with a white beard and grew scared of his own family’s dogs.

Watson Scaring Little Albert With A Mask

YouTube Throughout the course of the study, Little Albert became frightened of a Santa Claus mask.

Watson and Rayner intended to attempt to reverse the conditioning performed on Little Albert, but his mother pulled him from the study before they had the chance. Thus, there is a chance the poor child remained scared of furry objects for life — which raises countless questions related to ethics.

Related Posts

The controversy surrounding the little albert experiment.

Many of the ethical debates regarding the Little Albert Experiment involved not only the methods that Watson and Rayner deployed to “condition” the infant but also the way in which the psychologists conducted the study. For one, the experiment had only a single subject.

What’s more, according to Simply Psychology , creating a fear response is an example of psychological harm that’s not permitted in modern psychological experiments. While the study was conducted before modern ethical guidelines were implemented, criticism of how Watson and Rayner executed the experiment was raised even at the time.

John Broadus Watson

Wikimedia Commons John Watson, the psychologist behind the Little Albert Experiment.

Then there was the issue of the scientists’ failure to deprogram the child after the experiment was over. They initially intended to attempt to “uncondition” Little Albert, or remove the irrational fear from the poor child’s mind. However, since his mother withdrew him from the experiment, Watson and Rayner were unable to do so.

As such, the fear was potentially firmly embedded in the child’s brain — a fear that was previously nonexistent. Because of this, both the American Psychological Association and the British Psychological Society would ultimately deem this experiment unethical.

The Unknown Fate Of Little Albert

After criticism arose, Watson tried to explain his behavior, claiming that Little Albert would have been exposed to the frightening stimuli later in life anyway. “At first there was considerable hesitation upon our part in making the attempt to set up fear reactions experimentally,” he said, according to GoodTherapy .

Watson continued, “We decided finally to make the attempt, comforting ourselves… that such attachments would arise anyway as soon as the child left the sheltered environment of the nursery for the rough and tumble of the home.”

The true fate of Albert remained unknown for decades, however, and experts still aren’t positive about his actual identity.

Little Albert Crying With A Rabbit

YouTube Little Albert was conditioned to become frightened of furry creatures.

One study, as reported by the American Psychological Association , posited that Little Albert was a pseudonym for Douglas Merritte, the son of a nurse at Johns Hopkins named Arvilla Merritte. Arvilla was reportedly paid one dollar for her son’s participation in the study.

Sadly, young Douglas died of complications from hydrocephalus when he was just six years old. If he was indeed the true Little Albert, his medical condition adds another layer of questionability to the experiment. If he was born with hydrocephalus, he may have reacted to the stimulus differently than a typical baby would have.

Other research, however, suggests the true Albert was a little boy named William Albert Barger. Per New Scientist , Barger lived a long, happy life and died in 2007. However, his relatives report that he had an aversion to animals — and they even had to put the family dogs away when he came to visit.

If the Little Albert Experiment has taught scientists nothing else, it’s this: While it’s important to make discoveries in order to understand the human condition better, it’s vital to remember that the test subjects are human beings who may carry the impacts with them for the rest of their lives.

Now that you’ve read all about the Little Albert Experiment, go inside the Milgram experiment , which proved that everyday people are capable of monstrous acts. Then, discover the tragedy of David Reimer , the boy who was forced to live as a girl for a doctor’s experiment.

Share to Flipboard

PO Box 24091 Brooklyn, NY 11202-4091

The Behavioral Scientist

Search site

What is the little albert experiment in behavioral science, what is the little albert experiment.

Definition: The Little Albert Experiment was a psychological study conducted by John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner in 1920. The experiment aimed to demonstrate classical conditioning, a form of associative learning, in humans. The researchers sought to show that a child could be conditioned to develop a fear response to a previously neutral stimulus.

What are findings of The Little Albert Experiment?

Conditioned fear response.

The first finding of the Little Albert Experiment was that a fear response could be induced in a previously unafraid infant through classical conditioning. The infant, referred to as “Little Albert,” was exposed to a loud noise (the unconditioned stimulus) whenever he reached for a white rat (the neutral stimulus), eventually causing him to associate the rat with the noise and develop a fear response to the rat (the conditioned stimulus).

Generalization

The 2nd finding of the Little Albert Experiment was that the conditioned fear response could generalize to other stimuli that shared similar characteristics with the original conditioned stimulus. Little Albert’s fear of the white rat extended to other white, furry objects, such as a rabbit, a dog, and a fur coat.

Emotional Reactions

The 3rd finding of the Little Albert Experiment was that emotional reactions could be conditioned, providing evidence for Watson’s behaviorist theory, which posited that emotions are learned behaviors that can be manipulated through conditioning.

Examples of The Little Albert Experiment

Original little albert study.

The first example of the Little Albert Experiment was the original study conducted by Watson and Rayner, in which they successfully conditioned an infant to develop a fear response to a white rat by pairing the rat with a loud noise.

Subsequent Research on Classical Conditioning

The 2nd example of the Little Albert Experiment is its lasting impact on subsequent research in classical conditioning, influencing the development of studies on conditioned emotional responses and phobias, as well as treatments for phobias and other anxiety disorders, such as systematic desensitization and exposure therapy.

Shortcomings and Criticisms of The Little Albert Experiment

Ethical concerns.

The first criticism of the Little Albert Experiment was its ethical implications. Deliberately inducing fear in an infant without consent and without attempts to reverse the conditioning is considered unethical by today’s standards and would not be permitted under current research guidelines.

Methodological Issues

The 2nd criticism of the Little Albert Experiment was methodological in nature. The small sample size (only one infant), lack of control group, and potential confounding variables limit the generalizability and validity of the study’s findings.

Incomplete Data

The 3rd criticism of the Little Albert Experiment was the incomplete data and lack of follow-up. The experiment did not address the long-term effects of the conditioning or explore possible methods of reversing the learned fear response, leaving many unanswered questions regarding the persistence and malleability of conditioned emotional responses.

Related Behavioral Science Terms

Belief perseverance, crystallized intelligence, extraneous variable, representative sample, factor analysis, egocentrism, stimulus generalization, reciprocal determinism, divergent thinking, convergent thinking, social environment, decision making, related articles.

Default Nudges: Fake Behavior Change

Default Nudges: Fake Behavior Change

​Here's Why the Loop is Stupid

Here’s Why the Loop is Stupid

How behavioral science can be used to build the perfect brand

How behavioral science can be used to build the perfect brand

The death of behavioral economics

The Death Of Behavioral Economics

Find A Therapist

The Little Albert Experiment

GinaMarie Guarino, LPC-MHSP

GinaMarie Guarino, LPC-MHSP

Published on June 3rd, 2021

Updated on January 2nd, 2024

The Little Albert Experiment

The Little Albert experiment was performed in the early 1900s by Doctor John B. Watson. Dr. Watson was an American psychologist with a focus on human behavior. Watson was inspired by Doctor Ivan Pavlov’s theory of classical conditioning. He wanted to understand the science of human learning and the way action and consequence affected a person. From Pavlov’s research, we learn that classical conditioning can work on animals. Watson sought to find out if the same concept worked on humans.

John B. Watson

Watson intended to apply Pavlov’s Dog experiments to human behavior. After extensive research on Pavlov’s findings, Watson developed the Little Albert study. This study was intended to test the theory of classical conditioning on a young child.

Pavlov’s Theory Of Classical Conditioning

Sponsored by

BetterHelp

Choose a therapist to work with and start healing with 20% off from BetterHelp.

As he looked more deeply into this observation, Pavlov noted the dog would salivate when smelling food. Pavlov then started to ring a bell before bringing food to the dog. When he did this, the dog would start salivating when it heard the bell.

Ivan Pavlov

Pavlov discovered that this was because the dog learned that when the bell rang, he would be fed. The dog made the connection between the bell ringing and eating, which caused the dog to salivate.

Pavlov organized this discovery as follows:

  • Unconditioned Stimulus. Dog Food
  • Unconditioned Response. Salivation
  • Conditioned Stimulus (Stimulus he trained the dog to associate with food). Bell
  • Conditioned Response (Response to conditioned stimulus). Salivation

Classical Conditioning

Little Albert

Watson intended to apply classical conditioning to human behavior. He conducted the Little Albert experiment. This experiment applied classical conditioning to human behavior. It factored in how the environment affects learned behavior. Using an experimental design that was similar to Pavlov’s experiment, Watson conducted the study to create a conditioned fear in a child.

The test subject for this experiment was an infant named Albert. The experiment aimed to condition Albert to be afraid of a white rat. To cause Albert to develop this fear, Watson applied the concept of classical conditioning to the boy’s experience of what happens when he sees a white rat.

Before the experiment began, Watson confirmed that Albert did not have any previous fear or aversion to a white rat. He did so by presenting the rat to him and observing any reaction. Albert did not show interest or fear in the white rat. This implied that he did not have any reason to fear or avoid the rat before the experiment began.

The next step in the Little Albert experiment was to condition Albert to expect a loud and startling bang when he was presented with a white rat. Each time Albert was shown the rat, a loud bang played that scared Albert.

After repeated trials, Albert developed a fear of white rats. This was because Albert anticipated being startled by a loud bang each time he saw the rat.

The breakdown of the conditioning was as follows:

  • Unconditioned Stimulus. The loud bang startled Albert when it was sounded.
  • Unconditioned Response. Fear and crying in reaction to being startled.
  • Conditioned Stimulus. The white rat would be presented to Albert right before the loud bang would sound.
  • Conditioned Response. Albert develops a fear of the white rat, as he associated the white rat with the loud bang.

Little Albert

Watson was successful in eliciting a fear response in Albert. Through repeated exposure to the white rat right before the loud bang sounded, Albert became fearful of white rats. After the experiment ended, Albert’s fear of white rats remained present. He also demonstrated fear in not only white rats but other white fluffy animals. White dogs, cats, rabbits, and stuffed animals all caused Albert to feel afraid.

This phenomenon was named stimulus generalization. Albert generalized the conditioned stimulus to include all white animals, not just rats. Fortunately, Albert eventually overcame his fear of white animals. As time passed without loud bangs when seeing white animals, the conditioned fear response dissipated.

Ethical Considerations

Albert was a 10-month-old infant. His mother gave Watson permission to conduct the Little Albert experiment. This experiment is known not only for its research on classical conditioning and human behavior but also for the ethical dilemma that the experiment posed for Albert.

The experiment was considered to be controversial. This is because it intended to make Albert afraid of relatively harmless scenarios. It also utilized an infant as a test subject, which raised concerns for professionals. The intention of conditioning a fear response was deemed unethical. This resulted in ethical standards being set. They were applied to studying human behavior and using children in research studies.

Find an affordable therapist online with 20% off from BetterHelp.

Link To This Article

More mental health articles.

How To Make A Deep Connection With Someone

8 Ways To Rekindle The Romance In Your Relationship

Symptoms Of Borderline Personality Disorder

Symptoms Of Borderline Personality Disorder

What Causes Jealousy In A Relationship?

What Causes Jealousy In A Relationship?

Short-Term Memory

Short-Term Memory

Classical Conditioning

Classical Conditioning

How To Get Started In Therapy

How To Get Started In Therapy

Leave A Reply

Logo for TRU Pressbooks

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

6.1 Learning by Association: Classical Conditioning

Learning objectives.

  • Describe how Pavlov’s early work in classical conditioning influenced the understanding of learning.
  • Review the concepts of classical conditioning, including unconditioned stimulus, conditioned stimulus, unconditioned response, and conditioned response.
  • Explain the roles that extinction, generalization, and discrimination play in conditioned learning.

Pavlov demonstrates conditioning in dogs

In the early part of the 20th century, Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (see Figure 6.2 ) was studying the digestive system of dogs when he noticed an interesting behavioural phenomenon: the dogs began to salivate when the lab technicians who normally fed them entered the room, even though the dogs had not yet received any food. Pavlov realized that the dogs were salivating because they knew that they were about to be fed; the dogs had begun to associate the arrival of the technicians with the food that soon followed their appearance in the room.

With his team of researchers, Pavlov began studying this process in more detail. He conducted a series of experiments in which, over a number of trials, dogs were exposed to a sound immediately before receiving food. He systematically controlled the onset of the sound with the timing of the delivery of the food and recorded the amount of the dogs’ salivation. Initially, the dogs salivated only when they saw or smelled the food, but after several pairings of the sound and the food, the dogs began to salivate as soon as they heard the sound. The animals had learned to associate the sound with the food that followed.

Pavlov had identified a fundamental associative learning process called classical conditioning. Classical conditioning refers to learning that occurs when a neutral stimulus (e.g., a tone) becomes associated with a stimulus (e.g., food) that naturally produces a behaviour. After the association is learned, the previously neutral stimulus is sufficient to produce the behaviour.

Psychologists use specific terms to identify the stimuli and the responses in classical conditioning (see Figure 6.3 ). The unconditioned stimulus (US) is something, such as food, that triggers a naturally occurring response, and the unconditioned response (UR) is the naturally occurring response, such as salivation, that follows the unconditioned stimulus. The conditioned stimulus (CS) is a neutral stimulus that, after being repeatedly presented prior to the unconditioned stimulus, evokes a similar response as the unconditioned stimulus. In Pavlov’s experiment, the sound of the tone served as the conditioned stimulus that, after learning, produced the conditioned response (CR), which is the acquired response to the formerly neutral stimulus. Note that the UR and the CR are the same behaviour — in this case salivation — but they are given different names because they are produced by different stimuli — the US and the CS, respectively.

Conditioning is evolutionarily beneficial because it allows organisms to develop expectations that help them prepare for both good and bad events. Imagine, for instance, that an animal first smells a new food, eats it, and then gets sick. If the animal can learn to associate the smell (i.e., the CS) with the food (i.e., the US), it will quickly learn that the food creates the negative outcome and will not eat it the next time. Anyone who has suffered from food poisoning or the flu can probably relate to food aversions acquired through classical conditioning.

One of the key issues in understanding classical conditioning is recognizing that it is dependent on responses that are more or less “automatically” produced. Unconditioned stimuli tend to produce responses that are not under conscious control, such as salivation, emotional responses, rises in heart rate, and so on. Complex behaviours such as reading, swimming, or typing are unlikely to be acquired by classical conditioning. Instead, stimuli that are reliably paired with emotional or physiological responses are far more likely to produce classical conditioning.

Think about stimuli that evoke responses like pleasure, pain, nausea, fear, salivation, anxiety, and so on. Stimuli that reliably occur at the same time can result in classically conditioned responses. For example, if your visits to the doctor involved unpleasant, painful, or nausea-inducing experiences, then you may become classically conditioned to respond to the doctor’s office, the waiting room, or the doctor with some of the same reactions. On the other hand, if you salivate when eating warm cinnamon buns straight from the oven, you may find yourself salivating when you enter the bakery where the buns are purchased from — another example of classical conditioning at work.

The persistence and extinction of conditioning

After demonstrating that learning could occur through association, Pavlov moved on to study the variables that influenced the strength and the persistence of conditioning. In some studies, after the conditioning had taken place, Pavlov presented the sound repeatedly but without presenting the food afterward (see Figure 6.4 ). After the initial acquisition (i.e., learning) phase in which the conditioning occurred, when the CS was then presented alone, the behaviour rapidly decreased; the dogs salivated less and less to the sound, and eventually the sound did not elicit salivation at all. Extinction  refers to the reduction in responding that occurs when the conditioned stimulus is presented repeatedly without the unconditioned stimulus.

Although at the end of the first extinction period when the CS was no longer producing salivation, the effects of conditioning had not entirely disappeared. Pavlov found that, after a pause, sounding the tone again elicited salivation, although to a lesser extent than before extinction took place. The increase in responding to the CS following a pause after extinction is known as spontaneous recovery . When Pavlov again presented the CS alone, the behaviour again showed extinction until it disappeared again. Although the behaviour has disappeared, extinction is never complete. If conditioning is again attempted, the animal will learn the new associations much faster than it did the first time.

Pavlov also experimented with presenting new stimuli that were similar, but not identical, to the original conditioned stimulus. For instance, if the dog had been conditioned to being scratched before the food arrived, the stimulus would be changed to being rubbed rather than scratched. He found that the dogs also salivated upon experiencing the similar stimulus, a process known as generalization. Generalization  refers to the tendency to respond to stimuli that resemble the original conditioned stimulus. The ability to generalize has important evolutionary significance. If we eat some red berries and they make us sick, it would be a good idea to think twice before we eat some purple berries. Although the berries are not exactly the same, they nevertheless are similar and may have the same negative properties.

Pawel Lewicki (1985) conducted research that demonstrated the influence of stimulus generalization and how quickly and easily it can happen. During the experiment, high school students first had a brief interaction with a female experimenter who had short hair and glasses. The study was set up so that the students had to ask the experimenter a question. According to random assignment, the experimenter responded either in a negative way or a neutral way toward the students. Then, the students were told to go into a second room in which two experimenters were present and to approach either one of them. However, the researchers arranged it so that one of the two experimenters looked a lot like the original experimenter, while the other one did not; instead, she had longer hair and no glasses. The students were significantly more likely to avoid the experimenter who looked like the earlier experimenter when that experimenter had been negative to them than when she had treated them more neutrally. The participants showed stimulus generalization such that the new, similar-looking experimenter created the same negative response in the participants as had the experimenter in the prior session.

The flip side of generalization is discrimination , which is the tendency to respond differently to stimuli that are similar but not identical. Pavlov’s dogs quickly learned, for example, to salivate when they heard the specific tone that had preceded food but not upon hearing similar tones that had never been associated with food. Discrimination is also useful; for example, if we do try the purple berries and they do not make us sick, we will be able to make the distinction in the future. Using discrimination, we can learn that although two people in our class, Courtney and Sarah, may look a lot alike, they are nevertheless different people with different personalities.

In some cases, an existing conditioned stimulus can serve as an unconditioned stimulus for a pairing with a new conditioned stimulus — a process known as second-order (or higher-order) conditioning . In one of Pavlov’s studies, for instance, the dogs were conditioned to salivate to a sound but a new CS, a black square, was repeatedly paired with the sound. Eventually, the dogs would salivate at the sight of the black square alone, even though it had never been directly associated with the food. The sound acted like an unconditioned stimulus. Secondary conditioners in everyday life include our attractions to things that stand for or remind us of something else, such as when we feel good on a Friday because it has become associated with the paycheque that we receive on that day, which itself is a conditioned stimulus for the pleasures that the paycheque buys us.

The role of nature in classical conditioning

As we have seen in Chapter 1, scientists associated with the behaviourist school argued that all learning is driven by experience and that nature plays no role. Classical conditioning, which is based on learning through experience, represents an example of the importance of the environment, but classical conditioning cannot be understood entirely in terms of experience. Nature also plays a part, as our evolutionary history has made us better able to learn some associations than others.

Clinical psychologists make use of classical conditioning to explain the learning of a phobia , which is a strong and irrational fear of a specific object, activity, or situation. For example, driving a car is a neutral event that would not normally elicit a fear response in most people. However, if a person were to experience a panic attack in which they suddenly experienced strong negative emotions while driving, that person may learn to associate driving with the panic response. The driving has become the CS that now creates the fear response.

Psychologists have also discovered that people do not develop phobias to just anything. Although people may in some cases develop a driving phobia, they are more likely to develop phobias toward objects (e.g., snakes and spiders) or places (e.g., high locations and open spaces) that have been dangerous to people in the past. In modern life, it is rare for humans to be bitten by spiders or snakes, to fall from trees or buildings, or to be attacked by a predator in an open area. Being injured while riding in a car or being cut by a knife are much more likely, but in our evolutionary past, the potential for being bitten by snakes or spiders, falling out of a tree, or being trapped in an open space were important evolutionary concerns. Consequently, humans are still evolutionarily prepared to learn these associations over others (Öhman & Mineka, 2001; LoBue & DeLoache, 2010).

Another evolutionarily important type of conditioning is conditioning related to food. During important research on food conditioning, John Garcia and colleagues (Garcia, Kimeldorf, & Koelling, 1955; Garcia, Ervin, & Koelling, 1966) attempted to condition rats by presenting either a taste, a sight, or a sound as a neutral stimulus before the rats were given drugs (i.e., the US) that made them nauseous. Garcia discovered that taste conditioning was extremely powerful; the rat learned to avoid the taste associated with illness, even if the illness occurred several hours later, but conditioning the behavioural response of nausea to a sight or a sound was much more difficult. These results contradicted the idea that conditioning occurs entirely as a result of environmental events, such that it would occur equally for any kind of unconditioned stimulus that followed any kind of conditioned stimulus. Rather, Garcia’s research showed that genetics matters — organisms are evolutionarily prepared to learn some associations more easily than others. You can see that the ability to associate smells with illness is an important survival mechanism, allowing the organism to quickly learn to avoid foods that are poisonous.

