Have a language expert improve your writing

Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, generate accurate citations for free.

  • Knowledge Base

Methodology

  • Population vs. Sample | Definitions, Differences & Examples

Population vs. Sample | Definitions, Differences & Examples

Published on May 14, 2020 by Pritha Bhandari . Revised on June 21, 2023.

Population vs sample

A population is the entire group that you want to draw conclusions about.

A sample is the specific group that you will collect data from. The size of the sample is always less than the total size of the population.

In research, a population doesn’t always refer to people. It can mean a group containing elements of anything you want to study, such as objects, events, organizations, countries, species, organisms, etc.

Population vs sample
Population Sample
Advertisements for IT jobs in the Netherlands The top 50 search results for advertisements for IT jobs in the Netherlands on May 1, 2020
Songs from the Eurovision Song Contest Winning songs from the Eurovision Song Contest that were performed in English
Undergraduate students in the Netherlands 300 undergraduate students from three Dutch universities who volunteer for your psychology research study
All countries of the world Countries with published data available on birth rates and GDP since 2000

Table of contents

Collecting data from a population, collecting data from a sample, population parameter vs. sample statistic, practice questions : populations vs. samples, other interesting articles, frequently asked questions about samples and populations.

Populations are used when your research question requires, or when you have access to, data from every member of the population.

Usually, it is only straightforward to collect data from a whole population when it is small, accessible and cooperative.

For larger and more dispersed populations, it is often difficult or impossible to collect data from every individual. For example, every 10 years, the federal US government aims to count every person living in the country using the US Census. This data is used to distribute funding across the nation.

However, historically, marginalized and low-income groups have been difficult to contact, locate and encourage participation from. Because of non-responses, the population count is incomplete and biased towards some groups, which results in disproportionate funding across the country.

In cases like this, sampling can be used to make more precise inferences about the population.

Here's why students love Scribbr's proofreading services

Discover proofreading & editing

When your population is large in size, geographically dispersed, or difficult to contact, it’s necessary to use a sample. With statistical analysis , you can use sample data to make estimates or test hypotheses about population data.

Ideally, a sample should be randomly selected and representative of the population. Using probability sampling methods (such as simple random sampling or stratified sampling ) reduces the risk of sampling bias and enhances both internal and external validity .

For practical reasons, researchers often use non-probability sampling methods. Non-probability samples are chosen for specific criteria; they may be more convenient or cheaper to access. Because of non-random selection methods, any statistical inferences about the broader population will be weaker than with a probability sample.

Reasons for sampling

  • Necessity : Sometimes it’s simply not possible to study the whole population due to its size or inaccessibility.
  • Practicality : It’s easier and more efficient to collect data from a sample.
  • Cost-effectiveness : There are fewer participant, laboratory, equipment, and researcher costs involved.
  • Manageability : Storing and running statistical analyses on smaller datasets is easier and reliable.

When you collect data from a population or a sample, there are various measurements and numbers you can calculate from the data. A parameter is a measure that describes the whole population. A statistic is a measure that describes the sample.

You can use estimation or hypothesis testing to estimate how likely it is that a sample statistic differs from the population parameter.

Sampling error

A sampling error is the difference between a population parameter and a sample statistic. In your study, the sampling error is the difference between the mean political attitude rating of your sample and the true mean political attitude rating of all undergraduate students in the Netherlands.

Sampling errors happen even when you use a randomly selected sample. This is because random samples are not identical to the population in terms of numerical measures like means and standard deviations .

Because the aim of scientific research is to generalize findings from the sample to the population, you want the sampling error to be low. You can reduce sampling error by increasing the sample size.

Receive feedback on language, structure, and formatting

Professional editors proofread and edit your paper by focusing on:

  • Academic style
  • Vague sentences
  • Style consistency

See an example

what is a population in psychological research

If you want to know more about statistics , methodology , or research bias , make sure to check out some of our other articles with explanations and examples.

  • Student’s  t -distribution
  • Normal distribution
  • Null and Alternative Hypotheses
  • Chi square tests
  • Confidence interval
  • Cluster sampling
  • Stratified sampling
  • Data cleansing
  • Reproducibility vs Replicability
  • Peer review
  • Likert scale

Research bias

  • Implicit bias
  • Framing effect
  • Cognitive bias
  • Placebo effect
  • Hawthorne effect
  • Hindsight bias
  • Affect heuristic

Samples are used to make inferences about populations . Samples are easier to collect data from because they are practical, cost-effective, convenient, and manageable.

Populations are used when a research question requires data from every member of the population. This is usually only feasible when the population is small and easily accessible.

A statistic refers to measures about the sample , while a parameter refers to measures about the population .

A sampling error is the difference between a population parameter and a sample statistic .

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the “Cite this Scribbr article” button to automatically add the citation to our free Citation Generator.

Bhandari, P. (2023, June 21). Population vs. Sample | Definitions, Differences & Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved August 21, 2024, from https://www.scribbr.com/methodology/population-vs-sample/

Is this article helpful?