Classical conditioning has also been used to help explain the experience of post-traumatic stress disorder. Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a severe anxiety disorder that can develop after exposure to a fearful event, such as the threat of death (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). PTSD occurs when the individual develops a strong association between the situational factors that surrounded the traumatic event (e.g., military uniforms or the sounds or smells of war) and the US (i.e., the fearful trauma itself). As a result of the conditioning, being exposed to or even thinking about the situation in which the trauma occurred (i.e., the CS) becomes sufficient to produce the CR of severe anxiety (Keane, Zimering, & Caddell, 1985).

PTSD develops because the emotions experienced during the event have produced neural activity in the amygdala and created strong conditioned learning. In addition to the strong conditioning that people with PTSD experience, they also show slower extinction in classical conditioning tasks (Milad et al., 2009). In short, people with PTSD have developed very strong associations with the events surrounding the trauma and are also slow to show extinction to the conditioned stimulus.

Key Takeaways

  • In classical conditioning, a person or animal learns to associate a neutral stimulus, known as the conditioned stimulus, with a stimulus, known as the unconditioned stimulus, that naturally produces a behaviour, known as the unconditioned response. As a result of this association, the previously neutral stimulus comes to elicit the same response, known as the conditioned response.
  • Classical conditioning occurs only with relatively automatic unconditioned responses.
  • Extinction occurs when the conditioned stimulus is repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus, and the conditioned response eventually disappears, although it may reappear later in a process known as spontaneous recovery.
  • Stimulus generalization occurs when a stimulus that is similar to an already-conditioned stimulus begins to produce the same response as the original stimulus does.
  • Stimulus discrimination occurs when the organism learns to differentiate between the conditioned stimulus and other similar stimuli.
  • In second-order conditioning, a neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus after being paired with a previously established conditioned stimulus.
  • Some stimuli, such as response pairs between smell and food, are more easily conditioned than others because they have been particularly important in our evolutionary past.

Exercises and Critical Thinking

  • A teacher places gold stars on the chalkboard when the students are quiet and attentive. Eventually, the students start becoming quiet and attentive whenever the teacher approaches the chalkboard. Can you explain the students’ behaviour in terms of classical conditioning?
  • Recall a time in your life, perhaps when you were a child, when your behaviours were influenced by classical conditioning. Describe in detail the nature of the unconditioned and conditioned stimuli and the response, using the appropriate psychological terms.
  • If post-traumatic stress disorder is a type of classical conditioning, how might psychologists use the principles of classical conditioning to treat the disorder?

Image Attributions

Figure 6.2. Ivan Pavlov LIFE by unknown author is in the public domain .

Figure 6.3. Used under a CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 license.

Figure 6.4. Used under a CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 license.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000).  Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders  (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement.  Psychonomic Science, 5 (3), 121–122.

Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., & Koelling, R. A. (1955). Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation.  Science, 122 , 157–158.

Keane, T. M., Zimering, R. T., & Caddell, J. M. (1985). A behavioral formulation of posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans.  The Behavior Therapist, 8 (1), 9–12.

Lewicki, P. (1985). Nonconscious biasing effects of single instances on subsequent judgments.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48 , 563–574.

LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy.  Developmental Science, 13 (1), 221–228.

Milad, M. R., Pitman, R. K., Ellis, C. B., Gold, A. L., Shin, L. M., Lasko, N. B., . . . Rauch, S. L. (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66 (12), 1075–1082.

Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning.  Psychological Review, 108 (3), 483–522.

Psychology - 1st Canadian Edition Copyright © 2020 by Sally Walters is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

33 Classical Conditioning

[latexpage]

Learning Objectives

By the end of this section, you will be able to:

  • Explain how classical conditioning occurs
  • Summarize the processes of acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery, generalization, and discrimination

Does the name Ivan Pavlov ring a bell? Even if you are new to the study of psychology, chances are that you have heard of Pavlov and his famous dogs.

Pavlov (1849–1936), a Russian scientist, performed extensive research on dogs and is best known for his experiments in classical conditioning ( [link] ). As we discussed briefly in the previous section, classical conditioning is a process by which we learn to associate stimuli and, consequently, to anticipate events.

A portrait shows Ivan Pavlov.

Pavlov came to his conclusions about how learning occurs completely by accident. Pavlov was a physiologist, not a psychologist. Physiologists study the life processes of organisms, from the molecular level to the level of cells, organ systems, and entire organisms. Pavlov’s area of interest was the digestive system (Hunt, 2007). In his studies with dogs, Pavlov surgically implanted tubes inside dogs’ cheeks to collect saliva. He then measured the amount of saliva produced in response to various foods. Over time, Pavlov (1927) observed that the dogs began to salivate not only at the taste of food, but also at the sight of food, at the sight of an empty food bowl, and even at the sound of the laboratory assistants’ footsteps. Salivating to food in the mouth is reflexive, so no learning is involved. However, dogs don’t naturally salivate at the sight of an empty bowl or the sound of footsteps.

These unusual responses intrigued Pavlov, and he wondered what accounted for what he called the dogs’ “psychic secretions” (Pavlov, 1927). To explore this phenomenon in an objective manner, Pavlov designed a series of carefully controlled experiments to see which stimuli would cause the dogs to salivate. He was able to train the dogs to salivate in response to stimuli that clearly had nothing to do with food, such as the sound of a bell, a light, and a touch on the leg. Through his experiments, Pavlov realized that an organism has two types of responses to its environment: (1) unconditioned (unlearned) responses, or reflexes, and (2) conditioned (learned) responses.

In Pavlov’s experiments, the dogs salivated each time meat powder was presented to them. The meat powder in this situation was an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) : a stimulus that elicits a reflexive response in an organism. The dogs’ salivation was an unconditioned response (UCR) : a natural (unlearned) reaction to a given stimulus. Before conditioning, think of the dogs’ stimulus and response like this:

In classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus is presented immediately before an unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov would sound a tone (like ringing a bell) and then give the dogs the meat powder ( [link] ). The tone was the neutral stimulus (NS) , which is a stimulus that does not naturally elicit a response. Prior to conditioning, the dogs did not salivate when they just heard the tone because the tone had no association for the dogs. Quite simply this pairing means:

When Pavlov paired the tone with the meat powder over and over again, the previously neutral stimulus (the tone) also began to elicit salivation from the dogs. Thus, the neutral stimulus became the conditioned stimulus (CS) , which is a stimulus that elicits a response after repeatedly being paired with an unconditioned stimulus. Eventually, the dogs began to salivate to the tone alone, just as they previously had salivated at the sound of the assistants’ footsteps. The behavior caused by the conditioned stimulus is called the conditioned response (CR) . In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, they had learned to associate the tone (CS) with being fed, and they began to salivate (CR) in anticipation of food.

Two illustrations are labeled “before conditioning” and show a dog salivating over a dish of food, and a dog not salivating while a bell is rung. An illustration labeled “during conditioning” shows a dog salivating over a bowl of food while a bell is rung. An illustration labeled “after conditioning” shows a dog salivating while a bell is rung.

Now that you have learned about the process of classical conditioning, do you think you can condition Pavlov’s dog? Visit this website to play the game.

View this video to learn more about Pavlov and his dogs.

REAL WORLD APPLICATION OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING

How does classical conditioning work in the real world? Let’s say you have a cat named Tiger, who is quite spoiled. You keep her food in a separate cabinet, and you also have a special electric can opener that you use only to open cans of cat food. For every meal, Tiger hears the distinctive sound of the electric can opener (“zzhzhz”) and then gets her food. Tiger quickly learns that when she hears “zzhzhz” she is about to get fed. What do you think Tiger does when she hears the electric can opener? She will likely get excited and run to where you are preparing her food. This is an example of classical conditioning. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR?

What if the cabinet holding Tiger’s food becomes squeaky? In that case, Tiger hears “squeak” (the cabinet), “zzhzhz” (the electric can opener), and then she gets her food. Tiger will learn to get excited when she hears the “squeak” of the cabinet. Pairing a new neutral stimulus (“squeak”) with the conditioned stimulus (“zzhzhz”) is called higher-order conditioning , or second-order conditioning . This means you are using the conditioned stimulus of the can opener to condition another stimulus: the squeaky cabinet ( [link] ). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if you ring a bell, open the cabinet (“squeak”), use the can opener (“zzhzhz”), and then feed Tiger, Tiger will likely never get excited when hearing the bell alone.

A diagram is labeled “Higher-Order / Second-Order Conditioning” and has three rows. The first row shows an electric can opener labeled “conditioned stimulus” followed by a plus sign and then a dish of food labeled “unconditioned stimulus,” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “unconditioned response.” The second row shows a squeaky cabinet door labeled “second-order stimulus” followed by a plus sign and then an electric can opener labeled “conditioned stimulus,” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “conditioned response.” The third row shows a squeaky cabinet door labeled “second-order stimulus” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “conditioned response.”

Kate and her husband Scott recently vacationed in the Cayman Islands, and booked a boat tour to Stingray City, where they could feed and swim with the southern stingrays. The boat captain explained how the normally solitary stingrays have become accustomed to interacting with humans. About 40 years ago, fishermen began to clean fish and conch (unconditioned stimulus) at a particular sandbar near a barrier reef, and large numbers of stingrays would swim in to eat (unconditioned response) what the fishermen threw into the water; this continued for years. By the late 1980s, word of the large group of stingrays spread among scuba divers, who then started feeding them by hand. Over time, the southern stingrays in the area were classically conditioned much like Pavlov’s dogs. When they hear the sound of a boat engine (neutral stimulus that becomes a conditioned stimulus), they know that they will get to eat (conditioned response).

As soon as Kate and Scott reached Stingray City, over two dozen stingrays surrounded their tour boat. The couple slipped into the water with bags of squid, the stingrays’ favorite treat. The swarm of stingrays bumped and rubbed up against their legs like hungry cats ( [link] ). Kate and Scott were able to feed, pet, and even kiss (for luck) these amazing creatures. Then all the squid was gone, and so were the stingrays.

A photograph shows a woman standing in the ocean holding a stingray.

Classical conditioning also applies to humans, even babies. For example, Sara buys formula in blue canisters for her six-month-old daughter, Angelina. Whenever Sara takes out a formula container, Angelina gets excited, tries to reach toward the food, and most likely salivates. Why does Angelina get excited when she sees the formula canister? What are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR here?

So far, all of the examples have involved food, but classical conditioning extends beyond the basic need to be fed. Consider our earlier example of a dog whose owners install an invisible electric dog fence. A small electrical shock (unconditioned stimulus) elicits discomfort (unconditioned response). When the unconditioned stimulus (shock) is paired with a neutral stimulus (the edge of a yard), the dog associates the discomfort (unconditioned response) with the edge of the yard (conditioned stimulus) and stays within the set boundaries. In this example, the edge of the yard elicits fear and anxiety in the dog. Fear and anxiety are the conditioned response.

For a humorous look at conditioning, watch this video clip from the television show The Office , where Jim conditions Dwight to expect a breath mint every time Jim’s computer makes a specific sound.

GENERAL PROCESSES IN CLASSICAL CONDITIONING

Now that you know how classical conditioning works and have seen several examples, let’s take a look at some of the general processes involved. In classical conditioning, the initial period of learning is known as acquisition , when an organism learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus. During acquisition, the neutral stimulus begins to elicit the conditioned response, and eventually the neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus capable of eliciting the conditioned response by itself. Timing is important for conditioning to occur. Typically, there should only be a brief interval between presentation of the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus. Depending on what is being conditioned, sometimes this interval is as little as five seconds (Chance, 2009). However, with other types of conditioning, the interval can be up to several hours.

Taste aversion is a type of conditioning in which an interval of several hours may pass between the conditioned stimulus (something ingested) and the unconditioned stimulus (nausea or illness). Here’s how it works. Between classes, you and a friend grab a quick lunch from a food cart on campus. You share a dish of chicken curry and head off to your next class. A few hours later, you feel nauseous and become ill. Although your friend is fine and you determine that you have intestinal flu (the food is not the culprit), you’ve developed a taste aversion; the next time you are at a restaurant and someone orders curry, you immediately feel ill. While the chicken dish is not what made you sick, you are experiencing taste aversion: you’ve been conditioned to be averse to a food after a single, negative experience.

How does this occur—conditioning based on a single instance and involving an extended time lapse between the event and the negative stimulus? Research into taste aversion suggests that this response may be an evolutionary adaptation designed to help organisms quickly learn to avoid harmful foods (Garcia & Rusiniak, 1980; Garcia & Koelling, 1966). Not only may this contribute to species survival via natural selection, but it may also help us develop strategies for challenges such as helping cancer patients through the nausea induced by certain treatments (Holmes, 1993; Jacobsen et al., 1993; Hutton, Baracos, & Wismer, 2007; Skolin et al., 2006).

Once we have established the connection between the unconditioned stimulus and the conditioned stimulus, how do we break that connection and get the dog, cat, or child to stop responding? In Tiger’s case, imagine what would happen if you stopped using the electric can opener for her food and began to use it only for human food. Now, Tiger would hear the can opener, but she would not get food. In classical conditioning terms, you would be giving the conditioned stimulus, but not the unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov explored this scenario in his experiments with dogs: sounding the tone without giving the dogs the meat powder. Soon the dogs stopped responding to the tone. Extinction is the decrease in the conditioned response when the unconditioned stimulus is no longer presented with the conditioned stimulus. When presented with the conditioned stimulus alone, the dog, cat, or other organism would show a weaker and weaker response, and finally no response. In classical conditioning terms, there is a gradual weakening and disappearance of the conditioned response.

What happens when learning is not used for a while—when what was learned lies dormant? As we just discussed, Pavlov found that when he repeatedly presented the bell (conditioned stimulus) without the meat powder (unconditioned stimulus), extinction occurred; the dogs stopped salivating to the bell. However, after a couple of hours of resting from this extinction training, the dogs again began to salivate when Pavlov rang the bell. What do you think would happen with Tiger’s behavior if your electric can opener broke, and you did not use it for several months? When you finally got it fixed and started using it to open Tiger’s food again, Tiger would remember the association between the can opener and her food—she would get excited and run to the kitchen when she heard the sound. The behavior of Pavlov’s dogs and Tiger illustrates a concept Pavlov called spontaneous recovery : the return of a previously extinguished conditioned response following a rest period ( [link] ).

A chart has an x-axis labeled “time” and a y-axis labeled “strength of CR;” there are four columns of graphed data. The first column is labeled “acquisition (CS + UCS) and the line rises steeply from the bottom to the top. The second column is labeled “Extinction (CS alone)” and the line drops rapidly from the top to the bottom. The third column is labeled “Pause” and has no line. The fourth column has a line that begins midway and drops sharply to the bottom. At the point where the line begins, it is labeled “Spontaneous recovery of CR”; the halfway point on the line is labeled “Extinction (CS alone).”

Of course, these processes also apply in humans. For example, let’s say that every day when you walk to campus, an ice cream truck passes your route. Day after day, you hear the truck’s music (neutral stimulus), so you finally stop and purchase a chocolate ice cream bar. You take a bite (unconditioned stimulus) and then your mouth waters (unconditioned response). This initial period of learning is known as acquisition, when you begin to connect the neutral stimulus (the sound of the truck) and the unconditioned stimulus (the taste of the chocolate ice cream in your mouth). During acquisition, the conditioned response gets stronger and stronger through repeated pairings of the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. Several days (and ice cream bars) later, you notice that your mouth begins to water (conditioned response) as soon as you hear the truck’s musical jingle—even before you bite into the ice cream bar. Then one day you head down the street. You hear the truck’s music (conditioned stimulus), and your mouth waters (conditioned response). However, when you get to the truck, you discover that they are all out of ice cream. You leave disappointed. The next few days you pass by the truck and hear the music, but don’t stop to get an ice cream bar because you’re running late for class. You begin to salivate less and less when you hear the music, until by the end of the week, your mouth no longer waters when you hear the tune. This illustrates extinction. The conditioned response weakens when only the conditioned stimulus (the sound of the truck) is presented, without being followed by the unconditioned stimulus (chocolate ice cream in the mouth). Then the weekend comes. You don’t have to go to class, so you don’t pass the truck. Monday morning arrives and you take your usual route to campus. You round the corner and hear the truck again. What do you think happens? Your mouth begins to water again. Why? After a break from conditioning, the conditioned response reappears, which indicates spontaneous recovery.

Acquisition and extinction involve the strengthening and weakening, respectively, of a learned association. Two other learning processes—stimulus discrimination and stimulus generalization—are involved in distinguishing which stimuli will trigger the learned association. Animals (including humans) need to distinguish between stimuli—for example, between sounds that predict a threatening event and sounds that do not—so that they can respond appropriately (such as running away if the sound is threatening). When an organism learns to respond differently to various stimuli that are similar, it is called stimulus discrimination . In classical conditioning terms, the organism demonstrates the conditioned response only to the conditioned stimulus. Pavlov’s dogs discriminated between the basic tone that sounded before they were fed and other tones (e.g., the doorbell), because the other sounds did not predict the arrival of food. Similarly, Tiger, the cat, discriminated between the sound of the can opener and the sound of the electric mixer. When the electric mixer is going, Tiger is not about to be fed, so she does not come running to the kitchen looking for food.

On the other hand, when an organism demonstrates the conditioned response to stimuli that are similar to the condition stimulus, it is called stimulus generalization , the opposite of stimulus discrimination. The more similar a stimulus is to the condition stimulus, the more likely the organism is to give the conditioned response. For instance, if the electric mixer sounds very similar to the electric can opener, Tiger may come running after hearing its sound. But if you do not feed her following the electric mixer sound, and you continue to feed her consistently after the electric can opener sound, she will quickly learn to discriminate between the two sounds (provided they are sufficiently dissimilar that she can tell them apart).

Sometimes, classical conditioning can lead to habituation. Habituation occurs when we learn not to respond to a stimulus that is presented repeatedly without change. As the stimulus occurs over and over, we learn not to focus our attention on it. For example, imagine that your neighbor or roommate constantly has the television blaring. This background noise is distracting and makes it difficult for you to focus when you’re studying. However, over time, you become accustomed to the stimulus of the television noise, and eventually you hardly notice it any longer.

BEHAVIORISM

John B. Watson , shown in [link] , is considered the founder of behaviorism. Behaviorism is a school of thought that arose during the first part of the 20th century, which incorporates elements of Pavlov’s classical conditioning (Hunt, 2007). In stark contrast with Freud, who considered the reasons for behavior to be hidden in the unconscious, Watson championed the idea that all behavior can be studied as a simple stimulus-response reaction, without regard for internal processes. Watson argued that in order for psychology to become a legitimate science, it must shift its concern away from internal mental processes because mental processes cannot be seen or measured. Instead, he asserted that psychology must focus on outward observable behavior that can be measured.

A photograph shows John B. Watson.

Watson’s ideas were influenced by Pavlov’s work. According to Watson, human behavior, just like animal behavior, is primarily the result of conditioned responses. Whereas Pavlov’s work with dogs involved the conditioning of reflexes, Watson believed the same principles could be extended to the conditioning of human emotions (Watson, 1919). Thus began Watson’s work with his graduate student Rosalie Rayner and a baby called Little Albert. Through their experiments with Little Albert, Watson and Rayner (1920) demonstrated how fears can be conditioned.

In 1920, Watson was the chair of the psychology department at Johns Hopkins University. Through his position at the university he came to meet Little Albert’s mother, Arvilla Merritte, who worked at a campus hospital (DeAngelis, 2010). Watson offered her a dollar to allow her son to be the subject of his experiments in classical conditioning. Through these experiments, Little Albert was exposed to and conditioned to fear certain things. Initially he was presented with various neutral stimuli, including a rabbit, a dog, a monkey, masks, cotton wool, and a white rat. He was not afraid of any of these things. Then Watson, with the help of Rayner, conditioned Little Albert to associate these stimuli with an emotion—fear. For example, Watson handed Little Albert the white rat, and Little Albert enjoyed playing with it. Then Watson made a loud sound, by striking a hammer against a metal bar hanging behind Little Albert’s head, each time Little Albert touched the rat. Little Albert was frightened by the sound—demonstrating a reflexive fear of sudden loud noises—and began to cry. Watson repeatedly paired the loud sound with the white rat. Soon Little Albert became frightened by the white rat alone. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR? Days later, Little Albert demonstrated stimulus generalization—he became afraid of other furry things: a rabbit, a furry coat, and even a Santa Claus mask ( [link] ). Watson had succeeded in conditioning a fear response in Little Albert, thus demonstrating that emotions could become conditioned responses. It had been Watson’s intention to produce a phobia—a persistent, excessive fear of a specific object or situation— through conditioning alone, thus countering Freud’s view that phobias are caused by deep, hidden conflicts in the mind. However, there is no evidence that Little Albert experienced phobias in later years. Little Albert’s mother moved away, ending the experiment, and Little Albert himself died a few years later of unrelated causes. While Watson’s research provided new insight into conditioning, it would be considered unethical by today’s standards.

A photograph shows a man wearing a mask with a white beard; his face is close to a baby who is crawling away. A caption reads, “Now he fears even Santa Claus.”

View scenes from John Watson’s experiment in which Little Albert was conditioned to respond in fear to furry objects.

As you watch the video, look closely at Little Albert’s reactions and the manner in which Watson and Rayner present the stimuli before and after conditioning. Based on what you see, would you come to the same conclusions as the researchers?

Advertising executives are pros at applying the principles of associative learning. Think about the car commercials you have seen on television. Many of them feature an attractive model. By associating the model with the car being advertised, you come to see the car as being desirable (Cialdini, 2008). You may be asking yourself, does this advertising technique actually work? According to Cialdini (2008), men who viewed a car commercial that included an attractive model later rated the car as being faster, more appealing, and better designed than did men who viewed an advertisement for the same car minus the model.

Have you ever noticed how quickly advertisers cancel contracts with a famous athlete following a scandal? As far as the advertiser is concerned, that athlete is no longer associated with positive feelings; therefore, the athlete cannot be used as an unconditioned stimulus to condition the public to associate positive feelings (the unconditioned response) with their product (the conditioned stimulus).

Now that you are aware of how associative learning works, see if you can find examples of these types of advertisements on television, in magazines, or on the Internet.

Pavlov’s pioneering work with dogs contributed greatly to what we know about learning. His experiments explored the type of associative learning we now call classical conditioning. In classical conditioning, organisms learn to associate events that repeatedly happen together, and researchers study how a reflexive response to a stimulus can be mapped to a different stimulus—by training an association between the two stimuli. Pavlov’s experiments show how stimulus-response bonds are formed. Watson, the founder of behaviorism, was greatly influenced by Pavlov’s work. He tested humans by conditioning fear in an infant known as Little Albert. His findings suggest that classical conditioning can explain how some fears develop.

Review Questions

A stimulus that does not initially elicit a response in an organism is a(n) ________.

  • unconditioned stimulus
  • neutral stimulus
  • conditioned stimulus
  • unconditioned response

In Watson and Rayner’s experiments, Little Albert was conditioned to fear a white rat, and then he began to be afraid of other furry white objects. This demonstrates ________.

  • higher order conditioning
  • acquisition
  • stimulus discrimination
  • stimulus generalization

Extinction occurs when ________.

  • the conditioned stimulus is presented repeatedly without being paired with an unconditioned stimulus
  • the unconditioned stimulus is presented repeatedly without being paired with a conditioned stimulus
  • the neutral stimulus is presented repeatedly without being paired with an unconditioned stimulus
  • the neutral stimulus is presented repeatedly without being paired with a conditioned stimulus

In Pavlov’s work with dogs, the psychic secretions were ________.

  • unconditioned responses
  • conditioned responses
  • unconditioned stimuli
  • conditioned stimuli

Critical Thinking Questions

If the sound of your toaster popping up toast causes your mouth to water, what are the UCS, CS, and CR?

The food being toasted is the UCS; the sound of the toaster popping up is the CS; salivating to the sound of the toaster is the CR.

Explain how the processes of stimulus generalization and stimulus discrimination are considered opposites.

In stimulus generalization, an organism responds to new stimuli that are similar to the original conditioned stimulus. For example, a dog barks when the doorbell rings. He then barks when the oven timer dings because it sounds very similar to the doorbell. On the other hand, stimulus discrimination occurs when an organism learns a response to a specific stimulus, but does not respond the same way to new stimuli that are similar. In this case, the dog would bark when he hears the doorbell, but he would not bark when he hears the oven timer ding because they sound different; the dog is able to distinguish between the two sounds.

How does a neutral stimulus become a conditioned stimulus?

This occurs through the process of acquisition. A human or an animal learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus. During the acquisition phase, the neutral stimulus begins to elicit the conditioned response. The neutral stimulus is becoming the conditioned stimulus. At the end of the acquisition phase, learning has occurred and the neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus capable of eliciting the conditioned response by itself.

Personal Application Question

Can you think of an example in your life of how classical conditioning has produced a positive emotional response, such as happiness or excitement? How about a negative emotional response, such as fear, anxiety, or anger?

Creative Commons License

Share This Book

  • Increase Font Size
  • For educators
  • English (US)
  • English (India)
  • English (UK)
  • Greek Alphabet

Your solution’s ready to go!

Our expert help has broken down your problem into an easy-to-learn solution you can count on.

Question: In an experiment, laboratory rats were classically conditioned to get sick after drinking sweetened water that was paired with a drug that compromises immune response. These rats continued to die after the experiment because of the conditioned response to the sweetened water alone. Which conclusion can be drawn from this research?

The conclusion that can be drawn from this research is:

Classical Conditioning Can Induce Physiologic...

answer image blur

Not the question you’re looking for?

Post any question and get expert help quickly.

Pavlov’s Dogs Experiment and Pavlovian Conditioning Response

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

pavlov2 e1698175375621

Like many great scientific advances, Pavlovian conditioning (aka classical conditioning) was discovered accidentally. Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849–1936) was a physiologist, not a psychologist.

During the 1890s, Pavlov researched salivation in dogs in response to being fed. He inserted a small test tube into the cheek of each dog to measure saliva when the dogs were fed (with a powder made from meat).

Pavlov predicted the dogs would salivate in response to the food in front of them, but he noticed that his dogs would begin to salivate whenever they heard the footsteps of his assistant, who was bringing them the food.

When Pavlov discovered that any object or event that the dogs learned to associate with food (such as the lab assistant) would trigger the same response, he realized that he had made an important scientific discovery.

Accordingly, he devoted the rest of his career to studying this type of learning.

Pavlovian Conditioning: Theory of Learning

Pavlov’s theory of learning, known as classical conditioning, or Pavlovian conditioning, posits that behaviors can be learned through the association between different stimuli.

Classical conditioning (later developed by Watson, in 1913) involves learning to associate an unconditioned stimulus that already brings about a particular response (i.e., a reflex) with a new (conditioned) stimulus, so that the new stimulus brings about the same response.

Pavlov developed some rather unfriendly technical terms to describe this process:
  • Neutral Stimulus (NS) : A stimulus that initially does not elicit a particular response or reflex action. In other words, before any conditioning takes place, the neutral stimulus has no effect on the behavior or physiological response of interest. For example, in Pavlov’s experiment, the sound of a metronome was a neutral stimulus initially, as it did not cause the dogs to salivate.
  • Unconditioned Stimulus (UCS): This is a stimulus that naturally and automatically triggers a response without any learning needed. In Pavlov’s experiment, the food was the unconditioned stimulus as it automatically induced salivation in the dogs.
  • Conditioned Stimulus (CS): This is a previously neutral stimulus that, after being repeatedly associated with an unconditioned stimulus, comes to trigger a conditioned response. For instance, in Pavlov’s experiment, the metronome became a conditioned stimulus when the dogs learned to associate it with food.
  • Conditioned Response (CR): This is a learned response to the conditioned stimulus. It typically resembles the unconditioned response but is triggered by the conditioned stimulus instead of the unconditioned stimulus. In Pavlov’s experiment, salivating in response to the metronome was the conditioned response.
  • Unconditioned Response (UR): This is an automatic, innate reaction to an unconditioned stimulus. It does not require any learning. In Pavlov’s experiment, the dogs’ automatic salivation in response to the food is an example of an unconditioned response.

Pavlov’s Dog Experiment

Pavlov (1902) started from the idea that there are some things that a dog does not need to learn. For example, dogs don’t learn to salivate whenever they see food. This reflex is ‘hard-wired’ into the dog.

Pavlov showed that dogs could be conditioned to salivate at the sound of a bell if that sound was repeatedly presented at the same time that they were given food.

Pavlov’s studies of classical conditioning have become famous since his early work between 1890 and 1930. Classical conditioning is “classical” in that it is the first systematic study of the basic laws of learning (also known as conditioning).

Pavlov’s dogs were individually situated in secluded environments, secured within harnesses. A food bowl was positioned before them, and a device was employed to gauge the frequency of their salivary gland secretions.

The data from these measurements were systematically recorded onto a rotating drum, allowing Pavlov to meticulously monitor the rates of salivation throughout the course of the experiments.

First, the dogs were presented with the food, and they salivated. The food was the unconditioned stimulus and salivation was an unconditioned (innate) response. (i.e., a stimulus-response connection that required no learning).

Unconditioned Stimulus (Food) > Unconditioned Response (Salivate)

In his experiment, Pavlov used a metronome as his neutral stimulus. By itself, the metronome did not elicit a response from the dogs. 

Neutral Stimulus (Metronome) > No Response

Next, Pavlov began the conditioning procedure, whereby the clicking metronome was introduced just before he gave food to his dogs. After a number of repeats (trials) of this procedure, he presented the metronome on its own.

As you might expect, the sound of the clicking metronome on its own now caused an increase in salivation.

Conditioned Stimulus (Metronome) > Conditioned Response (Salivate)

So, the dog had learned an association between the metronome and the food, and a new behavior had been learned.

Because this response was learned (or conditioned), it is called a conditioned response (and also known as a Pavlovian response). The neutral stimulus has become a conditioned stimulus.

Pavlovs Dogs Experiment

Temporal contiguity

Pavlov found that for associations to be made, the two stimuli had to be presented close together in time (such as a bell).

He called this the law of temporal contiguity. If the time between the conditioned stimulus (bell) and the unconditioned stimulus (food) is too great, then learning will not occur.

‘Unconditioning’ through experimental extinction

In extinction, the conditioned stimulus (the bell) is repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus (the food).

Over time, the dog stops associating the sound of the bell with the food, and the conditioned response (salivation) weakens and eventually disappears.

In other words, the conditioned response is “unconditioned” or “extinguished.”

Spontaneous recovery

Pavlov noted the occurrence of “spontaneous recovery,” where the conditioned response can briefly reappear when the conditioned stimulus is presented after a rest period, even though the response has been extinguished.

This discovery added to the understanding of conditioning and extinction, indicating that these learned associations, while they can fade, are not completely forgotten.

Generalization

The principle of generalization suggests that after a subject has been conditioned to respond in a certain way to a specific stimulus, the subject will also respond in a similar manner to stimuli that are similar to the original one.

In Pavlov’s famous experiments with dogs, he found that after conditioning dogs to salivate at the sound of a bell (which was paired with food), the dogs would also salivate in response to similar sounds, like a buzzer.

This demonstrated the principle of generalization in classical conditioning.

However, the response tends to be more pronounced when the new stimulus closely resembles the original one used in conditioning.

This relationship between the similarity of the stimulus and the strength of the response is known as the generalization gradient.

This principle has been exemplified in research, including a study conducted by Meulders and colleagues in 2013.

Impact of Pavlov’s Research

Ivan Pavlov’s key contribution to psychology was the discovery of classical conditioning, demonstrating how learned associations between stimuli can influence behavior.

His work laid the foundation for behaviorism, influenced therapeutic techniques, and informed our understanding of learning and memory processes.

Behaviorism: Pavlov’s work laid the foundation for behaviorism , a major school of thought in psychology. The principles of classical conditioning have been used to explain a wide range of behaviors, from phobias to food aversions.

Therapy Techniques: Techniques based on classical conditioning, such as systematic desensitization and exposure therapy , have been developed to treat a variety of psychological disorders, including phobias and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

In these therapies, a conditioned response (such as fear) can be gradually “unlearned” by changing the association between a specific stimulus and its response.

  • Little Albert Experiment : The Little Albert experiment, conducted by John B. Watson in 1920, demonstrated that emotional responses could be classically conditioned in humans. A young child, “Little Albert,” was conditioned to fear a white rat, which generalized to similar objects. 

Educational Strategies: Educational strategies, like repetitive learning and rote memorization, can be seen as applications of the principles of classical conditioning. The repeated association between stimulus and response can help to reinforce learning.

Marketing and Advertising: Principles from Pavlov’s conditioning experiments are often used in advertising to build brand recognition and positive associations.

For instance, a brand may pair its product with appealing stimuli (like enjoyable music or attractive visuals) to create a positive emotional response in consumers, who then associate the product with it.

Critical Evaluation

Pavlovian conditioning is traditionally described as learning an association between a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS) and an unconditioned stimulus (US), such that the CS comes to elicit a conditioned response (CR). This fits many lab studies but misses the adaptive function of conditioning (Domjan, 2005).

From a functional perspective, conditioning likely evolves to help organisms effectively interact with biologically important unconditioned stimuli (US) in their natural environment.

For conditioning to happen naturally, the conditioned stimulus (CS) can’t be arbitrary, but must have a real ecological relationship to the US as a precursor or feature of the US object.

Pavlovian conditioning prepares organisms for important biological events by conditioning compensatory responses that improve the organism’s ability to cope.

The critical behavior change from conditioning may not be conditioned responses (CRs), but rather conditioned modifications of unconditioned responses (URs) to the US that improve the organism’s interactions with it.

Evidence shows conditioning occurs readily with naturalistic CSs, like tastes before illness, infant cues before nursing, prey sights before attack. This conditioning is more robust and resistant to effects like blocking.

Traditional descriptions of Pavlovian conditioning as simply the acquired ability of one stimulus to evoke the original response to another stimulus paired with it are inadequate and misleading (Rescorla, 1988).

New research shows conditioning is actually about learning relationships between events, which allows organisms to build mental representations of their environment.

Just pairing stimuli together doesn’t necessarily cause conditioning. It depends on whether one stimulus gives information about the other.

Conditioning rapidly encodes relations among a broad range of stimuli, not just between a neutral stimulus and one eliciting a response. The learned associations allow complex representations of the world.

Recently, Honey et al. (2020, 2022) presented simulations using an alternative model called HeiDI that accounts for Rescorla’s findings. HeiDI differs by allowing reciprocal CS-US and US-CS associations. It uses consistent learning rules applied to all stimulus pairs.

The simulations suggest HeiDI explains Rescorla’s results via two mechanisms:

  • Changes in US-CS associations during compound conditioning, allowing greater change in some US-CS links
  • Indirect influences of CS-CS associations enabling compounds to recruit associative strength from absent stimuli

HeiDI integrates various conditioning phenomena and retains key Rescorla-Wagner insights about surprise driving learning. However, it moves beyond the limitations of Rescorla-Wagner by providing a framework to address how learning translates into performance.

HeiDI refers to the authors of the model (Honey, Dwyer, Iliescu) as well as highlighting a key feature of the model – the bidirectional or reciprocal associations it proposes between conditioned stimuli and unconditioned stimuli.

H – Honey (the lead author’s surname), ei – Bidirectional (referring to the reciprocal associations), D – Dwyer (the second author’s surname), I – Iliescu (the third author’s surname).

  • Domjan, M. (2005). Pavlovian conditioning: A functional perspective.  Annu. Rev. Psychol. ,  56 , 179-206.
  • Honey, R.C., Dwyer, D.M., & Iliescu, A.F. (2020a). HeiDI: A model for Pavlovian learning and performance with reciprocal associations. Psychological Review, 127, 829-852.
  • Honey, R. C., Dwyer, D. M., & Iliescu, A. F. (2022). Associative change in Pavlovian conditioning: A reappraisal .  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition .
  • Meulders A, Vandebroek, N. Vervliet, B. and Vlaeyen, J.W.S. (2013). Generalization Gradients in Cued and Contextual Pain-Related Fear: An Experimental Study in Health Participants .  Frontiers in Human Neuroscience ,  7 (345). 1-12.
  • Pavlov, I. P. (1897/1902). The work of the digestive glands. London: Griffin.
  • Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflexes . (Translated by W.H. Gantt) London: Allen and Unwin.
  • Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex . Translated and edited by Anrep, GV (Oxford University Press, London, 1927).
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is .  American Psychologist ,  43 (3), 151.
  • Pavlov, I. P. (1955). Selected works . Moscow: Foreign Languages Publishing House.
  • Watson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist Views It. Psychological Review, 20 , 158-177.
  • Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions.  Journal of experimental psychology ,  3 (1), 1.

Further Reading

  • Logan, C. A. (2002). When scientific knowledge becomes scientific discovery: The disappearance of classical conditioning before Pavlov. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 38 (4), 393-403.
  • Learning and Behavior PowerPoint

What was the main point of Ivan Pavlov’s experiment with dogs?

The main point of Ivan Pavlov’s experiment with dogs was to study and demonstrate the concept of classical conditioning.

Pavlov showed that dogs could be conditioned to associate a neutral stimulus (such as a bell) with a reflexive response (such as salivation) by repeatedly pairing the two stimuli together.

This experiment highlighted the learning process through the association of stimuli and laid the foundation for understanding how behaviors can be modified through conditioning.

What is Pavlovian response?

The Pavlovian response, also known as a conditioned response, refers to a learned, automatic, and involuntary response elicited by a previously neutral stimulus through classical conditioning. It is a key concept in Pavlov’s experiments, where dogs learned to salivate in response to a bell.

When did Pavlov discover classical conditioning?

Ivan Pavlov discovered classical conditioning during his dog experiments in the late 1890s and early 1900s. His seminal work on classical conditioning, often called Pavlovian conditioning, laid the foundation for our understanding of associative learning and its role in behavior modification.

pavlovs dogs

In an Experiment, Laboratory Rats Were Classically Conditioned to Get

Question 103

In an experiment, laboratory rats were classically conditioned to get sick after drinking sweetened water that was paired with a drug that compromises immune response.These rats continued to die after the experiment because of the conditioned response to the sweetened water alone.Which conclusion can be drawn from this research?

A) Animals and humans can be classically conditioned to any neutral stimulus B) Any response the body is capable of making can be conditioned, including dying C) Classical conditioning is always automatic and cannot be avoided with strategies D) Classical conditioning works with voluntary responses as well

Correct Answer:

Unlock this answer now Get Access to more Verified Answers free of charge

Q98: Based on observational learning research, which of

Q99: Young Matthew is a very fussy eater.He

Q100: _ were influential in early studies of

Q101: Garcia and his colleagues taught coyotes to

Q102: Which of the following is an appropriate

Q104: Garcia's taste aversion studies demonstrated that Pavlov

Q105: There are some behaviors that cannot be

Q106: Chickens could not be taught to play

Q107: Biological preparedness suggests that we would be

Q108: According to your text, research has shown

Unlock this Answer For Free Now!

View this answer and more for free by performing one of the following actions

qr-code

Scan the QR code to install the App and get 2 free unlocks

upload documents

Unlock quizzes for free by uploading documents

Logo for

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

Classical Conditioning

Learning Objectives

By the end of this section, you will be able to:

  • Explain how classical conditioning occurs
  • Summarize the processes of acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery, generalization, and discrimination

Pavlov (1849–1936), a Russian scientist, performed extensive research on dogs and is best known for his experiments in classical  conditioning  (Figure 7.3). As we discussed briefly in the previous section,  classical conditioning  is a process by which we learn to associate stimuli and, consequently, to anticipate events.

A portrait shows Ivan Pavlov.

Pavlov came to his conclusions about how learning occurs completely by accident. Pavlov was a physiologist, not a psychologist. Physiologists study the life processes of organisms, from the molecular level to the level of cells, organ systems, and entire organisms. Pavlov’s area of interest was the digestive system (Hunt, 2007). In his studies with dogs, Pavlov measured the amount of saliva produced in response to various foods. Over time, Pavlov (1927) observed that the dogs began to salivate not only at the taste of food, but also at the sight of food, at the sight of an empty food bowl, and even at the sound of the laboratory assistants’ footsteps. Salivating to food in the mouth is reflexive, so no learning is involved. However, dogs don’t naturally salivate at the sight of an empty bowl or the sound of footsteps.