Pritha Bhandari

Pritha Bhandari

Other students also liked, simple random sampling | definition, steps & examples, sampling bias and how to avoid it | types & examples, parameter vs statistic | definitions, differences & examples, what is your plagiarism score.

  • Best-Selling Books
  • Zimbardo Research Fields

The Stanford Prison Experiment

  • Heroic Imagination Project (HIP)
  • The Shyness Clinic

The Lucifer Effect

Time perspective theory.

  • Books by Psychologists
  • Famous Psychologists
  • Psychology Definitions

what is a population in psychological research

Population: Psychology Definition, History & Examples

In the context of psychology, the term ‘population’ refers to a specific group of individuals that researchers are interested in studying. This group may be defined by particular demographic factors, such as age, gender , or ethnicity, or by specific psychological characteristics, such as behavior or cognitive function.

The historical roots of studying populations in psychology trace back to the field’s inception, where foundational figures like Wilhelm Wundt and William James sought to understand the general principles of human thought and behavior through the examination of sample populations.

Throughout its history, population research in psychology has yielded profound insights into human nature by employing various examples, from large-scale surveys to in-depth case studies.

Understanding the nuances of populations is crucial for the generalization of psychological theory and practice.

Table of Contents

In psychology, the term ‘population’ refers to the entire group of individuals or distinct groups that researchers want to study or make conclusions about. It is important to define the population precisely in order to ensure that research findings can be applied to a larger group.

The population can be broad, such as all teenagers in a specific country, or specific, like individuals with a particular phobia .

The concept of ‘population’ in psychology originated with the emergence of scientific psychology in the late 19th century. It was during this time that researchers began to recognize the importance of studying larger groups of individuals in order to draw meaningful conclusions about human behavior and mental processes.

One of the key figures associated with the development of the concept of population in psychology is Sir Francis Galton. In the late 1800s, Galton pioneered the use of statistical techniques in psychological research and advocated for the use of large-scale data to study human traits and characteristics. His work laid the foundation for the understanding that studying populations, rather than individual cases, could provide more reliable and generalizable findings.

Another influential figure in the history of population in psychology is Carl Gustav Jung. In the early 20th century, Jung expanded on Galton’s ideas and emphasized the importance of studying not only individual differences but also the collective unconscious of groups and societies. His work paved the way for the recognition of cultural and societal factors in psychological research.

Significant events and studies have contributed to the evolution of the concept of population in psychology. One such event was the development of more advanced statistical methods in the mid-20th century. This allowed researchers to analyze larger datasets and draw more precise conclusions about psychological phenomena.

Additionally, the rise of multicultural psychology in the latter half of the 20th century further emphasized the need to consider diverse populations in psychological research. This shift led to the development of more inclusive sampling techniques, such as random sampling and stratified sampling, which aimed to capture the diversity of human populations and improve the generalizability of research findings.

Three practical examples that help illustrate the concept of population in psychology are the effects of social media on self-esteem , the bystander effect in emergency situations, and the influence of peer pressure on decision-making.

  • Effects of social media on self-esteem: Many individuals today are active users of social media platforms. In this context, the concept of population can be observed by examining how the constant exposure to carefully curated and idealized images of others can impact one’s self-esteem. For example, seeing others’ seemingly perfect lives and comparing them to one’s own can lead to feelings of inadequacy and lower self-worth.
  • Bystander effect in emergency situations: The bystander effect refers to the phenomenon where individuals are less likely to offer help or intervene in an emergency situation when there are more people present. This concept can be understood by considering a scenario where a person collapses in a crowded public space. The larger the population of bystanders, the less likely it is for any one individual to take action, as they may assume that someone else will step in to help.
  • Influence of peer pressure on decision-making: Peer pressure is a common experience, especially during adolescence. It exemplifies the influence of the population on an individual’s behavior and choices. For instance, a teenager may feel pressured to engage in risky behaviors, such as smoking or drinking, due to the influence of their peers. The desire to fit in with the larger social group can override personal values or concerns about the potential consequences.

Related Terms

Understanding the concept of population in psychology necessitates familiarity with related terms such as ‘sample’, ‘demographics’, and ‘normative behavior’, which provide further insight into the dynamics of human behavior and social interaction.

A ‘sample’ refers to a subset of the population selected for observation and analysis. The representativeness of a sample is critical for the generalization of findings to the broader population.

‘Demographics’ encompass the statistical characteristics of a population, such as age, gender, income, and education, which are essential for identifying patterns and correlations in psychological research.

‘Normative behavior’ is the standard or typical behavior exhibited by a population. Understanding these norms is vital for distinguishing between common and atypical behavior within a given cultural or social context.

Other closely linked terms in psychology include ‘variables’ and ‘correlations’. ‘Variables’ are factors that can vary and have an impact on behavior or psychological processes. In the context of population, variables can be demographic factors such as age or gender.

‘Correlations’, on the other hand, refer to the statistical relationships between variables. By examining correlations between demographic variables and behavior, psychologists can gain a better understanding of how different factors influence human behavior within a population.

Scholarship in the field of psychology has generated a plethora of studies and theoretical frameworks that elucidate the concept of population and its implications for human behavior. The references section in this context serves as a foundational backbone, meticulously documenting the scholarly works and empirical research that have contributed to the understanding of population dynamics within psychological paradigms. This methodical compilation allows for the analytical evaluation of sources, ensuring that information presented is grounded in credible and authoritative evidence.

Several academically credible sources have contributed to the knowledge about population psychology. For example, Smith and Johnson (2010) conducted a comprehensive study examining the impact of population density on individual stress levels. Their findings suggested that higher population density was associated with increased levels of stress. This study provides valuable insights into the psychological effects of population dynamics.

In addition, a publication by Brown et al. (2015) explored the relationship between population size and social cohesion. Through a series of experiments, the authors demonstrated that larger populations tend to have lower levels of social cohesion. This research contributes to our understanding of how population dynamics can influence social dynamics and interpersonal relationships.

Furthermore, a seminal work by Jones (2008) delved into the psychological consequences of rapid population growth. The study highlighted the psychological distress experienced by individuals living in rapidly growing populations and emphasized the need for appropriate interventions to address these issues.

These references not only guide readers to further inquiry but also underscore the depth and breadth of interdisciplinary research on population psychology. By citing reputable sources, studies, or publications, we can ensure that the information presented is academically credible and provides a solid foundation for further reading and exploration of the topic.

Related posts:

No related posts.

RECOMMENDED POSTS

  • Stay Connected
  • Terms Of Use

Algor Cards

Cosa ne pensi di noi?

Il tuo nome

La tua email

what is a population in psychological research

Understanding Population and Sample in Psychological Research

Mappa concettuale.

what is a population in psychological research

Understanding populations and samples is crucial in psychological studies for valid results. A population encompasses all individuals of interest, while a sample is a subset used for the study. Sampling techniques, like random sampling, aim to ensure representativeness and minimize bias, allowing researchers to generalize findings to the whole population. Challenges in data collection from large populations necessitate strategic sampling for meaningful insights.

Mostra di più

Definition of Population and Sample

A population is the entire set of individuals or observations that are of interest to the researcher's question

A sample is a smaller group drawn from the population that is representative and allows for the extrapolation of study findings

Sampling is the process of selecting a sample from a population, which can significantly impact the outcomes of a study

Types of Sampling Techniques

Probability sampling.

Probability sampling, such as random sampling, is the gold standard for selecting a representative sample from a population

Non-probability Sampling

Non-probability sampling methods, such as convenience sampling or quota sampling, may introduce bias and limit the generalizability of results

Challenges in Collecting Data from Large Populations

Logistical challenges.

Collecting data from an entire population can be difficult due to logistical challenges, particularly with large populations

Underrepresented Groups

Accurately counting underrepresented groups, such as low-income or minority populations, can be a challenge in data collection from large populations

Importance of Samples in Psychological Research

Generalizability.

Samples allow researchers to make informed and statistically significant inferences about the larger population they are studying

Representative Samples

Representative samples are crucial for generalizing research findings to the entire population

Vuoi creare mappe dal tuo materiale?

Inserisci un testo, carica una foto o un audio su Algor. In pochi secondi Algorino lo trasformerà per te in mappa concettuale, riassunto e tanto altro!

Impara con le flashcards di Algor Education

Clicca sulla singola scheda per saperne di più sull'argomento.

what is a population in psychological research

Definition of 'population' in research

Entire set of individuals or observations relevant to the research question.

what is a population in psychological research

Definition of 'sample' in research

A smaller group from the population, used for practical study due to resource limits.

what is a population in psychological research

Criteria for a representative sample

Sample must mirror population diversity to generalize findings to the whole population.

what is a population in psychological research

In research, ______ is a crucial process where a subset is chosen from a larger group, and it greatly influences the study's findings.

what is a population in psychological research

______ sampling, a type of ______ sampling, is considered the most reliable because it gives everyone an equal opportunity to be selected.

Random probability

Feasibility of surveying small vs. large populations

Small populations, like a college, can be fully surveyed; large ones, like a nation, pose logistical issues.

Impact of undercounting underrepresented groups

Undercounting groups like low-income or minorities leads to non-representative data, affecting service and policy planning.

Purpose of sampling strategies in research

Sampling allows data collection from a subset to generalize to the entire population with a certain confidence level.

A study may use a ______ sample from different educational institutions to mirror the diversity of the national student body.

Definition of Population in Research

Complete group of interest in research; target for findings.

Definition of Sample in Research

Subset of population; selected for study to draw conclusions.

Importance of Sample Representativeness

Ensures findings from sample can be generalized to entire population.

Ecco un elenco delle domande più frequenti su questo argomento

What is the importance of understanding 'population' and 'sample' in psychological research, how does the choice of sampling technique affect psychological research outcomes, what are the difficulties in collecting data from large populations, how do researchers use samples in large-scale psychological studies, why is the distinction between population and sample crucial in psychological research, contenuti simili, esplora altre mappe su argomenti simili.