These unusual responses intrigued Pavlov, and he wondered what accounted for what he called the dogs’ “psychic secretions” (Pavlov, 1927). To explore this phenomenon in an objective manner, Pavlov designed a series of carefully controlled experiments to see which stimuli would cause the dogs to salivate. He was able to train the dogs to salivate in response to stimuli that clearly had nothing to do with food, such as the sound of a bell, a light, and a touch on the leg. Through his experiments, Pavlov realized that an organism has two types of responses to its environment: (1) unconditioned (unlearned) responses, or reflexes, and (2) conditioned (learned) responses.

In Pavlov’s experiments, the dogs salivated each time meat powder was presented to them. The meat powder in this situation was an  unconditioned stimulus (UCS) : a stimulus that elicits a reflexive response in an organism. The dogs’ salivation was an  unconditioned response (UCR) : a natural (unlearned) reaction to a given stimulus. Before conditioning, think of the dogs’ stimulus and response like this:

Meat powder (UCS)→Salivation (UCR)

In classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus is presented immediately before an unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov would sound a tone (like ringing a bell) and then give the dogs the meat powder (Figure 7.4). The tone was the  neutral stimulus (NS) , which is a stimulus that does not naturally elicit a response. Prior to conditioning, the dogs did not salivate when they just heard the tone because the tone had no association for the dogs.

Tone (NS) + Meat Powder (UCS)→Salivation (UCR)

When Pavlov paired the tone with the meat powder over and over again, the previously neutral stimulus (the tone) also began to elicit salivation from the dogs. Thus, the neutral stimulus became the  conditioned stimulus (CS) , which is a stimulus that elicits a response after repeatedly being paired with an unconditioned stimulus. Eventually, the dogs began to salivate to the tone alone, just as they previously had salivated at the sound of the assistants’ footsteps. The behaviour caused by the conditioned stimulus is called the  conditioned response (CR) . In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, they had learned to associate the tone (CS) with being fed, and they began to salivate (CR) in anticipation of food.

Tone (CS)→Salivation (CR)

Two illustrations are labeled “before conditioning” and show a dog salivating over a dish of food, and a dog not salivating while a bell is rung. An illustration labeled “during conditioning” shows a dog salivating over a bowl of food while a bell is rung. An illustration labeled “after conditioning” shows a dog salivating while a bell is rung.

Real World Application of Classical Conditioning

How does classical conditioning work in the real world? Consider the case of Moisha, who was diagnosed with cancer. When she received her first chemotherapy treatment, she vomited shortly after the chemicals were injected. In fact, every trip to the doctor for chemotherapy treatment shortly after the drugs were injected, she vomited. Moisha’s treatment was a success and her cancer went into remission. Now, when she visits her oncologist’s office every 6 months for a check-up, she becomes nauseous. In this case, the chemotherapy drugs are the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), vomiting is the unconditioned response (UCR), the doctor’s office is the conditioned stimulus (CS) after being paired with the UCS, and nausea is the conditioned response (CR). Let’s assume that the chemotherapy drugs that Moisha takes are given through a syringe injection. After entering the doctor’s office, Moisha sees a syringe, and then gets her medication. In addition to the doctor’s office, Moisha will learn to associate the syringe will the medication and will respond to syringes with nausea. This is an example of higher-order (or second-order) conditioning, when the conditioned stimulus (the doctor’s office) serves to condition another stimulus (the syringe). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if someone rang a bell every time Moisha received a syringe injection of chemotherapy drugs in the doctor’s office, Moisha likely will never get sick in response to the bell.

Consider another example of classical conditioning. Let’s say you have a cat named Tiger, who is quite spoiled. You keep her food in a separate cabinet, and you also have a special electric can opener that you use only to open cans of cat food. For every meal, Tiger hears the distinctive sound of the electric can opener (“zzhzhz”) and then gets her food. Tiger quickly learns that when she hears “zzhzhz” she is about to get fed. What do you think Tiger does when she hears the electric can opener? She will likely get excited and run to where you are preparing her food. This is an example of classical conditioning. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR?

What if the cabinet holding Tiger’s food becomes squeaky? In that case, Tiger hears “squeak” (the cabinet), “zzhzhz” (the electric can opener), and then she gets her food. Tiger will learn to get excited when she hears the “squeak” of the cabinet. Pairing a new neutral stimulus (“squeak”) with the conditioned stimulus (“zzhzhz”) is called  higher-order conditioning , or  second-order conditioning . This means you are using the conditioned stimulus of the can opener to condition another stimulus: the squeaky cabinet (Figure 7.5).

A diagram is labeled “Higher-Order / Second-Order Conditioning” and has three rows. The first row shows an electric can opener labeled “conditioned stimulus” followed by a plus sign and then a dish of food labeled “unconditioned stimulus,” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “unconditioned response.” The second row shows a squeaky cabinet door labeled “second-order stimulus” followed by a plus sign and then an electric can opener labeled “conditioned stimulus,” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “conditioned response.” The third row shows a squeaky cabinet door labeled “second-order stimulus” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “conditioned response.”

EVERYDAY CONNECTION: Classical Conditioning at Stingray City

A photograph shows a woman standing in the ocean holding a stingray.

Kate and her spouse recently vacationed in the Cayman Islands, and booked a boat tour to Stingray City, where they could feed and swim with the southern stingrays. The boat captain explained how the normally solitary stingrays have become accustomed to interacting with humans. About 40 years ago, fishermen began to clean fish and conch (unconditioned stimulus) at a particular sandbar near a barrier reef, and large numbers of stingrays would swim in to eat (unconditioned response) what the fishermen threw into the water; this continued for years. By the late 1980s, word of the large group of stingrays spread among scuba divers, who then started feeding them by hand. Over time, the southern stingrays in the area were classically conditioned much like Pavlov’s dogs. When they hear the sound of a boat engine (neutral stimulus that becomes a conditioned stimulus), they know that they will get to eat (conditioned response).

As soon as they reached Stingray City, over two dozen stingrays surrounded their tour boat. The couple slipped into the water with bags of squid, the stingrays’ favorite treat. The swarm of stingrays bumped and rubbed up against their legs like hungry cats (Figure 7.6). Kate was able to feed, pet, and even kiss (for luck) these amazing creatures. Then all the squid was gone, and so were the stingrays.

Classical conditioning also applies to humans, even babies. For example, Sara buys formula in blue canisters for her six-month-old daughter, Angelina. Whenever Sara takes out a formula container, Angelina gets excited, tries to reach toward the food, and most likely salivates. Why does Angelina get excited when she sees the formula canister? What are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR here?

So far, all of the examples have involved food, but classical conditioning extends beyond the basic need to be fed. Consider an example of a dog whose owners install an invisible electric dog fence. A small electrical shock (unconditioned stimulus) elicits discomfort (unconditioned response). When the unconditioned stimulus (shock) is paired with a neutral stimulus (the edge of a yard), the dog associates the discomfort (unconditioned response) with the edge of the yard (conditioned stimulus) and stays within the set boundaries. In this example, the edge of the yard elicits fear and anxiety in the dog. Fear and anxiety are the conditioned response.

General Processes in Classical Conditioning

Now that you know how classical conditioning works and have seen several examples, let’s take a look at some of the general processes involved. In classical conditioning, the initial period of learning is known as  acquisition , when an organism learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus. During acquisition, the neutral stimulus begins to elicit the conditioned response, and eventually the neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus capable of eliciting the conditioned response by itself. Timing is important for conditioning to occur. Typically, there should only be a brief interval between presentation of the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus. Depending on what is being conditioned, sometimes this interval is as little as five seconds (Chance, 2009). However, with other types of conditioning, the interval can be up to several hours.

Taste aversion  is a type of conditioning in which an interval of several hours may pass between the conditioned stimulus (something ingested) and the unconditioned stimulus (nausea or illness). Here’s how it works: Between classes, you and a friend grab a quick lunch from a food cart on campus. You share a dish of chicken curry and head off to your next class. A few hours later, you feel nauseous and become ill. Although your friend is fine and you determine that you have intestinal flu (the food is not the culprit), you’ve developed a taste aversion; the next time you are at a restaurant and someone orders curry, you immediately feel ill. While the chicken dish is not what made you sick, you are experiencing conditioned taste aversion: you’ve been conditioned to be averse to a food after a single, bad experience.

How does this occur—conditioning based on a single instance and involving an extended time lapse between the event and the negative stimulus? Research into taste aversion suggests that this response may be an evolutionary adaptation designed to help organisms quickly learn to avoid harmful foods (Garcia & Rusiniak, 1980; Garcia & Koelling, 1966). Not only may this contribute to species survival via natural selection, but it may also help us develop strategies for challenges such as helping cancer patients through the nausea induced by certain treatments (Holmes, 1993; Jacobsen et al., 1993; Hutton, Baracos, & Wismer, 2007; Skolin et al., 2006). Garcia and Koelling (1966) showed not only that taste aversions could be conditioned, but also that there were biological constraints to learning. In their study, separate groups of rats were conditioned to associate either a flavor with illness, or lights and sounds with illness. Results showed that all rats exposed to flavor-illness pairings learned to avoid the flavor, but none of the rats exposed to lights and sounds with illness learned to avoid lights or sounds. This added evidence to the idea that classical conditioning could contribute to species survival by helping organisms learn to avoid stimuli that posed real dangers to health and welfare.

Once we have established the connection between the unconditioned stimulus and the conditioned stimulus, how do we break that connection and get the dog, cat, or child to stop responding? In Tiger’s case, imagine what would happen if you stopped using the electric can opener for her food and began to use it only for human food. Now, Tiger would hear the can opener, but she would not get food. In classical conditioning terms, you would be giving the conditioned stimulus, but not the unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov explored this scenario in his experiments with dogs: sounding the tone without giving the dogs the meat powder. Soon the dogs stopped responding to the tone.  Extinction  is the decrease in the conditioned response when the unconditioned stimulus is no longer presented with the conditioned stimulus. When presented with the conditioned stimulus alone, the dog, cat, or other organism would show a weaker and weaker response, and finally no response. In classical conditioning terms, there is a gradual weakening and disappearance of the conditioned response.

What happens when learning is not used for a while—when what was learned lies dormant? As we just discussed, Pavlov found that when he repeatedly presented the bell (conditioned stimulus) without the meat powder (unconditioned stimulus), extinction occurred; the dogs stopped salivating to the bell. However, after a couple of hours of resting from this extinction training, the dogs again began to salivate when Pavlov rang the bell. What do you think would happen with Tiger’s behaviour if your electric can opener broke, and you did not use it for several months? When you finally got it fixed and started using it to open Tiger’s food again, Tiger would remember the association between the can opener and her food—she would get excited and run to the kitchen when she heard the sound. The behaviour of Pavlov’s dogs and Tiger illustrates a concept Pavlov called  spontaneous recovery : the return of a previously extinguished conditioned response following a rest period (Figure 7.7).

A chart has an x-axis labeled “time” and a y-axis labeled “strength of CR;” there are four columns of graphed data. The first column is labeled “acquisition (CS + UCS) and the line rises steeply from the bottom to the top. The second column is labeled “Extinction (CS alone)” and the line drops rapidly from the top to the bottom. The third column is labeled “Pause” and has no line. The fourth column has a line that begins midway and drops sharply to the bottom. At the point where the line begins, it is labeled “Spontaneous recovery of CR”; the halfway point on the line is labeled “Extinction (CS alone).”

Of course, these processes also apply in humans. For example, let’s say that every day when you walk to campus, an ice cream truck passes your route. Day after day, you hear the truck’s music (neutral stimulus), so you finally stop and purchase a chocolate ice cream bar. You take a bite (unconditioned stimulus) and then your mouth waters (unconditioned response). This initial period of learning is known as acquisition, when you begin to connect the neutral stimulus (the sound of the truck) and the unconditioned stimulus (the taste of the chocolate ice cream in your mouth). During acquisition, the conditioned response gets stronger and stronger through repeated pairings of the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. Several days (and ice cream bars) later, you notice that your mouth begins to water (conditioned response) as soon as you hear the truck’s musical jingle—even before you bite into the ice cream bar. Then one day you head down the street. You hear the truck’s music (conditioned stimulus), and your mouth waters (conditioned response). However, when you get to the truck, you discover that they are all out of ice cream. You leave disappointed. The next few days you pass by the truck and hear the music, but don’t stop to get an ice cream bar because you’re running late for class. You begin to salivate less and less when you hear the music, until by the end of the week, your mouth no longer waters when you hear the tune. This illustrates extinction. The conditioned response weakens when only the conditioned stimulus (the sound of the truck) is presented, without being followed by the unconditioned stimulus (chocolate ice cream in the mouth). Then the weekend comes. You don’t have to go to class, so you don’t pass the truck. Monday morning arrives and you take your usual route to campus. You round the corner and hear the truck again. What do you think happens? Your mouth begins to water again. Why? After a break from conditioning, the conditioned response reappears, which indicates spontaneous recovery.

Acquisition and extinction involve the strengthening and weakening, respectively, of a learned association. Two other learning processes—stimulus discrimination and stimulus generalization—are involved in determining which stimuli will trigger learned responses. Animals (including humans) need to distinguish between stimuli—for example, between sounds that predict a threatening event and sounds that do not—so that they can respond appropriately (such as running away if the sound is threatening). When an organism learns to respond differently to various stimuli that are similar, it is called  stimulus discrimination . In classical conditioning terms, the organism demonstrates the conditioned response only to the conditioned stimulus. Pavlov’s dogs discriminated between the basic tone that sounded before they were fed and other tones (e.g., the doorbell), because the other sounds did not predict the arrival of food. Similarly, Tiger, the cat, discriminated between the sound of the can opener and the sound of the electric mixer. When the electric mixer is going, Tiger is not about to be fed, so she does not come running to the kitchen looking for food. In our other example, Moisha, the cancer patient, discriminated between oncologists and other types of doctors. She learned not to feel ill when visiting doctors for other types of appointments, such as her annual physical.

On the other hand, when an organism demonstrates the conditioned response to stimuli that are similar to the condition stimulus, it is called  stimulus generalization , the opposite of stimulus discrimination. The more similar a stimulus is to the condition stimulus, the more likely the organism is to give the conditioned response. For instance, if the electric mixer sounds very similar to the electric can opener, Tiger may come running after hearing its sound. But if you do not feed her following the electric mixer sound, and you continue to feed her consistently after the electric can opener sound, she will quickly learn to discriminate between the two sounds (provided they are sufficiently dissimilar that she can tell them apart). In our other example, Moisha continued to feel ill whenever visiting other oncologists or other doctors in the same building as her oncologist.

Behaviorism

John B.  Watson , shown in Figure 7.8, is considered the founder of behaviourism. Behaviourism is a school of thought that arose during the first part of the 20th century, which incorporates elements of Pavlov’s classical conditioning (Hunt, 2007). In stark contrast with Freud, who considered the reasons for behaviour to be hidden in the unconscious, Watson championed the idea that all behaviour can be studied as a simple stimulus-response reaction, without regard for internal processes. Watson argued that in order for psychology to become a legitimate science, it must shift its concern away from internal mental processes because mental processes cannot be seen or measured. Instead, he asserted that psychology must focus on outward observable behaviour that can be measured.

A photograph shows John B. Watson.

Watson’s ideas were influenced by Pavlov’s work. According to Watson, human behaviour, just like animal behaviour, is primarily the result of conditioned responses. Whereas Pavlov’s work with dogs involved the conditioning of reflexes, Watson believed the same principles could be extended to the conditioning of human emotions (Watson, 1919). Thus began Watson’s work with his graduate student Rosalie Rayner and a baby called Little Albert. Through their experiments with Little Albert, Watson and Rayner (1920) demonstrated how fears can be conditioned.

In 1920, Watson was the chair of the psychology department at Johns Hopkins University. Through his position at the university he came to meet Little Albert’s mother, Arvilla Merritte, who worked at a campus hospital (DeAngelis, 2010). Watson offered her a dollar to allow her son to be the subject of his experiments in classical conditioning. Through these experiments, Little Albert was exposed to and conditioned to fear certain things. Initially he was presented with various neutral stimuli, including a rabbit, a dog, a monkey, masks, cotton wool, and a white rat. He was not afraid of any of these things. Then Watson, with the help of Rayner, conditioned Little Albert to associate these stimuli with an emotion—fear. For example, Watson handed Little Albert the white rat, and Little Albert enjoyed playing with it. Then Watson made a loud sound, by striking a hammer against a metal bar hanging behind Little Albert’s head, each time Little Albert touched the rat. Little Albert was frightened by the sound—demonstrating a reflexive fear of sudden loud noises—and began to cry. Watson repeatedly paired the loud sound with the white rat. Soon Little Albert became frightened by the white rat alone. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR? Days later, Little Albert demonstrated stimulus generalization—he became afraid of other furry things: a rabbit, a furry coat, and even a Santa Claus mask (Figure 7.9). Watson had succeeded in conditioning a fear response in Little Albert, thus demonstrating that emotions could become conditioned responses. It had been Watson’s intention to produce a phobia—a persistent, excessive fear of a specific object or situation— through conditioning alone, thus countering Freud’s view that phobias are caused by deep, hidden conflicts in the mind. However, there is no evidence that Little Albert experienced phobias in later years. Little Albert’s mother moved away, ending the experiment. While Watson’s research provided new insight into conditioning, it would be considered unethical by today’s standards.

A photograph shows a man wearing a mask with a white beard; his face is close to a baby who is crawling away. A caption reads, “Now he fears even Santa Claus.”

EVERYDAY CONNECTION: Advertising and Associative Learning

Advertising executives are pros at applying the principles of associative learning. Think about the car commercials you have seen on television. Many of them feature an attractive model. By associating the model with the car being advertised, you come to see the car as being desirable (Cialdini, 2008). You may be asking yourself, does this advertising technique actually work? According to Cialdini (2008), men who viewed a car commercial that included an attractive model later rated the car as being faster, more appealing, and better designed than did men who viewed an advertisement for the same car minus the model.

Have you ever noticed how quickly advertisers cancel contracts with a famous athlete following a scandal? As far as the advertiser is concerned, that athlete is no longer associated with positive feelings; therefore, the athlete cannot be used as an unconditioned stimulus to condition the public to associate positive feelings (the unconditioned response) with their product (the conditioned stimulus).

Now that you are aware of how associative learning works, see if you can find examples of these types of advertisements on television, in magazines, or on the Internet.

when an organism learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus

a type of conditioning in which an interval of several hours may pass between the conditioned stimulus (something ingested) and the unconditioned stimulus (nausea or illness)

the decrease in the conditioned response when the unconditioned stimulus is no longer presented with the conditioned stimulus

the return of a previously extinguished conditioned response following a rest period

When an organism learns to respond differently to various stimuli that are similar

when an organism demonstrates the conditioned response to stimuli that are similar to the condition stimulus

Introduction to Psychology Copyright © 2021 by Southern Alberta Institution of Technology (SAIT) is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 18 August 2022

Ecological analysis of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats

  • Peter R. Zambetti   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-3927-7720 1 ,
  • Bryan P. Schuessler 1 ,
  • Bryce E. Lecamp 2 ,
  • Andrew Shin   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-3903-1432 3 ,
  • Eun Joo Kim   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-8499-9135 1 &
  • Jeansok J. Kim   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-7964-106X 1  

Communications Biology volume  5 , Article number:  830 ( 2022 ) Cite this article

5298 Accesses

11 Citations

9 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Behavioural ecology
  • Learning and memory

Pavlovian fear conditioning, which offers the advantage of simplicity in both the control of conditional and unconditional stimuli (CS, US) presentation and the analysis of specific conditional and unconditional responses (CR, UR) in a controlled laboratory setting, has been the standard model in basic and translational fear research. Despite 100 years of experiments, the utility of fear conditioning has not been trans-situationally validated in real-life contexts. We thus investigated whether fear conditioning readily occurs and guides the animal’s future behavior in an ecologically-relevant environment. To do so, Long-Evans rats foraging for food in an open arena were presented with a tone CS paired with electric shock US to their dorsal neck/body that instinctively elicited escape UR to the safe nest. On subsequent test days, the tone-shock paired animals failed to exhibit fear CR to the CS. In contrast, animals that encountered a realistic agent of danger (a looming artificial owl) paired with a shock, simulating a plausible predatory strike, instantly fled to the nest when presented with a tone for the first time. These results highlight the possibility of a nonassociative, rather than standard associative, fear process providing survival function in life-threatening situations that animals are likely to encounter in nature.