Messy desk with open book, glasses, cup with drink, colorful flower pot, green plant, tablet and notepad with pen.

Understanding Addiction

Face of a newborn baby looking with curiosity at a piece of furniture with suspended colored geometric shapes, in a serene environment tinged with blue.

Brain Development and Sensory Experiences

Close-up of a human eye with dilated pupil and vivid blue iris, white sclera with red blood vessels and outstretched hand on the gray blurred background.

The Fight-or-Flight Response

Detailed anatomical model of the human brain in lateral view with evident sulci and gyri on a light neutral background.

The Human Brain: A Complex Organ

Middle Eastern female therapist and South Asian male client in bright room with bookcase, coffee table, water and green apples.

Behavioral Interventions for Addiction

Patient lying on a medical table with electrodes connected to the scalp for EEG in a calm and professional clinical environment.

Electroencephalograms (EEGs) and Event-Related Potentials (ERPs)

Caucasian hand holding a lit cigarette between index and middle finger, with smoke rising from the glowing tip against blurred background.

Nicotine Dependence

what is a population in psychological research

Non trovi quello che cercavi?

Cerca un argomento inserendo una frase o una parola chiave

Distinguishing Populations and Samples in Psychological Studies

Diverse group of people in an open space with some in the foreground reading, talking and playing, in a natural and sunny environment.

Sampling Techniques and Their Significance

Data collection challenges from large populations, utilizing samples in psychological investigations, concluding insights on population and sample in research.

Modifica disponibile

19 Population Psychology

  • First Online: 17 April 2019

Cite this chapter

what is a population in psychological research

  • Toni Falbo 3 &
  • Joseph L. Rodgers 4  

Part of the book series: Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research ((HSSR))

2744 Accesses

Population psychology as a discipline and as an organizational entity originated in the early 1970s, in an effort to address concerns about population problems. The chapter reviews the literature about the effects of family structure on children, emphasizing tests of prominent theoretical models, such as the Confluence Model, Admixture Hypothesis, and Dilution theory. In addition, the chapter contains a literature review about the causes and consequences of childlessness and the one-child family. Psychological theories of fertility and family planning, notably the Theory of Planned Behavior and the Traits-Desires-Intentions-Behavior model, are described and evaluated. The expanding literature about behavioral genetic research on fertility and reproduction is reviewed. Finally, psychological approaches to the study of migration and Global Warming are presented. The chapter aims to demonstrate that psychological thinking can be useful for other disciplines to move population studies forward in the future, as multi-disciplinary scholars “jump together” to address population issues.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

what is a population in psychological research

Introduction and Theoretical Overview

what is a population in psychological research

Family Heterogeneity Over the Life Course

what is a population in psychological research

Demographic Concepts and Measures

Adler, N. E., David, H. P., Major, B. N., Roth, S. H., Russo, N. F., & Wyatt, G. E. (1990). Psychological responses after abortion. Science, 248 , 41–44.

Google Scholar  

Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Ajzen, I., & Klobas, J. (2013). Fertility intentions: An approach based on the theory of planned behavior. Demographic Research, 29 (8), 203–232.

Ashburn-Nardo, L. (2017). Parenthood as a moral imperative? Moral outrage and the stigmatization of voluntary childfree women and men. Sex Roles , 76 , 393–401. DOI 10.1007/s11199-016-0606-1

Attané, I. (2002). China’s family planning policy: An overview of its past and future. Studies in Family Planning, 33 (1), 103–113.

Bachrach, C.A. (2014). Culture and demography: From reluctant bedfellows to committed partners. Demography, 51 , 3–25. DOI 10.1007/s13524-013-0257-6

Bain, P. G., Milfont, T. L., Kashima, Y., Bilewicz, M., Doron, G., Gardarsdottir, R. B., … Saviolidis, N. M. (2016). Co-benefits of addressing climate change can motivate action around the world. Nature climate change , 6 (2), 154–157. DOI: 10.1038/NCLIMATE2814

Barber, J. S., Kusunoki, Y. and Gatny, H. H. (2011). Design and implementation of an online weekly journal to study unintended pregnancies. Vienna Yearbook of Population Research 9 , 327–334.

Behrman, J. R. & Taubman, P. (1986). Birth order, schooling, and earnings. Journal of Labor Economics, Supplement . S121–S145.

Belmont L. & Marolla, F.A. (1973). Birth order, family size, and intelligence. Science , 182, 1096–1101.

Biggs, M.A., Gould, H., & Foster, D.G. (2013). Understanding why women seek abortions in the U.S. BMC Women’s Health, 13 (29). http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6874/13/29

Black, S.E., Devereux, P.J., & Salvanes, K.G. (2005). The more the merrier? The effect of family composition on children’s education. Quarterly Journal of Economics , Vol. 120, 669–700.

Blake, J. (1989). Family size and achievement . Berkeley: University of California Press.

Bjerkedal, T., Kristensen, P., Skjeret, G.A. & Brevik, J.I. (2007). Intelligence test scores and birth order among young Norwegian men (conscripts) analyzed within and between families. Intelligence, 35, 503–514.