Similar content being viewed by others

in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

Audible pain squeaks can mediate emotional contagion across pre-exposed rats with a potential effect of auto-conditioning

in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

Looming stimuli reliably drive innate defensive responses in male rats, but not learned defensive responses

in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

Multiple factors contribute to flight behaviors during fear conditioning

Introduction.

Since the time of Watson and Morgan’s 1 conception that emotions, such as fear, should be studied as conditional (acquired) reactions and Watson and Rayner’s 2 demonstration that fear can be rapidly learned in 9-month-old “Little Albert,” Pavlovian (or classical) fear conditioning has been the paradigm par excellence for studying both normal and abnormal fear behaviors 3 , 4 , 5 , 6 , 7 . Briefly, fear conditioning focuses on how an initially innocuous conditional stimulus (CS; e.g., auditory, visual, contextual cues), upon pairing with a noxious unconditional stimulus (US; usually electric shock) that reflexively elicits unconditional responses (UR; namely defensive reactions), becomes capable of eliciting conditional responses (CR; e.g., freezing in rodents, increased skin conductance in humans). A century of fear conditioning research has led to wide-ranging discoveries. In particular, fear conditioning experiments have fundamentally transformed learning theories from the archaic contiguity (or temporal) relationship 8 , 9 , 10 to the modern contingency (or informational) relationship between the CS and US 11 , 12 , 13 , 14 , revealed detailed neurobiological mechanisms of learning and memory 15 , 16 , 17 and influenced contemporary cognitive behavioral therapy for various anxiety and trauma-related disorders, such as panic, phobic and posttraumatic stress disorders 18 , 19 , 20 , 21 , 22 .

Despite the utility and appeal of fear conditioning paradigms, in particular the fact that conditional fear memory can transpire after a single CS-US pairing and be retained across the adult lifespan 23 , 24 , they nonetheless simplify behavioral analyses of fear, ignoring the multitude of actions and decisions that animals and humans utilize to survive the breadth of risky situations in the real world 25 , 26 , 27 , 28 , 29 , 30 . Indeed, standard rodent fear conditioning studies performed in small experimental chambers encapsulate Thorndike’s notion of studying unadulterated learning by placing animals in artificial situations that inhibit “instinctive activities (e.g., instinctive fears),” as instinctive behaviors may be opposite to learned behaviors in complex environments 31 . Hence, the prevalent notion that fear conditioning produces biologically functional associative fear memory needs to be ecologically validated. In fact, some researchers have questioned the evolutionary logic underlying fear conditioning; “No owl hoots or whistles 5 seconds before pouncing on a mouse…Nor will the owl give the mouse enough trials for the necessary learning to occur…What keeps animals alive in the wild is that they have very effective innate defensive reactions which occur when they encounter any kind of new or sudden stimulus” 32 . Consistent with this contrarian view are findings that laboratory rodents exhibit unlearned, instinctive fear responses to advancing artificial terrestrial and aerial predators 33 , 34 , overhead looming stimuli 35 , and predator odors 36 .

Here, we investigated for the first time to the best of our knowledge, whether fear conditioning readily transpires and modifies subsequent behavior of animals in a naturalistic environment. To achieve this, hunger-motivated rats searching for a food pellet in a large arena—that is, engaging in a purposive behavior as they would in nature 37 —were presented with a discrete tone CS followed by a painful US to their dorsal neck/body region by means of chronically implanted subcutaneous wires (Fig.  1a ). A dorsal neck/body shock better simulates real predatory strike compared to footshock used in standard fear conditioning studies, as it is unlikely that predators direct their attacks on small prey animal’s paws. In addition, in nature, bodily injuries are normally inflicted by external agents (namely, predators in animals and perpetrators in humans). Thus, other groups of rats were presented with a looming aerial predator (i.e., a lifelike great horned owl) preceded with and without a tone CS and followed by the same US (Fig.  1b–d ). A single trial tone-shock, tone-owl, tone/owl-shock, and owl-shock training was employed because multiple CS-US trial-and-error (rehearsal) learning, endangering the animal to repeated bodily harm, would prove fatal in nature and is antithetical to the natural selection of fear conditioning 23 , 24 , 32 . Later, all animals’ reactions to the tone cue were examined while foraging for food in the open arena. Because the dorsal neck/body shock US has never been used before in fear research, its efficacy to support a single trial tone fear conditioning was also examined in a standard conditioning chamber.

figure 1

a An illustration of a tethered rat foraging for a food pellet in the open arena (inset shows a headstage and placement of subcutaneous shock wires). b Timeline of experiment. Habituation: Rats were placed in a closed nest with dispersed food pellets for 30 min/day. Baseline: Rats were allowed to leave the nest to discover food pellets placed 25–125 cm (in 25 cm increments from the nest) in the foraging arena. Training: Animals approaching the pellet location experienced a delayed pairing of tone-shock (T-S), tone-owl (T-O), tone/owl-shock (T/O-S), or owl-shock (O-S). Tone Test: On subsequent days, all rats were placed back in the foraging arena and upon nearing the food pellet, the tone was activated. c Schemas of delayed pairings of stimuli. The T-S, T-O, and T/O-S (but not O-S) groups were presented with a tone 5 s before the gate opening that stayed on until the animals were within 25 cm of the food pellet, at which the tone co-terminated with the triggered shock (1 s), owl (1 s,) or owl-shock (100 ms interstimulus interval, ISI) stimuli. d A representative rat in the foraging arena (208 cm length × 66–120 cm expanding width × 61 cm height) during a baseline trial, where the animal successfully acquires the pellet, and during a T/O-S trial, where the animal flees from looming owl and shock into the nest (69 cm length × 58–66 cm width × 61 cm height).

Baseline foraging in an ethologically-relevant environment

Female and male rats were pseudo-randomly assigned to tone-shock (8 females, 8 males), owl-shock (8 females, 8 males), tone/owl-shock (6 females, 8 males), and tone-owl (4 females, 4 males) groups and implanted with subcutaneous wires in their dorsal neck/body (Fig.  1a–c ). After recovery from surgery and habituation to the nest compartment of the arena, the hunger-motivated rats were trained to exit the nest via a computer-controlled automated gateway to procure a sizable 0.5 g food pellet placed at variable distances in the large, expanding open area of the arena (Fig.  1d , top panel). Once the animals returned to the nest for pellet consumption, the gateway closed until the next trial (3 trials/day). On the first baseline day, female rats took a significantly longer amount of time to procure the food pellet compared to male rats (Supplementary Fig.  1 , Baseline day 1). This initial difference in foraging behavior likely represents heightened spatial neophobia (risk-averse to novel environments) in female rats. As rats became familiar with the foraging arena, the latency and duration measures declined across 5 baseline days comparably in both sexes, with no further statistical differences in latencies for pellet procurement. Because there were no reliable sex differences in subsequent fear conditioning dependent variables (Supplementary Fig.  2 and Supplementary Table  1 ), the four groups were collapsed across sexes.

Fear conditioning in an ethologically-relevant environment

On the training day, all rats first underwent three foraging trials with pellets fixed at the longest distance (125 cm) to confirm comparable pre-fear conditioning foraging behavior between groups (Fig.  2a , Baseline). Afterward, animals were exposed to a tone-shock, an owl-shock, a tone/owl-shock, or a tone-owl pairing in the manner shown in Fig.  1 (Supplementary Movie  1 ). Those rats presented with the tone CS 5-s prior to the gate opening (i.e., tone-shock, tone-owl, tone/owl-shock groups) took more time to enter the foraging arena in comparisons to owl-shock animals unexposed to the tone (Fig.  2b , Leave nest latency); this indicates that the tone was a salient cue that animals were attentive to and thus conditionable. Once in the foraging arena, all animals readily advanced toward the pellet and breached the trigger zone (25 cm from the pellet) to activate the shock, owl, or owl-shock stimuli (Fig.  2b , Trigger zone latency). In response to the shock, owl, or owl-shock, all rats promptly fled from the foraging arena to the nest (Fig.  2b , Escape latency; Fig.  2d, e , Escape speed). Figure  2c shows representative track plot examples of tone-shock, owl-shock, tone/owl-shock, and tone-owl animals successfully procuring the pellet during pre-tone baseline but not during tone conditioning. The fact that the escape latency and running speed were not significantly different between the tone-owl and other groups indicates that the looming owl-induced innate fear sans pain was just as effective in eliciting the flight UR as the painful shock or owl-shock combination. However, inspections of the escape trajectories revealed that the tone-shock and tone-owl groups tended to flee linearly to the nest, whereas the owl-shock and tone/owl-shock groups that experienced a dorsal neck/body shock 100 ms after the looming owl (mimicking realistic predatory attack) and begun their flight to the nest inclined to escape circuitously (Fig.  2f, h ). This was supported by significant group differences in the escape distances (Fig.  2g ) and variance of trajectory angles (Fig.  2i ), where owl-shock and tone/owl-shock groups traveled longer distances and had higher angle variances, respectively, during their escape routes than tone-shock and tone-owl groups.

figure 2

a Pre-conditioning baseline latencies (mean ± SEM) to procure food pellets in the foraging arena were equivalent between T-S (red), O-S (blue), T/O-S (dark gray), and T-O (light gray) groups (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 2.694, p  = 0.441). b During fear conditioning, the T-S, T/O-S, and T-O groups exposed to the tone 5 s before the gate opening had significantly longer latencies to leave the nest than the O-S group (left panel, Kruskal–Wallis, H = 18.6, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  = 0.008 for T-S vs. O-S, p  = 0.011 for O-S vs. T-O, p  < 0.001 for O-S vs. T/O-S, p  = 0.69 for T-S vs. T-O, p  = 0.631 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  = 0.343 for T/O-S vs. T-O). Once outside the nest, however, the latency to breach the trigger zone, enroute to the pellet, was not reliably different among the groups (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 7.453, p  = 0.059). In response to the triggered shock, owl or owl-shock, all groups showed similar escape-to-nest latencies (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 6.141, p  = 0.105). c Representative track plot examples from T-S, O-S, T/O-S, and T-O animals during the baseline, when animals successfully procured the pellet, and during the fear conditioning, when the same animals fled from shock, owl or owl-shock stimuli and thus unable to attain the pellet. d Mean instantaneous speed (±SEM) of each group 2 s before and after the shock, owl or owl-shock onset ( t  = 0). Thin, gray lines represent individual animal data. e All groups showed comparable escape speed to the shock, owl, and owl-shock stimuli (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 0.901, p  = 0.825). f Representative track plots showing escape paths of T-S, O-S, T/O-S, and T-O animals. The inset silhouette images show that the T-S and T-O animals were facing forward at the time of the shock or owl stimulus whereas the O-S and T/O-S animals were turning back at the time of the shock stimulus because of the 100 ms owl-shock interstimulus interval. g Mean escape distance (±SEM) from the trigger zone to the nest. The O-S and T/O-S groups traveled longer distances to escape compared to the T-S and T-O groups (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 21.98, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  = 0.014 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  = 0.008 for T/O-S vs T-O, p  = 0.001 for T-S vs. O-S, p  = 0.001 for O-S vs T-O). h Representative vector plots of each group showing variabilities in their escape paths. i Mean variance (±SEM) of escape trajectory angles (radian) from the trigger zone to the nest. The O-S and T/O-S groups had greater variance in their escape trajectories when fleeing back to the nest (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 22.37, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  = 0.022 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  = 0.003 for T/O-S vs T-O, p  = 0.002 for T-S vs. O-S, p  < 0.001 for O-S vs T-O) († compared to T-S, T/O-S, and T-O; * compared to O-S and T/O-S, p  < 0.05, ** p  < 0.01, *** p  < 0.001; # compared to T/O-S, p  < 0.05, ## p  < 0.01).

Context (pre-tone) testing in an ethologically-relevant environment

On the following day, animals were placed back in the nest and underwent three pre-tone baseline trials (maximum 300 s to retrieve the food pellet placed at 125 cm) to assess whether previous encounters with tone-shock, owl-shock, tone/owl-shock, and tone-owl stimuli combinations produced fear of the arena. As can be seen in Fig.  3a , the owl-shock and tone/owl-shock groups took significantly longer latencies to procure the pellet (i.e., the time from gate opening-to-return to nest with the pellet) than the tone-shock and tone-owl groups on the first day of testing. The lengthened times to enter the foraging arena exhibited by owl-shock and tone/owl-shock rats likely reflect inhibitory avoidance resulting from the previous predatory attack experience in the arena 38 . In contrast, the fact that the pre-tone test baseline latencies of tone-shock and tone-owl rats (Supplementary Fig.  3 ) were not reliably different from their baseline latencies from the fear conditioning day (prior to experiencing tone-shock or tone-owl) suggests that contextual fear conditioning failed to transpire in these animals despite their robust escape behavior to tone-shock and tone-owl experiences. Similar patterns of group differences, albeit lesser magnitudes, were observed on the second day of pre-tone baseline trials (Fig.  3c ).

figure 3

a The mean latency (±SEM) to procure the pellet during the pre-tone baseline trials on testing day 1 (D-1). Both O-S and T/O-S groups took significantly longer times to exit (gate opening, t  = 0) and return to the nest with the pellet than T-S and T-O groups (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 20.518, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, P  = 0.003 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  < 0.001 for T/O-S vs. T-O, p  = 0.013 for T-S vs. O-S, p  < 0.001 for O-S vs. T-O). b The times (mean ± SEM) to leave nest and reach trigger zone on day 1 tone test trials. Both O-S and T/O-S groups had longer latencies to leave nest (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 27.071, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  = 0.003 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  < 0.001 for T/O-S vs. T-O, p  = 0.044 for T-S vs. O-S, p  < 0.001 for O-S vs. T-O. Once outside the nest, the T/O-S group took longer time to reach the trigger zone than the T-S and T-O (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 9.153, p  = 0.027; pairwise comparisons, p  = 0.019 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  = 0.042 for T/O-S vs. T-O). During the tone test, the latencies to procure the pellet within the 60 s allotted time were significantly longer in O-S and T/O-S animals compared to T-S and T-O animals (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 34.428, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  < 0.001 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  < 0.001 for T/O-S vs. T-O, p  = 0.002 for T-S vs. O-S, p  < 0.001 for O-S vs. T-O). c The mean latency (±SEM) to procure the pellet during the pre-tone baseline trials on testing day 2 (D-2). O-S and T/O-S groups continued to have longer latencies to exit (gate opening, t  = 0) and return to the nest with the pellet than T-S and T-O groups (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 12.47, p  = 0.006; pairwise comparisons, p  = 0.022 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  = 0.002 for T/O-S vs. T-O, P  = 0.009 for O-S vs. T-O). d The times (mean ± SEM) to leave nest and reach trigger zone on day 2 tone test trials. There were group differences in the latencies to leave nest (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 21.505, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  = 0.001 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  < 0.001 for T/O-S vs. T-O, p  = 0.002 for O-S vs. T-O). Once outside the nest, there were group differences in the latencies to reach the trigger zone (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 21.531, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  < 0.001 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  < 0.001 for T/O-S vs. T-O, p  = 0.037 for O-S vs. T-O). During the tone test, the latencies to procure the pellet within the 60 s allotted time were significantly longer in O-S and T/O-S animals compared to T-S and T-O animals (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 37.223, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  < 0.001 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  < 0.001 for T/O-S vs. T-O, p  < 0.001 for T-S vs. O-S, p  < 0.001 for O-S vs. T-O). e Individual track plots from all animals from each group displaying the XY trajectory coordinates each rat took during the first tone exposure. The parenthesized numbers next to plots represent the trial(s) needed for successful foraging. f The overall success rates of procuring the pellet on the first testing day were significantly lower in the O-S and T/O-S groups compared to the T-S and T-O groups (Kruskal–Wallis, H = 32.299, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  < 0.001 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  < 0.001 for T/O-S vs. T-O, p  = 0.001 for T-S vs. O-S, p  = 0.003 for O-S vs. T-O). g The O-S and T/O-S animals required extended trials to obtain the pellet (Kruskal–Wallis, H =  = 32.004, p  < 0.001; pairwise comparisons, p  < 0.001 for T-S vs. T/O-S, p  < 0.001 for T/O-S vs. T-O, p  = 0.002 for T-S vs. O-S, p  = 0.011 for O-S vs. T-O). h In T-S and T/O-S animals, there were no reliable correlations (Spearman’s correlation coefficient) between the tone-induced suppression of pellet procurement (an index of fear) and the temporal intervals (i.e., ISIs) between tone CS onset and shock US onset in neither testing day 1 nor 2 (* compared to both O-S and T/O-S, p  < 0.05, ** p  < 0.01, *** p  < 0.001; # compared to T/O-S, p  < 0.05, p  < 0.01).

Tone testing in an ethologically-relevant environment

Immediately after the pre-tone baseline, all groups were subjected to three successive tone test trials (1 min apart). The owl-shock and tone/owl-shock animals continued to take longer latencies to exit the nest compared to tone-shock and tone-owl animals (Fig.  3b , Leave nest latency). Once in the foraging arena, the tone/owl-shock group’s latency to approach 25 cm from the pellet to trigger the tone were marginally but reliably longer than those of tone-shock and tone-owl groups, but not the owl-shock group (Fig.  3b , Trigger zone latency). Upon the activation of tone (60 s continuous), the majority of owl-shock and tone/owl-shock animals promptly fled to the nest (Supplementary Movie  2 ), thereby significantly increasing the latency to procure the pellet (60 s = unsuccessful), whereas the tone-shock and tone-owl animals were largely unaffected by the tone and readily procured the pellet (Fig.  3b , Procure pellet latency). No freezing (as measured by the ANY-maze tracking software with a 2 s threshold) was detected in the foraging arena during the tone presentations. The second day of tone testing yielded similar patterns of group differences (Fig.  3d ). Figure  3e shows individual track plots from all animals with the initial number of trial(s) necessitated for successful foraging. Further analyses across tone testing days (3 trials/day) showed that the overall success rates of procuring the pellet were significantly lower in owl-shock and tone/owl-shock groups compared to tone-shock and tone-owl groups (Fig.  3f ), and that owl-shock and tone/owl-shock animals required extended trials to reliably obtain the pellet (Fig.  3g ). Because the temporal interval between the CS and US is well known to be crucial in various types of Pavlovian conditioning, including fear conditioning 39 , we examined whether tone fear conditioning transpired in a specific (optimal) range of interstimulus intervals (ISI) but was masked by non-optimal ISIs. We found no significant correlation between the ISIs and the magnitudes of tone-induced suppression of pellet procurement in tone-shock animals, indicating that tone fear conditioning failed to materialize across varying ISIs of delay conditioning (Fig.  3h ). Conversely, in the tone/owl-shock animals, the tone-induced suppression of pellet procurement was uniformly observed across different ISIs, suggesting that the observed fear in these animals may not necessarily reflect Pavlovian conditioning (Fig.  3h ). Moreover, there were no significant differences between the owl-shock and tone/owl-shock animals on any of the measures (Supplementary Fig.  4 ). In particular, the latencies to pellet procurement during the 3rd and 4th tone tests were not correlated with the tone-shock intervals during training even though there was more variability within the tone/owl-shock group’s procurement times. The instantaneous speeds to the tone and angle trajectories of escape were also similar between the owl-shock and tone/owl-shock groups (Supplementary Fig.  5 ). These results suggest that there seems not to be an additional influence of Pavlovian memory in the tone/owl-shock group. The key results of delayed tone-shock paired animals failing to show conditional tone fear and contextual fear suggest that standard fear conditioning does not readily occur in a naturalistic environment. Instead, the finding of owl-shock animals displaying robust fear to a novel tone, which the animals never heard before, suggests that nonassociative processes play a crucial role in protecting animals in the real world.

Tone testing inside the nest

It is possible that tone fear conditioning transpired in the T-S group (Fig.  3 ) but conditional fear behavior was not observed because the tone CS was presented while the animals were in the large foraging arena. To address this, 8 experimentally naive rats (4 females, 4 males) underwent the identical surgical, food restriction, habituation, baseline foraging, and tone-shock conditioning procedures as described above (Fig.  1a, b ). After three pre-tone baseline trials, the tone CS was activated while the animals were inside the nest with the gateway closed. During the 60 s of continuous tone, none of the animals exhibited reliable freezing behavior (Supplementary Fig.  6a , left). When the gateway opened while the tone CS remained on, all rats readily entered the foraging arena and procured the pellet (Supplementary Fig.  6a , right). The animals then underwent 2 additional tone-shock pairings and were retested in the same manner. Even after a total of 3 tone-shock pairings, the tone CS failed to elicit freezing inside the nest with the gateway closed and inhibit/delay the latency to foraging when the gateway opened (Supplementary Fig.  6b ). These results further suggest that no associative learning to the tone CS was acquired in our naturalistic environment.

Fear conditioning in a standard chamber

To determine whether the absence of tone fear conditioning in a naturalistic environment (Fig.  3 ) was due to rats receiving subdermal pain to their dorsal neck/body region, as opposed to dermal pain to their paws in standard fear conditioning, 8 other experimentally naive rats (4 females and 4 males) that underwent the same aforementioned subcutaneous wire implant surgery, food restriction, habituation, and baseline foraging procedures were presented with a tone CS and dorsal neck/body shock US pairing in a standard conditioning chamber (Fig.  4a ). An additional 8 experimentally naive rats (4 females and 4 males), except for being ad lib -fed akin to most fear conditioning studies (e.g., refs. 38 , 39 , 40 ), underwent the same tone CS-dorsal neck/body shock US pairing. The fixed CS duration (24.1 s) employed was based on the mean CS duration of tone-shock animals in the naturalistic fear conditioning experiment (Fig.  3h ). Following the CS-US pairing, both restricted-food and ad lib -food animals exhibited reliable postshock freezing (fear conditioning day 1; Fig.  4b, e and Supplementary Fig.  7a, d ) and tone CS-elicited freezing in a contextually-altered chamber (tone testing day 2; Fig.  4c, d, f, g and Supplementary Fig.  7b, c, e, f ). There were no reliable group differences between restricted-food and ad lib -food animals in postshock freezing (Fig.  4b , 47.76 ± 4.24% vs. Fig.  4e , 52.1 ± 7.12%; independent t test, t (14) = −0.365, p  = 0.721) and tone CS-elicited freezing (Fig.  4d , 52.44 ± 4.66% vs. Fig.  4g , 46.04 ± 11.8%; independent t test, t (14) = 0.504, p  = 0.622). The fact that fear conditioning transpired with a single tone-shock pairing in a standard chamber comparably in restricted-food animals and ad lib -food animals suggests that the absence of conditioned tone-elicited fear in a naturalistic environment is unlikely due to attributes of tone CS and dorsal neck/body shock US (as opposed to a footshock) or due to sustained hunger motivation.

figure 4

a Illustrations of a rat implanted with wires subcutaneously in the dorsal neck/body region undergoing successive days of habituation (10 min tethered, conditioning chamber), training (a single tone CS-shock US pairing), and tone testing (context shift). (restricted-food) b Mean (crimson line) and individual (gray lines) percent freezing data from 8 rats (4 females, 4 males) during training in context A: 3 min baseline (BL1, BL2, BL3); 23.1 s epoch of tone (T); 1 min postshock (PS). c Mean and individual percent freezing data during tone testing in context B: 1 min baseline (BL1); 3 min tone (T1, T2, T3); 1 min post-tone (PT). d Mean ± SEM (bar) and individual (dots) percent freezing to tone CS before (Train, T) and after (Test, T1) undergoing auditory fear conditioning (paired t test; t (7) = −7.319, p  < 0.001). ( ad lib -food) e Mean (crimson line) and individual (gray lines) percent freezing data from 8 rats (4 females, 4 males) during training in context A: 3 min baseline (BL1, BL2, BL3); 23.1 s epoch of tone (T); 1 min postshock (PS). f Mean and individual percent freezing data during tone testing in context B: 1 min baseline (BL1); 3 min tone (T1, T2, T3); 1 min post-tone (PT). g Mean ± SEM (bar) and individual (dots) percent freezing to tone CS before (Train, T) and after (Test, T1) undergoing auditory fear conditioning (paired t test; t (7) = −3.188, p  = 0.015). * p  < 0.05, *** p  < 0.001.

It is generally believed (though never validated) that there is behavioral continuity of Pavlovian fear conditioning from the laboratory to real-life situations, and thus understanding the mechanisms of fear conditioning will have clinical relevance. The present study directly investigated whether fear conditioning readily occurs in naturalistic situations that animals are likely to encounter in their habitats. Standard fear conditioning in rodents takes place in small experimental chambers, and several studies have shown that a single tone CS-footshock US pairing (i.e., delay fear conditioning) reliably produces conditioned freezing in rats and conditioned tachycardia/freezing in mice 40 . One-trial delay tone fear conditioning has also been demonstrated in human subjects using a loud white noise US and assessing conditioned skin conductance response 41 . However, in the present study, where rats are exhibiting a purposive foraging behavior 37 in a large arena, a delayed pairing of tone CS and dorsal neck/body shock US (tone-shock group) produced virtually no evidence of auditory (and contextual) fear conditioning across a range of CS durations (i.e., ISIs). A similar pairing of tone CS and looming owl (tone-owl group) also failed to produce auditory fear conditioning despite the owl US evoking robust fleeing UR. In contrast, foraging rats that experienced a looming owl and shock pairing (owl-shock group) later exhibited robust fear (escape) behavior to a novel tone presentation. In the tone/owl-shock animals, the escape behavior was uniformly observed across different ISIs, suggesting that the observed fear to the tone CS in this group may also not be a Pavlovian response. These findings then point to a nonassociative process rather than associative tone fear memory, as playing a vital function in risky (i.e., predatory attack) situations that animals encounter in nature. Specifically, the owl-shock condition, where a novel tone prompted similar fleeing behavior caused by the owl-shock experience the previous day, may represent pseudo-conditioning, which refers to UR-like behavior emerging to a novel stimulus after mere exposure to a biologically significant US 42 , 43 . The observed contextual fear and subsequent fleeing to the novel tone in owl-shock (as well as tone/owl-shock) animals is also consistent with the finding that pseudo-conditioning transpires from conditioning of the context 44 .

The tone CS (3 kHz, 80 dB, ranging 9–86.6 s) and subcutaneous dorsal neck/body shock US (2.5 mA, 1 s) employed in the present study were effective in eliciting orienting and fleeing responses, respectively, and were presented to animals in the manner (i.e., a delay conditioning) that satisfied the stimuli saliency, intensity, surprising, and temporal contiguity requirements for conditioning 45 , 46 , 47 . Indeed, the same dorsal neck/body shock served as an effective US to generate one-trial tone fear conditioning in a standard (small) conditioning chamber in both restricted-food and ad lib -food animals. Then, what can account for one-trial auditory fear conditioning, demonstrated in standard Pavlovian paradigms in rats, mice, and humans 38 , 40 , 41 , 48 , not emerging in animals that left the safe nest to forage for food in an open arena? It may well be that rats are not biologically predisposed to associate discrete CS and US in natural (complex) environments where amalgamation of hunger-driven, fear-driven, and exploration-driven motivated behaviors are freely expressed. Indeed, in real-life, only a small minority of people experiencing trauma develop posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and even with re-exposure to the same trauma there is low incidence of PTSD 49 , 50 . In contrast, standard experimental chambers may be conducive to fear conditioning because they are simple and limit the repertoire of behavior 31 , effectively bypassing a “biological boundary” that prioritizes less costly defensive responses over trial-and-error learning mechanisms. The absence of one-trial fear conditioning in a naturalistic setting may be analogous to “The Rat Park Experiment,” where rats housed in an enriched environment with plants, trees, and social interaction resist drug addiction behavior evident in standard cage-housed rats 51 , 52 . Animals tested in naturalistic paradigms are given choices that do not force their behaviors into dichotomies (i.e., freezing or no freezing; drug craving or no drug craving). Allowing for an expanded behavioral repertoire, while more difficult to study, may thus yield a greater understanding of behaviors and their underlying brain mechanisms.

It should also be noted that fear encounters in real life generally occur in the presence of external agents or forms (i.e., predators/conspecifics in animals and assailants/combatants in humans), which is virtually nonexistent in standard Pavlovian fear conditioning paradigms. Thus, the effects of a discernable entity in associative fear learning have never been investigated. By simulating a realistic life-threatening situation, i.e., a looming aerial predator that instinctively elicited flight behavior followed by somatic pain, we found that rats engaged in purposive behavior likely utilize nonassociative pseudo-conditioning as their primary defensive mechanism. The fact that the owl-shock and tone/owl-shock animals exhibited relatively nonlinear, erratic escape trajectories to the nest compared to linear escape trajectories in tone-shock animals (Fig.  2f–i ) suggests the intriguing possibility that the same dorsal neck/body shock US may be interpreted as a life-or-death (panic) situation in the presence of an external threat agent versus a mere startling (nociceptive) situation in the absence of an external threat agent. The erratic flight behavior in the presence of a looming owl may represent the penultimate stage of circa-strike, or “life-or-death,” behavior within the “predatory imminence continuum” theory 53 . Functionally, a ‘sensitized’ fear system may intensify avoidance behavior, which in turn effectively transposes novel, neutral cues into “false positives” to prioritize survival in natural environments 32 . In other words, nonassociative process-based overestimation/generalization of danger may be a more prudent course for survival than associative process-based specific prediction of danger.

The present owl-shock-like procedure can perhaps be introduced in standard conditioning chambers outfitted with overhead monitors to produce two-dimensional (2D) looming stimuli (e.g., a rapidly expanding black disc) that can evoke freezing if the animal is distant from an enclosed shelter or fleeing if the animal is nearby an enclosed shelter 35 . In doing so, footshock can be delivered as the animals are either freezing or fleeing to the 2D looming disc to potentially investigate, for example, nonassociative aspects of fear mechanisms and whether the same predatory strike evokes differential defensive responses in animals are engaged in central amygdala-mediated freezing vs. basolateral amygdala-mediated active avoidance 54 , 55 . Incorporating external agents of danger into standard fear conditioning paradigms may lead to more realistic translational findings. However, it must also be considered that fear behaviors observed in an ecologically-relevant environment, where instinctive activities are unconstrained, might not be similarly observed in a standard operant chamber, where conditioning plays disproportionately dominant role over instinctive fear, and vice versa 31 .

Some caveats, however, must be considered in the present naturalistic study of fear conditioning. First, although neither the tone-shock group nor the tone-owl group showed overt manifestations of fear conditioning to the tone (as measured by fleeing or freezing in the arena) that prevented a successful procurement of food, the possibility of physiological (e.g., cardiovascular, respiratory) indices of fear 56 cannot be excluded in these animals. If so, the presence of tone-elicited fleeing and foraging termination behaviors in owl-shock and tone/owl-shock animals versus the absence of tone-elicited fleeing and foraging termination behaviors in tone-shock and tone-owl animals may reflect differences in the magnitude (rather than presence-absence) of fear conditioning. Second, the erratic escape trajectory behavior exhibited by owl-shock and tone/owl-shock animals may be indicative of rapid associative processes at work 57 . For example, the immediate-shock (and delayed shock-context shift) deficits in freezing, e.g., refs. 58 , 59 , provide compelling evidence that postshock freezing is not a UR but rather a CR to the contextual representation CS that rapidly became associated with the footshock US. In a similar vein then the erratic escape CR topography in owl-shock and tone/owl-shock animals might represent a shift in ‘functional CR topography’ 47 resulting from the rapid association between some salient features of the owl and the dorsal neck/body shock. A rapid owl-shock association nevertheless cannot explain the owl-shock animals’ subsequent fleeing behavior to a novel tone (in the absence of owl), which likely reflects nonassociative fear. Third, there are obvious procedural differences between standard fear conditioning versus naturalistic fear conditioning. In the former paradigm, typically ad libitum-fed animals are placed in an experimental chamber for a fixed time before receiving a CS-US pairing (irrespective of their ongoing behavior). Thus, the CS duration and ISI are constant across subjects. In our study, hunger-motivated rats searching for food must navigate to a fixed location in a large arena before experiencing a CS-US pairing (instrumental- or response-contingent). Because animals approach the US trigger zone at different latencies, the CS duration and ISI are variable across subjects. A more pertinent question is whether “procedurally pure” laboratory Pavlovian fear conditioning can possibly occur in real-world settings, where behaviors of animals and humans are largely purposive/goal-oriented 37 . Indeed, Bouton 43 articulated that, “Outside the laboratory, stimulus [Pavlovian] learning and response [Instrumental] learning are almost inseparable.” Last, tone fear conditioning might not have transpired in our foraging apparatus because the shock-induced pain was targeted to the dorsal neck/body region. As stated before, this is unlikely given that the same dorsal neck/body shock US effectively supported single trial tone fear conditioning in a standard conditioning chamber. Though predators would not direct their attacks underneath the paws of small prey animals, the possibility of a footshock US supporting tone fear conditioning in the foraging apparatus, however, cannot be excluded.

Clark Hull 60 has posited that Pavlovian fear conditioning offers biological utility by circumventing a “bad biological economy” of defense reaction always necessitating injury. This prevailing view that ascribes preeminent importance of fear conditioning as the primary defensive mechanism is likely to be a theoretical simplification and provides an incomplete picture of fear, as its function in a natural environment may be rather limited (i.e., lacks face validity). It may well be possible to produce fear conditioning in naturalistic settings with further CS-US trials, varying the CS and US intensity/duration or applying footshock but then this too would be a bad biological economy as such specific parameter-dependent learning would dramatically reduce biological fitness. It is also important to recognize inconsistencies in the literatures, such as clinical studies that have reported that patients with anxiety disorders, such as phobias, have trouble recalling the particular pairing of the fear event with its aversive consequences 61 , 62 . The increased utilization of naturalistic fear paradigms that simulate dangers that animals and humans encounter in real life will enable us to clarify, update, and revise fear concepts derived largely from fear conditioning studies and in doing so facilitate future progress in the treatment of fear disorders.

Eighty-six Long-Evans rats (3–4 months old; 44 females and 42 males, RRID:RGD_2308852), purchased from Charles-Rivers Laboratories, were initially pair-housed by sex for 5–7 days of acclimatization in a climate-controlled vivarium (accredited by the Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care), with a reversed 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7 PM). After undergoing subcutaneous wire implant surgery (described below), all animals were individually housed. Of 86 rats, 78 were placed on a standard restricted-food schedule with ad lib access to water to gradually reach and maintain ~85% normal body weight while the remaining 8 had ad lib access to both food and water. All experiments were performed during the dark phase of the cycle in strict compliance with the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines.

Under isoflurane anesthesia, rats were mounted on a stereotaxic instrument (Kopf), and two Teflon-coated stainless-steel wires (0.0003 inch bare, 0.0045 inch coated; A-M Systems, Everett, WA) were inserted in the dorsal neck/back region of body. The wire tips were exposed (~1 cm), bent to a V-shape, and hooked to subcutaneous tissue 39 . The other ends of the wires were affixed to a headstage (Plastics One, MS303-120), which was then cemented to the animal’s skull embedded with 6 anchoring screws. While still under anesthesia, animals were connected to a shock-apparatus and given a mild shock to observe muscle twitching; 6 rats that showed no reaction to shock were removed from the experiment. Animals were given 4 days of postoperative recovery and were adapted to handling for 5 days before nest habituation.

Foraging apparatus and stimuli

A custom-built foraging arena consisted of a nest (69 cm length × 58–66 cm width × 61 cm height) that opened via an automated sliding gate to reveal a large, expanded foraging area (208 cm length × 66–120 cm width × 61 cm height) where 0.5 g food pellets (grain-based; F0171, Bio-Serv) were placed at variable locations (Fig.  1a ). The testing room was kept under red light (11 lux foraging area, 2 lux nest area) with constant white noise (72 dB) playing in the background. Prior to placing each animal, the arena was wiped with 70% ethanol. The ANY-maze software and Ami interface system (Stoelting) connected to a PC automatically tracked the animal’s position in the arena, via a ceiling mounted camera, and triggered the tone, shock, and aerial predator stimuli: (i) 3 kHz, 80 dB tone CS (measured from the trigger location; 81 dB within the nest area) was produced using ANY-maze (Stoelting) and presented through two speakers mounted on the nest-foraging border; (ii) 1 s, 2.5 mA shock US was delivered to the animal’s dorsal neck/back region via a headstage tethered to a stimulus-isolator (Bak); (iii) A life-like model owl 34 , mounted onto a 92 cm pneumatic air cylinder (Bimba) at the opposite end of the foraging arena and hidden behind a black curtain, plunged downward towards the rat (46 cm/s), then retracted back to it starting position.

Behavioral procedure for naturalistic fear conditioning

A total of 62 rats (32 females and 30 males, all restricted-food) were used to investigate fear conditioning in an ecologically-relevant environment. Upon reaching and maintaining 85% normal body weight, animals were transported to the experimental room and underwent series of habituation, baseline, fear conditioning, and testing sessions.

Habituation days

Animals were placed in the nest scattered with 20 food pellets (0.5 g, grain-based, Bio-Serv) for 30 min/day for 2 consecutive days to acclimatize and associate the nest with food consumption.

Baseline days

After 1 min in the nest sans food pellets, the gate opened, and the animal was allowed to explore the large foraging arena and find a pellet placed 25 cm away from the nest (first trial). As soon as the animal took the sizeable 0.5 g pellet back to the nest, the gate closed. Once the animal finished eating, the second trial with the pellet placed 50 cm and then the third trial with the pellet placed 75 cm commenced in the same manner. Animals underwent 3–5 consecutive baseline days, with the pellet distances gradually extending to 75, 100, and 125 cm, and they were also accustomed to tethering beginning on baseline day 3 onward.

Fear conditioning day

Rats, pseudo-randomly assigned into tone-shock, tone-owl, tone/owl-shock, and owl-shock groups (Fig.  1 ), underwent 3 baseline trials with the pellet placed at 125 cm from the nest. On the 4th trial, the tone-shock, tone-owl, and tone/owl-shock animals were exposed to a tone CS that came on 5 s before the gate opened and remained on until they reached the trigger zone (25 cm to the pellet). For tone-shock and tone-owl animals, the tone co-terminated with the shock US and the owl looming, respectively. For tone/owl-shock animals, the shock occurred 0.1 s after the owl looming and co-terminated with the tone. Two animals in the tone/owl-shock group were excluded because they failed to leave the nest within 2 min. The owl-shock animals were subjected to the same owl looming-shock pairing (as the tone/owl-shock animals) but in the absence of tone. All rats fled to the nest in reaction to the shock and/or looming owl, at which time the gate was closed. After 1 min in the nest, the animals were placed back into their homecage.

Testing days

All rats underwent 3 baseline trials (a maximum of 300 s to retrieve the pellet) to assess whether shock and/or looming owl encounter the previous day resulted in the fear of the arena (i.e., contextual fear). Afterward, animals were presented with the tone cue when they approached the trigger zone (25 cm to the pellet). The tone played continuously for 60 s, after which the tone test trial ended. Animals underwent 3 tone tests daily until they successfully attained the pellet (i.e., fear extinction).

Behavioral procedure for tone testing inside the nest

Another tone-shock group of 8 rats (4 female, 4 males; restricted-food) underwent the same Habituation, Baseline, and Fear conditioning procedures described above. On the testing day, after 3 baseline trials (a maximum of 300 s to retrieve the pellet), while the animals were inside the nest with the gateway closed, the tone CS was activated continuously, and freezing behavior was measured for 60 s (freezing analysis described below). Then, while the tone remained on, the gateway opened to assess the latency to procure the pellet. Once the animals returned to the nest with the pellet, the tone CS terminated.

Behavioral procedure for standard fear conditioning

The remaining 16 rats (4 females and 4 males, restricted-food; 4 females and 4 males, ad lib -food) were subjected to one-trial tone fear conditioning in a standard conditioning chamber 38 instead of an ecologically-relevant foraging arena. The restricted-food animals underwent the aforementioned Habituation and Baseline procedures prior to standard fear conditioning, whereas the ad lib -food animals proceeded to standard fear conditioning directly (similar to most fear conditioning studies). A day prior to fear conditioning (day 0), both restricted-food and ad lib -food animals were tethered and placed in an experimental chamber for 10 mi of pre-exposure. Fear conditioning (day 1) commenced after 3 min of baseline in the chamber by exposing animals to 24.1 s tone CS (3 kHz, 80 dB) that co-terminated with 1 s dorsal neck/body shock (2.5 mA). The 24.1 s tone was based on the mean tone CS duration from the tone-shock group of the naturalistic fear conditioning experiment. Postshock freezing was assessed for 1 min before animals were removed from the conditioning chamber. For tone test (day 2), animals were placed in a novel chamber that differed in terms of the wall pattern, floor texture, background light, and smell 63 . After 1 min of baseline, the tone was presented for 3 min to assess CS-evoked freezing response, and the animals were left in the chamber for an additional minute before being placed back in their homecage. Freezing was again quantified using ANY-maze (Stoelting) tracking software with the freezing threshold set to 2 s.