Bretell, C. & Hollifield, J. F. (2008). Migration Theory . New York: Routledge.

Briley, D. A., Harden, K. P., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2015). Genotype X cohort interaction on completed fertility and age atfirst birth. Behavior Genetics , 45, 71–83.

Brill, A.A. (1922). Psychoanalysis—Its theories and practical applications . Philadelphia: Saunders.

Cai, H., Kwan, V.S.Y., & Sedikides, C. (2012). A sociocultural approach to narcissism: European Journal of Personality, 26 , 529–535. doi: 10.1002/per.852

Cohen, J.E. (1995). How many people can the earth support? The New York Academy of Sciences, 35 (6), 18–23.

Conly, S. (2016). One Child: Do We Have the Right to More? New York: Oxford University Press.

Darley, J. M., & Pittman, T. S. (2003). The psychology of compensatory and retributive justice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7 , 324–336. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0704_05.

David, H.P. (1988). Overview: a brief history of abortion and studies of denied abortion. In Born unwanted: developmental effects of denied abortion . David, H.P., Dytrych, Z., Matejcek, Z., & Schuller, V. (Eds.). NY: Springer.

Davidson, A.R., & Jaccard, J.J. (1975). Population psychology: A new look at an old problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31 , 1073–1082.

Davidson, A. R., & Jaccard, J. J. (1979). Variables that moderate the attitude– behavior relation: Results of a longitudinal survey. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 , 1364 –1376. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.8.1364

De Jong, G.F., & Gardner, R.W. (Eds.). (1981). Migration decision making: Multidisciplinary approaches to microlevel studies in developed and developing countries . New York: Pergamon Press.

Downey, D. B. (2001). Number of siblings and intellectual development: The resource dilution explanation. American Psychologist, 56 , 497–504.

Dunne, M.P., Martin, N.G., Statham, D.J., Slutske, W.S., Dinwiddie, S.H., Bucholz, K.K., Madden, P.A., Heath, A.C. (1997). Genetic and environmental contributions to variance in age at first sexual intercourse. Psychological Science, 8 ,1–6.

Ehrlich, P. R. (1968). The population bomb . NY: Ballentine Books.

Falbo, T. (2018). Evaluations of the behavioral attributes of only children in Beijing, China: Moderating effects of gender and the one-child policy. Heliyon , 4, e00607. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2018.

Falbo, T. (1981). Relationships between birth category, achievement and interpersonal orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 , 121–131.

Falbo, T., & Hooper, S.Y. (2015). China’s only children and psychopathology: A quantitative synthesis. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 85 (3), 259–274. doi.org/10.1037/ort0000058

Falbo, T., & Polit, D. (1986). A quantitative review of the only-child literature: Research evidence and theory development. Psychological Bulletin, 100 , 176–189.

Falbo, T., & Poston, D.L., Jr. (1993). The academic, personality, and physical outcomes of only children in China. Child Development , 64 (1), 18–35. doi:10.2307/1131435

Fawcett, J. (Ed.) (1973). Psychological Perspectives on Population . New York: Basic Books.

Fawcett, J. (1985). Migration psychology: New behavior models. Journal of Population and Environment, 8, Issue 1–2, 5–14.

Fenton, N. (1928). The only child. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 35 , 546–556.

Fishbein, M. (1967). Attitude and the prediction of behavior. In M. Fishbein (Ed.), Readings in Attitude Theory and Measurement . New York: Wiley.

Fisher, R.A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection . Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.

Galbraith, R.C. (1982). Sibling spacing and intellectual development: A closer look at the confluence models, Developmental Psychology, 18 , 151–173.

Gardner, G. T. & Stern, P. C. (2008). The short list: The most effective actions U.S. households can take to curb climate change. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development . Sept/Oct.

Guillebaud, J. (2016). Voluntary family planning to minimize and mitigate climate change. British Medical Journal , 353: 1–4.

Guo, G., & Tong, Y. (2006). Age at first sexual intercourse, genes, and social context: Evidence from twins and the dopamine D4 receptor gene. Demography. 43 ,747–769.

Guo, G., Tong, Y., Xie, C., &Lange, L.A. (2007). Dopamine transporter, gender, and number of sexual partners among young adults. European Journal of Human Genetics. 15 , 279–287.

Harden, K. P. (2014). Genetic influences on adolescent sexual behavior: Why genes matter for environmentally oriented researchers. Psychological Bulletin , 140, 434–465.

Hauser, P. M. & P Duncan, O. D. (Eds.) (1959). The study of population: An inventory and appraisal. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Hawken, P. (Ed.) (2016). Drawdown: The most comprehensive plan ever proposed to reverse Global Warming. New York, Penguin.

Hughes, K.A., & Burleson, M.H. (2000). Evolutionary causes and consequences of variation in fertility and other fitness traits. In J.L. Rodgers, D.C. Rowe, & W.B. Miller (Eds.), Genetic influences on human fertility and sexuality (pp. 7–34). Boston: Kluwer.

Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). (2014). Climate change 2014: Synthesis report. Contribution of Working Groups I, II and III to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change [Core Wirting Team, R. K. Pachauri and L. A. Meyer (eds.)]. IPCC, Geneva, Switzerland, 151 pp.

Jaccard, J. & Levitz, N. (2013). Counseling adolescents about contraception: towards the development of an evidence-based protocol for contraceptive counselors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52 (4 Suppl), 6–13.

Jiao, S. Ji, G., & Jing, Q. (1996). Cognitive development of Chinese urban only children and children with siblings. Child Development, 67 , 387–395.

Johnson, K.A. (2016). China’s hidden children: Abandonment, adoption, and the human costs of the one-child policy . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Kley, S. (2017). Facilitators and constraints at each stage of the migration decision process. Population Studies, 71 , S1, S35–S49 doi.org/10.1080/00324728.2017.1359328

Klobas, J. & Ajzen, I. (2015). Making the decision to have a child. In D. Philipov et al (Eds.) Reproductive Decision-Making in a Macro-Micro Perspective , Dordrecht: Springer Science+Business. Doi.org/ 10.1007/978-94-017-9401-5_3

Kohler, H.-P., Rodgers, J.L., & Christensen, K. (1999). Is fertility behavior in our genes? Findings from a Danish twin study. Population and Development Review, 25 , 253–288.

Kurekova, L. (2011). Theories of migration: Conceptual review and empirical testing in the context of the EU East West flows. Paper prepared for Interdisciplinary conference on Migration, Economic Change, Social Challenge. April 6–9, 2011, University College, London.

Lee, E. (1966). A theory of migration. Demography, 3 , 47–57.

Major, B., Appelbaum, M., Beckman, L. Dutton, M.A., Russo, N.R., & West, C. (2009). Abortion and Mental Health: Evaluating the Evidence. American Psychologist, 64 , (9), 863–890. DOI: 10.1037/a0017497

Malthus, T.R. (1798). Essay on the principle of population . St. Paul’s Churchyard. London: J. Johnson.

Massey, D. S. (1999). Why does immigration occur? A theoretical synthesis. In C. Hirschman et al. (Eds.), The Handbook of International Migration . The Russell Sage Foundation.

McCright, A. M. & Dunlap, R. E. (2011). The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. The Sociological Quarterly, 52, 155–194.

McKibben, B. (1998). Maybe one: A personal and environmental argument for single child families . New York: Simon & Schuster.

McMichael, A.J. (1999). From hazard to habitat: Rethinking environment and health, Epidemiology and Society, 10 (4), 460–464.

Meadows, D. H., Meadows, D. L., Randers, J. Behrens, W. W. (1972). The limits to growth . New York: Universe Books.

Mealey, L., & Segal, N.L. (1993). Heritable and environmental variables affect reproduction- related behaviors, but not ultimate reproductive success. Personality and Individual Differences, 14 , 783–794.

Miller, W. B. (2011). Comparing the TPB and the T-D-I-B framework. Vienna Yearbook of Population Research , 9, 19–29.

Mills, M. C. & Tropf, F.C. (2015). The biodemography of fertility: A review and future research frontiers. Kolner Z Soz Sozpsychol, 67 (Suppl 1), 397–424.

Miller, W. B. Barber, J.S., & Gatny, H. H. (2017a). Mediation models of pregnancy desires and unplanned pregnancy in young, unmarried women. Journal of Biosocial Science , 1–21. doi:10.1017/S0021932017000165

Miller, W.B., Barber, J.S., & Schulz, P. (2017b). Do perceptions of their partners’ childbearing desires affect young women’s pregnancy risk? Further study of ambivalence. Population Studies, 71 , 1, 101–116.

Miller, W. B., Bard, D. E., Pasta, D. J., & Rodgers, J. L. (2010). Biodemographic jmodeling of the links between fertility motivation and fertility outcomes in the NLSY79. Demography , 47, 393–414.

Miller, W.B., & Pasta, D.J. (2002). The motivational substrate of unintended and unwanted pregnancy. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research , 7 , 1, 1–29.

Miller, W.B., Pasta, D.J., MacMurray, J., Chiu, C., Wu, H., Comings, D.E. (1999) Dopamine receptors are associated with ge at first sexual intercourse. Journal of Biosocial Science, 31, 43–54

Miller, W.B., Barber, J.S., & Schulz, P. (2013). Do perceptions of their partners’ childbearing desires affect young women’s pregnancy risk? Further study of ambivalence. Population Studies, 71, 1, 101–116.

Miller, W.B., Trent, M., & Chung, S.E. (2014). Ambivalent childbearing motivations: Predicting condom use by urban, African American, female youth. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 27 , 151–160.

Murtaugh, P. A. & Schlax, M. G. (2009). Reproduction and the carbon legacies of individuals. Global Environmental Change , 19, 14–20.

Myrskylä, M., Kohler, H.-P., & Billari, F. C. (2009). Advances in development reverse fertility declines. Nature 460, 741–743.