Statistics and reproducibility

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (IBM, version 19) and R (The R Foundation, version 3.5.3). Body tracking positions were obtained using Deep Lab Cut 64 and analyzed using a self-written script in Python (Python Software Foundation). Animal sample sizes were determined using a power analysis performed by G*Power (G*Power, version 3.0.1, Franz Faul; power = 0.95, alpha = 0.05, effect size = 0.5, two-tailed). A Levene’s test for normality showed significance for the data, thus nonparametric tests were used for analyses. Because there were no significant sex differences in any stages of the experiment after the first day of baseline (Supplementary Fig.  1 and Supplementary Table  1 ), data from females and males were pooled together for all analyses (Supplementary Fig.  2 ). Statistical significance was set at P  < 0.05. Graphs were made using GraphPad Prism (version 8).

For the analyses of escape trajectories (Fig.  2h, i ), the coordinate data of each rat in the foraging arena taken at a frequency of 10 Hz was used to obtain the change in position vectors between each time point (black) and an overall change in position vector (red). To obtain the individual change in position vectors, we used Python and the Numpy, Pandas, and Matplotlib packages to calculate the changes in x and y position between coordinates. With each change in x and y positions, we were able to calculate the magnitude of the distance traveled and the angle of travel using an inverse tangent function. The resultant vector representing the average change in position vector was determined by taking the average change in x position and average change in y position to calculate an overall magnitude and angle. The (population) variance and standard deviation of the angles of the change in position vectors were obtained using Numpy.

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the  Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study and the relevant analysis code are available from the Dryad data repository. https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.76hdr7sxk .

Watson, J. B. & Morgan, J. J. B. Emotional reactions and psychological experimentation. Am. J. Psychol. 28 , 163–174 (1917).

Article   Google Scholar  

Watson, J. B. & Rayner, R. Conditioned emotional reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 3 , 1–14 (1920).

LeDoux, J. Fear and the brain: where have we been, and where are we going. Biol. Psychiatry 44 , 1229–1238 (1998).

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Fendt, M. & Fanselow, M. S. The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23 , 743–760 (1999).

Maren, S. & Quirk, G. J. Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5 , 844–852 (2004).

Bouton, M. E., Mineka, S. & Barlow, D. H. A modern learning theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder. Psychol. Rev. 108 , 4–32 (2001).

Kim, J. J. & Jung, M. W. Neural circuits and mechanisms involved in Pavlovian fear conditioning: a critical review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30 , 188–202 (2006).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Watson, J. B. Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Rev. 20 , 158–177 (1913).

Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex (Oxford University Press, 1927).

Guthrie, E. R. Conditioning as a principle of learning. Psychological Rev. 37 , 412–428 (1930).

Kamin, L. J. in Miami Symposium on the Prediction of Behavior (ed. Jones, M. R.) 9–33 (University of Miami Press, 1968).

Rescorla, R. A. Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 66 , 1–5 (1968).

Wagner, A. R., Logan, F. A., Haberlandt, K. & Price, T. Stimulus selection in animal discrimination learning. J. Exp. Psychol. 76 , 171–180 (1968).

Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. A Theory of Pavlovian Conditioning: Variations in the Effectiveness of Reinforcement and Nonreinforcement 64–99 (Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972).

Josselyn, S. A. & Tonegawa, S. Memory engrams: recalling the past and imagining the future. Science 367 , https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaw4325 (2020).

Tovote, P., Fadok, J. P. & Luthi, A. Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16 , 317–331 (2015).

Haubensak, W. et al. Genetic dissection of an amygdala microcircuit that gates conditioned fear. Nature 468 , 270–276 (2010).

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Foa, E. B. & Rothbaum, B. O. Treating the Trauma of Rape: Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for PTSD (Guilford Press, 1998).

Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M. & Beck, A. T. The empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26 , 17–31 (2006).

Delgado, M. R., Olsson, A. & Phelps, E. A. Extending animal models of fear conditioning to humans. Biol. Psychol. 73 , 39–48 (2006).

Mahan, A. L. & Ressler, K. J. Fear conditioning, synaptic plasticity and the amygdala: implications for posttraumatic stress disorder. Trends Neurosci. 35 , 24–35 (2012).

Craske, M. G. et al. What is an anxiety disorder? Focus 9 , 20 (2011).

Google Scholar  

LeDoux, J. E. The Emotional Brain: the Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life (Simon & Schuster, 1996).

Fanselow, M. S. From contextual fear to a dynamic view of memory systems. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14 , 7–15 (2010).

Lima, S. L. & Dill, L. M. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation—a review and prospectus. Can. J. Zool. 68 , 619–640 (1990).

Bednekoff, P. A. Foraging in the Face of Danger 305–329 (University of Chicago Press, 2007).

Stephens, D. W. Decision ecology: foraging and the ecology of animal decision making. Cogn. Affect Behav. Neurosci. 8 , 475–484 (2008).

Beckers, T., Krypotos, A. M., Boddez, Y., Effting, M. & Kindt, M. What's wrong with fear conditioning? Biol. Psychol. 92 , 90–96 (2013).

Mobbs, D. & Kim, J. J. Neuroethological studies of fear, anxiety, and risky decision-making in rodents and humans. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 5 , 8–15 (2015).

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Pellman, B. A. & Kim, J. J. What can ethobehavioral studies tell us about the Brain's fear system. Trends Neurosci. 39 , 420–431 (2016).

Thorndike, E. Biological Lectures from the Marine Laboratory at Woods' Holl, USA, for 1899. Nature 62 , 411 (1900).

Bolles, R. C. Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychol. Rev. 77 , 32–48 (1970).

Choi, J. S. & Kim, J. J. Amygdala regulates risk of predation in rats foraging in a dynamic fear environment. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107 , 21773–21777 (2010).

Zambetti, P. R., Schuessler, B. P. & Kim, J. J. Sex differences in foraging rats to naturalistic aerial predator stimuli. iScience 16 , 442–452 (2019).

Yilmaz, M. & Meister, M. Rapid innate defensive responses of mice to looming visual stimuli. Curr. Biol. 23 , 2011–2015 (2013).

Papes, F., Logan, D. W. & Stowers, L. The vomeronasal organ mediates interspecies defensive behaviors through detection of protein pheromone homologs. Cell 141 , 692–703 (2010).

Tolman, E. C. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 55 , 189–208 (1948).

Wilensky, A. E., Schafe, G. E. & LeDoux, J. E. The amygdala modulates memory consolidation of fear-motivated inhibitory avoidance learning but not classical fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 20 , 7059–7066 (2000).

Lee, T. & Kim, J. J. Differential effects of cerebellar, amygdalar, and hippocampal lesions on classical eyeblink conditioning in rats. J. Neurosci. 24 , 3242–3250 (2004).

Stiedl, O. & Spiess, J. Effect of tone-dependent fear conditioning on heart rate and behavior of C57BL/6N mice. Behav. Neurosci. 111 , 703–711 (1997).

Guimaraes, F. S., Hellewell, J., Hensman, R., Wang, M. & Deakin, J. F. Characterization of a psychophysiological model of classical fear conditioning in healthy volunteers: influence of gender, instruction, personality and placebo. Psychopharmacology 104 , 231–236 (1991).

Mackintosh, N. J. The Psychology of Animal Learning (Academic Press, 1974).

Bouton, M. E. Learning and Behavior (Sinauer Associates 2007).

Sheafor, P. J. "Pseudoconditioned" jaw movements of the rabbit reflect associations conditioned to contextual background cues. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process 1 , 245–260 (1975).

Rescorla, R. A. Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 11 , 329–352 (1988).

Thompson, R. F. & Krupa, D. J. Organization of memory traces in the mammalian brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 17 , 519–549 (1994).

Fanselow, M. S. & Wassum, K. M. The origins and organization of vertebrate pavlovian conditioning. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 8 , a021717 (2015).

Lee, H. J., Berger, S. Y., Stiedl, O., Spiess, J. & Kim, J. J. Post-training injections of catecholaminergic drugs do not modulate fear conditioning in rats and mice. Neurosci. Lett. 303 , 123–126 (2001).

Palgi, Y., Gelkopf, M. & Berger, R. The inoculating role of previous exposure to potentially traumatic life events on coping with prolonged exposure to rocket attacks: a lifespan perspective. Psychiatry Res. 227 , 296–301 (2015).

Somer, E. et al. Israeli civilians under heavy bombardment: prediction of the severity of post-traumatic symptoms. Prehosp. Disaster Med. 24 , 389–394 (2009).

Alexander, B. K., Beyerstein, B. L., Hadaway, P. F. & Coambs, R. B. Effect of early and later colony housing on oral ingestion of morphine in rats. Pharm. Biochem. Behav. 15 , 571–576 (1981).

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Gage, S. H. & Sumnall, H. R. Rat Park: how a rat paradise changed the narrative of addiction. Addiction 114 , 917–922 (2019).

Fanselow, M. S. & Lester, L. S. A Functional Behavioristic Approach to Aversively Motivated Behavior: Predatory Imminence as a Determinant of the Topography of Defensive Behavior 185–212 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc, 1988).

Cain, C. & LeDoux, J. Brain mechanisms of Pavlovian and instrumental aversive conditioning. Handb. Behav. Neurosci. 17 , 103–124 (2008).

Choi, J. S., Cain, C. K. & LeDoux, J. E. The role of amygdala nuclei in the expression of auditory signaled two-way active avoidance in rats. Learn Mem. 17 , 139–147 (2014).

Steimer, T. The biology of fear- and anxiety-related behaviors. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 4 , 231–249 (2002).

Fanselow, M. S. The role of learning in threat imminence and defensive behaviors. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 24 , 44–49 (2018).

Fanselow, M. S. Associative vs topographical accounts of the immediate shock freezing deficit in rats—implications for the response selection-rules governing species-specific defensive reactions. Learn. Motiv. 17 , 16–39 (1986).

Landeira-Fernandez, J., DeCola, J. P., Kim, J. J. & Fanselow, M. S. Immediate shock deficit in fear conditioning: effects of shock manipulations. Behav. Neurosci. 120 , 873–879 (2006).

Hull, C. L. A functional interpretation of the conditioned reflex. Psychol. Rev. 36 , 498–511 (1929).

Lazarus, A. A. Behavior Therapy and Beyond (McGraw-Hill Companies, 1971).

Öhman, A. & Mineka, S. Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108 , 483–522 (2001).

Lee, H. & Kim, J. J. Amygdalar NMDA receptors are critical for new fear learning in previously fear-conditioned rats. J. Neurosci. 18 , 8444–8454 (1998).

Mathis, A. et al. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat. Neurosci. 21 , 1281–1289 (2018).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Lori A. Zoellner for valuable comments on the manuscript, and Heather Wu for assistance in the experiment. This study was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant MH099073 (to J.J.K.).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, 98195, USA

Peter R. Zambetti, Bryan P. Schuessler, Eun Joo Kim & Jeansok J. Kim

Undergraduate Program in Neuroscience, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, 98195, USA

Bryce E. Lecamp

Undergraduate Program in Human Biology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA

Andrew Shin

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

P.R.Z., E.J.K., and J.J.K. designed the study; P.R.Z. and B.E.L. collected the data; P.R.Z., B.P.S., and A.S. conducted the statistical analyses; P.R.Z., B.P.S., E.J.K., and J.J.K. wrote the manuscript. J.J.K. supervised all aspects of the study.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jeansok J. Kim .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information.

Communications Biology thanks Ki Goosens and Christopher Cain for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: Daniel Bendor and Luke R. Grinham.  Peer reviewer reports are available.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Peer review file, supplementary information, description of additional supplementary files, supplementary movie 1, supplementary movie 2, reporting summary, rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Zambetti, P.R., Schuessler, B.P., Lecamp, B.E. et al. Ecological analysis of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Commun Biol 5 , 830 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-03802-1

Download citation

Received : 09 February 2022

Accepted : 04 August 2022

Published : 18 August 2022

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-03802-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

A “head-like” component of a terrestrial robot promotes anxiety-like and defensive behaviors.

  • Ji Hoon Jeong
  • Kyeong Im Jo
  • June-Seek Choi

Intelligent Service Robotics (2024)

MK-801 and cognitive functions: Investigating the behavioral effects of a non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist

  • Klaudia Lustyk
  • Karolina Pytka

Psychopharmacology (2023)

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines . If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

6. Learning

Classical conditioning, learning objectives.

By the end of this section, you will be able to:

  • Explain how classical conditioning occurs
  • Summarize the processes of acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery, generalization, and discrimination

Does the name Ivan Pavlov ring a bell? Even if you are new to the study of psychology, chances are that you have heard of Pavlov and his famous dogs.

Pavlov (1849–1936), a Russian scientist, performed extensive research on dogs and is best known for his experiments in classical conditioning ( [link] ). As we discussed briefly in the previous section, classical conditioning is a process by which we learn to associate stimuli and, consequently, to anticipate events.

A portrait shows Ivan Pavlov.

Ivan Pavlov’s research on the digestive system of dogs unexpectedly led to his discovery of the learning process now known as classical conditioning.

Pavlov came to his conclusions about how learning occurs completely by accident. Pavlov was a physiologist, not a psychologist. Physiologists study the life processes of organisms, from the molecular level to the level of cells, organ systems, and entire organisms. Pavlov’s area of interest was the digestive system (Hunt, 2007). In his studies with dogs, Pavlov surgically implanted tubes inside dogs’ cheeks to collect saliva. He then measured the amount of saliva produced in response to various foods. Over time, Pavlov (1927) observed that the dogs began to salivate not only at the taste of food, but also at the sight of food, at the sight of an empty food bowl, and even at the sound of the laboratory assistants’ footsteps. Salivating to food in the mouth is reflexive, so no learning is involved. However, dogs don’t naturally salivate at the sight of an empty bowl or the sound of footsteps.

These unusual responses intrigued Pavlov, and he wondered what accounted for what he called the dogs’ “psychic secretions” (Pavlov, 1927). To explore this phenomenon in an objective manner, Pavlov designed a series of carefully controlled experiments to see which stimuli would cause the dogs to salivate. He was able to train the dogs to salivate in response to stimuli that clearly had nothing to do with food, such as the sound of a bell, a light, and a touch on the leg. Through his experiments, Pavlov realized that an organism has two types of responses to its environment: (1) unconditioned (unlearned) responses, or reflexes, and (2) conditioned (learned) responses.

In Pavlov’s experiments, the dogs salivated each time meat powder was presented to them. The meat powder in this situation was an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) : a stimulus that elicits a reflexive response in an organism. The dogs’ salivation was an unconditioned response (UCR) : a natural (unlearned) reaction to a given stimulus. Before conditioning, think of the dogs’ stimulus and response like this:

In classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus is presented immediately before an unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov would sound a tone (like ringing a bell) and then give the dogs the meat powder ( [link] ). The tone was the neutral stimulus (NS) , which is a stimulus that does not naturally elicit a response. Prior to conditioning, the dogs did not salivate when they just heard the tone because the tone had no association for the dogs. Quite simply this pairing means:

When Pavlov paired the tone with the meat powder over and over again, the previously neutral stimulus (the tone) also began to elicit salivation from the dogs. Thus, the neutral stimulus became the conditioned stimulus (CS) , which is a stimulus that elicits a response after repeatedly being paired with an unconditioned stimulus. Eventually, the dogs began to salivate to the tone alone, just as they previously had salivated at the sound of the assistants’ footsteps. The behavior caused by the conditioned stimulus is called the conditioned response (CR) . In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, they had learned to associate the tone (CS) with being fed, and they began to salivate (CR) in anticipation of food.

Two illustrations are labeled “before conditioning” and show a dog salivating over a dish of food, and a dog not salivating while a bell is rung. An illustration labeled “during conditioning” shows a dog salivating over a bowl of food while a bell is rung. An illustration labeled “after conditioning” shows a dog salivating while a bell is rung.

Before conditioning, an unconditioned stimulus (food) produces an unconditioned response (salivation), and a neutral stimulus (bell) does not produce a response. During conditioning, the unconditioned stimulus (food) is presented repeatedly just after the presentation of the neutral stimulus (bell). After conditioning, the neutral stimulus alone produces a conditioned response (salivation), thus becoming a conditioned stimulus.

Link to Learning

Now that you have learned about the process of classical conditioning, do you think you can condition Pavlov’s dog? Visit this website to play the game.

  Link to Learning

View this video to learn more about Pavlov and his dogs.

REAL WORLD APPLICATION OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING

How does classical conditioning work in the real world? Let’s say you have a cat named Tiger, who is quite spoiled. You keep her food in a separate cabinet, and you also have a special electric can opener that you use only to open cans of cat food. For every meal, Tiger hears the distinctive sound of the electric can opener (“zzhzhz”) and then gets her food. Tiger quickly learns that when she hears “zzhzhz” she is about to get fed. What do you think Tiger does when she hears the electric can opener? She will likely get excited and run to where you are preparing her food. This is an example of classical conditioning. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR?

What if the cabinet holding Tiger’s food becomes squeaky? In that case, Tiger hears “squeak” (the cabinet), “zzhzhz” (the electric can opener), and then she gets her food. Tiger will learn to get excited when she hears the “squeak” of the cabinet. Pairing a new neutral stimulus (“squeak”) with the conditioned stimulus (“zzhzhz”) is called higher-order conditioning , or second-order conditioning . This means you are using the conditioned stimulus of the can opener to condition another stimulus: the squeaky cabinet ( [link] ). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if you ring a bell, open the cabinet (“squeak”), use the can opener (“zzhzhz”), and then feed Tiger, Tiger will likely never get excited when hearing the bell alone.

A diagram is labeled “Higher-Order / Second-Order Conditioning” and has three rows. The first row shows an electric can opener labeled “conditioned stimulus” followed by a plus sign and then a dish of food labeled “unconditioned stimulus,” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “unconditioned response.” The second row shows a squeaky cabinet door labeled “second-order stimulus” followed by a plus sign and then an electric can opener labeled “conditioned stimulus,” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “conditioned response.” The third row shows a squeaky cabinet door labeled “second-order stimulus” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “conditioned response.”

In higher-order conditioning, an established conditioned stimulus is paired with a new neutral stimulus (the second-order stimulus), so that eventually the new stimulus also elicits the conditioned response, without the initial conditioned stimulus being presented.

Everyday Connection: Classical Conditioning at Stingray City

Kate and her husband Scott recently vacationed in the Cayman Islands, and booked a boat tour to Stingray City, where they could feed and swim with the southern stingrays. The boat captain explained how the normally solitary stingrays have become accustomed to interacting with humans. About 40 years ago, fishermen began to clean fish and conch (unconditioned stimulus) at a particular sandbar near a barrier reef, and large numbers of stingrays would swim in to eat (unconditioned response) what the fishermen threw into the water; this continued for years. By the late 1980s, word of the large group of stingrays spread among scuba divers, who then started feeding them by hand. Over time, the southern stingrays in the area were classically conditioned much like Pavlov’s dogs. When they hear the sound of a boat engine (neutral stimulus that becomes a conditioned stimulus), they know that they will get to eat (conditioned response).

As soon as Kate and Scott reached Stingray City, over two dozen stingrays surrounded their tour boat. The couple slipped into the water with bags of squid, the stingrays’ favorite treat. The swarm of stingrays bumped and rubbed up against their legs like hungry cats ( [link] ). Kate and Scott were able to feed, pet, and even kiss (for luck) these amazing creatures. Then all the squid was gone, and so were the stingrays.

A photograph shows a woman standing in the ocean holding a stingray.

Classical conditioning also applies to humans, even babies. For example, Sara buys formula in blue canisters for her six-month-old daughter, Angelina. Whenever Sara takes out a formula container, Angelina gets excited, tries to reach toward the food, and most likely salivates. Why does Angelina get excited when she sees the formula canister? What are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR here?