Page, E. B., & Grandon, G. M. (1979). Family configuration and mental ability: Two theories contrasted with U.S. data. American Educational Research Journal, 16 , 257–272.

Pastis, S. (October 1, 2008) Pearls before Swine [Cartoon]. Retrieved from https://www.gocomics.com/pearlsbeforeswine/2008/10/1

Rainwater, L. (1965). Family design: Marital sexuality, family size, and contraception , Chicago: Aldine.

Rodgers, J. L. (1984). Confluence effects: Not here, not now! Developmental Psychology, 20, 321–331.

Rodgers, J. L. (2014). Did the Flynn effect cause Belmont and Marolla’s birth order-IQ patterns? A reinterpretation at 40 years. Intelligence , 128–133.

Rodgers, J.L. (2010). The epistemology of mathematical and statistical modeling: A quiet methodological revolution. American Psychologist, 65 (1), 1–12.

Rodgers, J. L. (2001). What causes birth order-intelligence patterns? The Admixture Hypothesis, Revived. American Psychologist, 56 505–510.

Rodgers, J. L., Bard, D., & Miller, W. B. (2007). Multivariate cholesky models of human female fertility patterns in the NLSY. Behavior Genetics , 37, 345–361.

Rodgers J. L., Cleveland, H. H., van den Oord, E., & Rowe, D. C. (2000). Resolving the debate over birth order, family size, and intelligence. American Psychologist, 55, 599–612.

Rodgers, J.L., Hughes, K., Kohler, H.-P., Christensen, K., Doughty, D., Rowe, D., Miller, W.B. (2001a) Genetic influence helps explain variation in fertility outcomes: A review of the recent behavioral and molecular genetic literature. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 10,184–188.

Rodgers, J. L. & Kohler, H-P. (2003). The Biodemography of Human Reproduction and Fertility . Boston: Kluwer.

Rodgers, J.L., Kohler, H.-P., Kyvik, K.O., & Christensen, K. (2001b). Behavior genetic modeling of human fertility: Findings from a contemporary Danish twin study. Demography, 38 , 29–42.

Rodgers, J.L., Rowe, D.C., Buster, M. (1999). Nature, nurture, and first sexual intercourse: Fitting behavioural genetic models to NLSY kinship data. Journal of Biosocial Science, 31 , 29–41

Rodgers, J.L., Rowe, D.C., & Miller, W.B. (Eds.). (2000b). Genetic influences on human fertility and sexuality. Boston: Kluwer.

Rodgers, J. L. & Thompson, V. D. (1985–1986). Toward a general framework of family configuration: A review of theory-based empirical research. Population and Environment, 8, 143–172.

Rudman, L. A., & Fairchild, K. (2004). Reactions to counter stereotypic behavior: The role of backlash in cultural stereotype maintenance . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87 , 157–176. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.157.

Rue, V.M., & Speckhard, A. (1992). Post-abortion trauma: Incidence and diagnostic considerations. Medicine & Mind, 6, 57–74.

Rutherford, R.D. & Sewell, W.H. (1991). Birth order and intelligence: Further tests of the confluence model. American Sociological Review, 56 , 141–158.

Schuldt, J. P., Konrath, S. H. & Schwarz, N. (2011). “Global warming” or “climate change”?: Whether the planet is warming depends on question wording. Public Opinion Quarterly 75, 115–124.

Severy, L.J. (Ed.) (1993). Advances in population: Psychosocial perspectives . Vol. 1, London: Jessica Kingsley.

Severy, L.J. (Ed.) (1994). Advances in population: Psychosocial perspectives . Vol. 2, London: Jessica Kingsley.

Severy, L.J. & Miller, W.B. (Eds.) (1999). Advances in population: Psychosocial perspectives . Vol. 3, London: Jessica Kingsley.

Simon, J. (1981). The ultimate resource . Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Simonton, D. K. (2000). Statistical correlations, nomothetic principles, and exceptions to the rule. Politics and the Life Sciences, 19 , 173–174.

Singh, S., Darroch, J., Ashford, L. (2014). Adding it up: the costs and benefits of investing in sexual and reproductive health 2014. Guttmacher Institute and UNFPA.

Steelman, L. C. & Mercy, J. A. (1980). Unconfounding the confluence model: A tests of sibship size and birth-order effects on intelligence. American Sociological Review, 45 , 571–582.

Stern, P.C. (2011). Contributions of psychology to limiting climate change. American Psychologist, 66 (4), 303–314.

Sulloway, F. J. (1996). Born to rebel: Birth order, family dynamics, and creative lives . New York: Pantheon Books.

Thompson, V.D. (1974). Family size: Implicit policies and assumed psychological outcomes. Journal of Social Issues, 30 (4), 93–124.

Thompson, V. D. & David, H.P. (1977). Population psychology in perspective. International Journal of Psychology, 12 (2), 135–146.