So far, all of the examples have involved food, but classical conditioning extends beyond the basic need to be fed. Consider our earlier example of a dog whose owners install an invisible electric dog fence. A small electrical shock (unconditioned stimulus) elicits discomfort (unconditioned response). When the unconditioned stimulus (shock) is paired with a neutral stimulus (the edge of a yard), the dog associates the discomfort (unconditioned response) with the edge of the yard (conditioned stimulus) and stays within the set boundaries.

For a humorous look at conditioning, watch this video clip from the television show The Office , where Jim conditions Dwight to expect a breath mint every time Jim’s computer makes a specific sound.

GENERAL PROCESSES IN CLASSICAL CONDITIONING

Now that you know how classical conditioning works and have seen several examples, let’s take a look at some of the general processes involved. In classical conditioning, the initial period of learning is known as acquisition , when an organism learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus. During acquisition, the neutral stimulus begins to elicit the conditioned response, and eventually the neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus capable of eliciting the conditioned response by itself. Timing is important for conditioning to occur. Typically, there should only be a brief interval between presentation of the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus. Depending on what is being conditioned, sometimes this interval is as little as five seconds (Chance, 2009). However, with other types of conditioning, the interval can be up to several hours.

Taste aversion is a type of conditioning in which an interval of several hours may pass between the conditioned stimulus (something ingested) and the unconditioned stimulus (nausea or illness). Here’s how it works. Between classes, you and a friend grab a quick lunch from a food cart on campus. You share a dish of chicken curry and head off to your next class. A few hours later, you feel nauseous and become ill. Although your friend is fine and you determine that you have intestinal flu (the food is not the culprit), you’ve developed a taste aversion; the next time you are at a restaurant and someone orders curry, you immediately feel ill. While the chicken dish is not what made you sick, you are experiencing taste aversion: you’ve been conditioned to be averse to a food after a single, negative experience.

How does this occur—conditioning based on a single instance and involving an extended time lapse between the event and the negative stimulus? Research into taste aversion suggests that this response may be an evolutionary adaptation designed to help organisms quickly learn to avoid harmful foods (Garcia & Rusiniak, 1980; Garcia & Koelling, 1966). Not only may this contribute to species survival via natural selection, but it may also help us develop strategies for challenges such as helping cancer patients through the nausea induced by certain treatments (Holmes, 1993; Jacobsen et al., 1993; Hutton, Baracos, & Wismer, 2007; Skolin et al., 2006).

Once we have established the connection between the unconditioned stimulus and the conditioned stimulus, how do we break that connection and get the dog, cat, or child to stop responding? In Tiger’s case, imagine what would happen if you stopped using the electric can opener for her food and began to use it only for human food. Now, Tiger would hear the can opener, but she would not get food. In classical conditioning terms, you would be giving the conditioned stimulus, but not the unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov explored this scenario in his experiments with dogs: sounding the tone without giving the dogs the meat powder. Soon the dogs stopped responding to the tone. Extinction is the decrease in the conditioned response when the unconditioned stimulus is no longer presented with the conditioned stimulus. When presented with the conditioned stimulus alone, the dog, cat, or other organism would show a weaker and weaker response, and finally no response. In classical conditioning terms, there is a gradual weakening and disappearance of the conditioned response.

What happens when learning is not used for a while—when what was learned lies dormant? As we just discussed, Pavlov found that when he repeatedly presented the bell (conditioned stimulus) without the meat powder (unconditioned stimulus), extinction occurred; the dogs stopped salivating to the bell. However, after a couple of hours of resting from this extinction training, the dogs again began to salivate when Pavlov rang the bell. What do you think would happen with Tiger’s behavior if your electric can opener broke, and you did not use it for several months? When you finally got it fixed and started using it to open Tiger’s food again, Tiger would remember the association between the can opener and her food—she would get excited and run to the kitchen when she heard the sound. The behavior of Pavlov’s dogs and Tiger illustrates a concept Pavlov called spontaneous recovery : the return of a previously extinguished conditioned response following a rest period ( [link] ).

A chart has an x-axis labeled “time” and a y-axis labeled “strength of CR;” there are four columns of graphed data. The first column is labeled “acquisition (CS + UCS) and the line rises steeply from the bottom to the top. The second column is labeled “Extinction (CS alone)” and the line drops rapidly from the top to the bottom. The third column is labeled “Pause” and has no line. The fourth column has a line that begins midway and drops sharply to the bottom. At the point where the line begins, it is labeled “Spontaneous recovery of CR”; the halfway point on the line is labeled “Extinction (CS alone).”

This is the curve of acquisition, extinction, and spontaneous recovery. The rising curve shows the conditioned response quickly getting stronger through the repeated pairing of the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus (acquisition). Then the curve decreases, which shows how the conditioned response weakens when only the conditioned stimulus is presented (extinction). After a break or pause from conditioning, the conditioned response reappears (spontaneous recovery).

Of course, these processes also apply in humans. For example, let’s say that every day when you walk to campus, an ice cream truck passes your route. Day after day, you hear the truck’s music (neutral stimulus), so you finally stop and purchase a chocolate ice cream bar. You take a bite (unconditioned stimulus) and then your mouth waters (unconditioned response). This initial period of learning is known as acquisition, when you begin to connect the neutral stimulus (the sound of the truck) and the unconditioned stimulus (the taste of the chocolate ice cream in your mouth). During acquisition, the conditioned response gets stronger and stronger through repeated pairings of the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. Several days (and ice cream bars) later, you notice that your mouth begins to water (conditioned response) as soon as you hear the truck’s musical jingle—even before you bite into the ice cream bar. Then one day you head down the street. You hear the truck’s music (conditioned stimulus), and your mouth waters (conditioned response). However, when you get to the truck, you discover that they are all out of ice cream. You leave disappointed. The next few days you pass by the truck and hear the music, but don’t stop to get an ice cream bar because you’re running late for class. You begin to salivate less and less when you hear the music, until by the end of the week, your mouth no longer waters when you hear the tune. This illustrates extinction. The conditioned response weakens when only the conditioned stimulus (the sound of the truck) is presented, without being followed by the unconditioned stimulus (chocolate ice cream in the mouth). Then the weekend comes. You don’t have to go to class, so you don’t pass the truck. Monday morning arrives and you take your usual route to campus. You round the corner and hear the truck again. What do you think happens? Your mouth begins to water again. Why? After a break from conditioning, the conditioned response reappears, which indicates spontaneous recovery.

Acquisition and extinction involve the strengthening and weakening, respectively, of a learned association. Two other learning processes—stimulus discrimination and stimulus generalization—are involved in distinguishing which stimuli will trigger the learned association. Animals (including humans) need to distinguish between stimuli—for example, between sounds that predict a threatening event and sounds that do not—so that they can respond appropriately (such as running away if the sound is threatening). When an organism learns to respond differently to various stimuli that are similar, it is called stimulus discrimination . In classical conditioning terms, the organism demonstrates the conditioned response only to the conditioned stimulus. Pavlov’s dogs discriminated between the basic tone that sounded before they were fed and other tones (e.g., the doorbell), because the other sounds did not predict the arrival of food. Similarly, Tiger, the cat, discriminated between the sound of the can opener and the sound of the electric mixer. When the electric mixer is going, Tiger is not about to be fed, so she does not come running to the kitchen looking for food.

On the other hand, when an organism demonstrates the conditioned response to stimuli that are similar to the condition stimulus, it is called stimulus generalization , the opposite of stimulus discrimination. The more similar a stimulus is to the condition stimulus, the more likely the organism is to give the conditioned response. For instance, if the electric mixer sounds very similar to the electric can opener, Tiger may come running after hearing its sound. But if you do not feed her following the electric mixer sound, and you continue to feed her consistently after the electric can opener sound, she will quickly learn to discriminate between the two sounds (provided they are sufficiently dissimilar that she can tell them apart).

Sometimes, classical conditioning can lead to habituation. Habituation occurs when we learn not to respond to a stimulus that is presented repeatedly without change. As the stimulus occurs over and over, we learn not to focus our attention on it. For example, imagine that your neighbor or roommate constantly has the television blaring. This background noise is distracting and makes it difficult for you to focus when you’re studying. However, over time, you become accustomed to the stimulus of the television noise, and eventually you hardly notice it any longer.

BEHAVIORISM

John B. Watson , shown in [link] , is considered the founder of behaviorism. Behaviorism is a school of thought that arose during the first part of the 20th century, which incorporates elements of Pavlov’s classical conditioning (Hunt, 2007). In stark contrast with Freud, who considered the reasons for behavior to be hidden in the unconscious, Watson championed the idea that all behavior can be studied as a simple stimulus-response reaction, without regard for internal processes. Watson argued that in order for psychology to become a legitimate science, it must shift its concern away from internal mental processes because mental processes cannot be seen or measured. Instead, he asserted that psychology must focus on outward observable behavior that can be measured.

A photograph shows John B. Watson.

John B. Watson used the principles of classical conditioning in the study of human emotion.

Watson’s ideas were influenced by Pavlov’s work. According to Watson, human behavior, just like animal behavior, is primarily the result of conditioned responses. Whereas Pavlov’s work with dogs involved the conditioning of reflexes, Watson believed the same principles could be extended to the conditioning of human emotions (Watson, 1919). Thus began Watson’s work with his graduate student Rosalie Rayner and a baby called Little Albert. Through their experiments with Little Albert, Watson and Rayner (1920) demonstrated how fears can be conditioned.

In 1920, Watson was the chair of the psychology department at Johns Hopkins University. Through his position at the university he came to meet Little Albert’s mother, Arvilla Merritte, who worked at a campus hospital (DeAngelis, 2010). Watson offered her a dollar to allow her son to be the subject of his experiments in classical conditioning. Through these experiments, Little Albert was exposed to and conditioned to fear certain things. Initially he was presented with various neutral stimuli, including a rabbit, a dog, a monkey, masks, cotton wool, and a white rat. He was not afraid of any of these things. Then Watson, with the help of Rayner, conditioned Little Albert to associate these stimuli with an emotion—fear. For example, Watson handed Little Albert the white rat, and Little Albert enjoyed playing with it. Then Watson made a loud sound, by striking a hammer against a metal bar hanging behind Little Albert’s head, each time Little Albert touched the rat. Little Albert was frightened by the sound—demonstrating a reflexive fear of sudden loud noises—and began to cry. Watson repeatedly paired the loud sound with the white rat. Soon Little Albert became frightened by the white rat alone. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR? Days later, Little Albert demonstrated stimulus generalization—he became afraid of other furry things: a rabbit, a furry coat, and even a Santa Claus mask ( [link] ). Watson had succeeded in conditioning a fear response in Little Albert, thus demonstrating that emotions could become conditioned responses. It had been Watson’s intention to produce a phobia—a persistent, excessive fear of a specific object or situation— through conditioning alone, thus countering Freud’s view that phobias are caused by deep, hidden conflicts in the mind. However, there is no evidence that Little Albert experienced phobias in later years. Little Albert’s mother moved away, ending the experiment, and Little Albert himself died a few years later of unrelated causes. While Watson’s research provided new insight into conditioning, it would be considered unethical by today’s standards.

A photograph shows a man wearing a mask with a white beard; his face is close to a baby who is crawling away. A caption reads, “Now he fears even Santa Claus.”

Through stimulus generalization, Little Albert came to fear furry things, including Watson in a Santa Claus mask.

View scenes from John Watson’s experiment in which Little Albert was conditioned to respond in fear to furry objects.

As you watch the video, look closely at Little Albert’s reactions and the manner in which Watson and Rayner present the stimuli before and after conditioning. Based on what you see, would you come to the same conclusions as the researchers?

Everyday Connection: Advertising and Associative Learning

Advertising executives are pros at applying the principles of associative learning. Think about the car commercials you have seen on television. Many of them feature an attractive model. By associating the model with the car being advertised, you come to see the car as being desirable (Cialdini, 2008). You may be asking yourself, does this advertising technique actually work? According to Cialdini (2008), men who viewed a car commercial that included an attractive model later rated the car as being faster, more appealing, and better designed than did men who viewed an advertisement for the same car minus the model.

Have you ever noticed how quickly advertisers cancel contracts with a famous athlete following a scandal? As far as the advertiser is concerned, that athlete is no longer associated with positive feelings; therefore, the athlete cannot be used as an unconditioned stimulus to condition the public to associate positive feelings (the unconditioned response) with their product (the conditioned stimulus).

Now that you are aware of how associative learning works, see if you can find examples of these types of advertisements on television, in magazines, or on the Internet.

Pavlov’s pioneering work with dogs contributed greatly to what we know about learning. His experiments explored the type of associative learning we now call classical conditioning. In classical conditioning, organisms learn to associate events that repeatedly happen together, and researchers study how a reflexive response to a stimulus can be mapped to a different stimulus—by training an association between the two stimuli. Pavlov’s experiments show how stimulus-response bonds are formed. Watson, the founder of behaviorism, was greatly influenced by Pavlov’s work. He tested humans by conditioning fear in an infant known as Little Albert. His findings suggest that classical conditioning can explain how some fears develop.

Self Check Questions

Critical thinking questions.

1. If the sound of your toaster popping up toast causes your mouth to water, what are the UCS, CS, and CR?

2. Explain how the processes of stimulus generalization and stimulus discrimination are considered opposites.

4. Can you think of an example in your life of how classical conditioning has produced a positive emotional response, such as happiness or excitement? How about a negative emotional response, such as fear, anxiety, or anger?

1. The food being toasted is the UCS; the sound of the toaster popping up is the CS; salivating to the sound of the toaster is the CR.

2. In stimulus generalization, an organism responds to new stimuli that are similar to the original conditioned stimulus. For example, a dog barks when the doorbell rings. He then barks when the oven timer dings because it sounds very similar to the doorbell. On the other hand, stimulus discrimination occurs when an organism learns a response to a specific stimulus, but does not respond the same way to new stimuli that are similar. In this case, the dog would bark when he hears the doorbell, but he would not bark when he hears the oven timer ding because they sound different; the dog is able to distinguish between the two sounds.

3. This occurs through the process of acquisition. A human or an animal learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus. During the acquisition phase, the neutral stimulus begins to elicit the conditioned response. The neutral stimulus is becoming the conditioned stimulus. At the end of the acquisition phase, learning has occurred and the neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus capable of eliciting the conditioned response by itself.

  • Psychology. Authored by : OpenStax College. Located at : http://cnx.org/contents/[email protected]:1/Psychology . License : CC BY: Attribution . License Terms : Download for free at http://cnx.org/content/col11629/latest/.

IMAGES

  1. Little Albert Experiment (Watson & Rayner)

    in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

  2. The Curious History of the Lab Rat

    in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

  3. In experiment 2, nulliparous and primiparous rats were conditioned with

    in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

  4. Experimental and apparatus design. a In the first experiment, rats were

    in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

  5. In experiment 3b, nulliparous rats were conditioned with one CS-US

    in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

  6. Laboratory rat

    in an experiment laboratory rats were classically conditioned

VIDEO

  1. Headjerking and rearing CRs to auditory and visual CSs

  2. implantation of dst micro-t in laboratory rat

  3. на этот раз @TEAM_laboratory_rats

  4. Lab Rats experiment explained in kannada #amazingfacts #facts #kannada #factshorts

  5. 13 RATS-Laboratory Rats

  6. Classical Conditioning

COMMENTS

  1. Psyc Chapter 6 Quiz

    In an experiment, laboratory rats were classically conditioned to get sick after drinking sweetened water that was paired with a drug that compromises immune response. These rats continued to die after the experiment because of the conditioned response to the sweetened water alone. Which conclusion can be drawn from this research?

  2. Little Albert experiment

    The Little Albert experiment was a controversial study by John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner that conditioned a fear of furry objects in a baby. The experiment is often cited as an example of classical conditioning, but its methods and ethics have been criticized and debated.

  3. Little Albert Experiment (Watson & Rayner)

    Watson and Rayner (1920) conditioned a nine-month-old boy named Little Albert to fear a white rat by pairing it with a loud noise. The experiment demonstrated that emotional responses could be learned through classical conditioning in humans, but also raised ethical concerns.

  4. The Little Albert Experiment

    Learn how Watson and Rayner conditioned a baby to fear a white rat and other stimuli, and what the experiment reveals about the conditioning process and stimulus generalization. Also, explore the criticism and ethical problems of the experiment, and the mystery of what happened to Little Albert.

  5. 6.2 Classical Conditioning

    Learn how Pavlov and his dogs discovered the process of classical conditioning, in which a neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus after being paired with an unconditioned stimulus. Explore the concepts of acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery, generalization, and discrimination with examples and applications.

  6. Classical Conditioning: Examples and How It Works

    Classical conditioning is a type of learning that creates a conditioned response through associations between a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus. Learn how it works, the key terms, and the examples of Pavlov's experiments with dogs.

  7. The Little Albert Experiment And The Chilling Story Behind It

    A 1920 study by Watson and Rayner conditioned a nine-month-old boy to fear a white rat and other furry objects by pairing them with a loud noise. The experiment was unethical and controversial, and the fate of the child, known as Little Albert, is still unknown.

  8. Little Albert Experiment

    The Little Albert Experiment was a study that conditioned a child to fear a white rat by pairing it with a loud noise. It demonstrated classical conditioning, generalization, and emotional reactions in humans, but also raised ethical and methodological concerns.

  9. The Little Albert Experiment

    The Little Albert experiment was performed in the early 1900s by Doctor John B. Watson. Dr. Watson was an American psychologist with a focus on human behavior. Watson was inspired by Doctor Ivan Pavlov's theory of classical conditioning. He wanted to understand the science of human learning and the way action and consequence affected a person.

  10. Classical Conditioning: How It Works With Examples

    Learn how classical conditioning works through association of stimuli and responses, with examples from Pavlov's dogs, Watson's Little Albert, and addiction. Find out the key principles, critical evaluation, and applications of this learning theory.

  11. psychology ch. 5 quiz Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Little Albert was conditioned by John B. Watson to fear a white rat. Eventually, however, Albert became fearful of any stimulus that looked white and furry. He became scared not only of rats, but also of rabbits, and even Santa Claus's beard. This phenomenon is called _____., Learning a. involves a systematic change b. involves a ...

  12. 6.1 Learning by Association: Classical Conditioning

    Classical conditioning is a type of learning that involves associating a neutral stimulus with a stimulus that produces a response. The conditioned stimulus becomes able to produce the response, which is called the conditioned response. Learn how Pavlov, extinction, generalization, and discrimination work in classical conditioning.

  13. Classical Conditioning

    Learn how Pavlov discovered the process of classical conditioning, by which a neutral stimulus becomes associated with an unconditioned stimulus and elicits a conditioned response. Explore the concepts of acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery, generalization, and discrimination with examples and activities.

  14. Solved In an experiment, laboratory rats were classically

    Psychology. Psychology questions and answers. In an experiment, laboratory rats were classically conditioned to get sick after drinking sweetened water that was paired with a drug that compromises immune response. These rats continued to die after the experiment because of the conditioned response to the sweetened water alone.

  15. Pavlov's Dogs Experiment & Pavlovian Conditioning Response

    Learn how Pavlov discovered classical conditioning by accident and how he used a metronome to train dogs to salivate at the sound of a bell. Find out the key terms, principles and examples of Pavlov's theory of learning.

  16. In an Experiment, Laboratory Rats Were Classically Conditioned to

    Verified Answer for the question: [Solved] In an experiment, laboratory rats were classically conditioned to get sick after drinking sweetened water that was paired with a drug that compromises immune response.These rats continued to die after the experiment because of the conditioned response to the sweetened water alone.Which conclusion can be drawn from this research?

  17. Classical Conditioning

    Figure 7.3 Ivan Pavlov's research on the digestive system of dogs unexpectedly led to his discovery of the learning process now known as classical conditioning. Pavlov came to his conclusions about how learning occurs completely by accident. Pavlov was a physiologist, not a psychologist. Physiologists study the life processes of organisms ...

  18. Ecological analysis of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats

    The absence of one-trial fear conditioning in a naturalistic setting may be analogous to "The Rat Park Experiment," where rats housed in an enriched environment with plants, trees, and social ...

  19. Classical Conditioning

    Learn how Pavlov discovered classical conditioning by accident and how it works in dogs and humans. Find out the terms and examples of unconditioned and conditioned stimuli and responses.

  20. In an experiment, laboratory rats were classically conditioned to get

    In an experiment, laboratory rats were classically conditioned to get sick after drinking sweetened water that was paired with a drug that compromises immune response. These rats continued to die after the experiment because of the conditioned response to the sweetened water alone. ... Animals and humans can be classically conditioned to any ...