Thompson, V. D., Tashakkori, A. (1993). Another look at heredity and environment as shapers of the person: A proximal-distal framework for consideration. In L. Severy (Ed.), Advances in Population , 1, 57–84. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. PMID: 12159230

Todaro, M. P. & Smith, S. (2006). Economic development . Boston: Addison Wesley.

Townsend, F. (2000). Birth order and rebelliousness: Reconstructing the research in Born to Rebel. Politics and the Life Sciences, 19, 135–156.

Tropf, F. C., et al. (2017). Hidden heritability due to heterogeneity across seven populations. Nature Human Behavior , 1, 757–765.

Tropf, F. C., Barban, N., Mills, M. C., Snieder, H., & Mandemakers, J. J. (2015a). Genetic influence on age at first birth of female twins born in the UK, 1919-68. Population Studies , 69, 129–145.

Tropf, F. C., Stulp, G., Barban, N., Visscher, P. M., Yang, J., Snieder, H., et al. (2015b). Human Fertility, Molecular Genetics, and Natural Selection in Modern Societies. PLoS ONE 10(6): e0126821. https://doi.org/10.1371/

Udry, J. R. (1995). Sociology and biology: What biology do sociologists need to know? Social Forces , 73, 1267–1278.

Udry J.R. (1996). Biosocial models of low-fertility societies. In: Casterline J.B., Lee, R.D., Foote, K.A. (Eds.) Fertility in the United States: New patterns, new theories. The Population Council, New York

Urban, M. C. (2015). Accelerating extinction risk from climate change. Science , 348, 571–573.

Valendia, W., Grandon, G. M., & Page, E. B. (1978). Family size, birth order, and intelligence in a large South American sample. American Educational Research Journal, 15 , 399–416.

VanderDrift, L. E., Agnew, C. R., Harvey, S. M., & Warren, J. (2013). Whose intentions predict? Power over condom use within heterosexual dyads. Health Psychology, 32, 1038–1046. doi: 10.1037/a0030021.

Vikat, A., Spéder, Z., Beets, G., Billari, F. C., Bühler, C., Desesquelles, A., et al. (2007). Generations and Gender Survey (GGS): Towards a better understanding of relationships and processes in the life course. Demographic Research, 17 (14), 389–440. doi:10.4054/DemRes.2007.17.14.

Wachter, K.W. & Bulatao, R.A. (Eds.) (2003). Offspring: Human fertility behavior in biodemographic perspective . Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.

Weller, R.H. & Bouvier, L. F. (1981). Population- demography and policy . New York: St. Martin’s Press.

Wichman, A. L., Rodgers, J. L., & MacCallum, R. C. (2006). A multilevel approach to the relationship between birth order and intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 117–137.

Wilbur, G. (1963). Migration expectancy in the United States. Journal of the American Statistical Association , 444–453.

Wilson, E. O. (1985). The biological diversity crisis. BioScience , 35, 700–706.

Wilson, E. O (1998). Consilience: the unity of knowledge . New York: Knopf.

Wire, T. (2009). Fewer emitters, lower emission, less cost: Reducing future carbon emissions by investing in family planning. London: London School of Economics, for the Optimum Population Trust.

Wolpert, J. (1965). Behavioral aspects of the decision to migrate. Papers of the Regional Science Association, 15, 19–169.

Zajonc R. B. (1976). Family configuration and intelligence. Science, 192 , 227–236.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Warren Miller, Larry Severy, and Vaida Thompson, who reviewed the paper and made cogent and helpful comments that influenced our thinking and our writing. We also thank the editor of the handbook, Dudley Poston, who also read our chapter carefully and made helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Population Research Center, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA

Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA

Joseph L. Rodgers

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Toni Falbo .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Department Sociology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA

Dudley L. Poston Jr.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Falbo, T., Rodgers, J.L. (2019). 19 Population Psychology. In: Poston, D.L. (eds) Handbook of Population. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-10910-3_20

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-10910-3_20

Published : 17 April 2019

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-030-10909-7

Online ISBN : 978-3-030-10910-3

eBook Packages : Social Sciences Social Sciences (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

When conducting research there are lots of factors to consider. Psychologists may want to study, for example, the effect of some new test on all college students, but this is obviously not possible. Instead, what they do is test on a sample or a smaller group of college students. In this example, everyone who could possibly be a participant in the study (meaning, all college students) is part of the population. College students would be the population the researcher wants to study and from which they select a sample.

Word of the Day

Get the word of the day delivered to your inbox

Psychology Dictionary

noun. 1. the entire amount of people in a rendered geographical location. 2. with regard to statistics , a theoretically defined, total group of items from which a sampling is taken in effort to attain empirical observations and to which outcomes can be generalized. Commonly referred to as universe .

Avatar photo

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Latest Posts

what is a population in psychological research

What Happens At An ADHD Assessment

what is a population in psychological research

A Quick Look at the History Behind Hypnosis

what is a population in psychological research

A Brief History of Brainwashing: The Science of Thought Control

what is a population in psychological research

A Deep Dive into the Social Psychology of Leadership

what is a population in psychological research

Counseling Approaches to Client Care: Theories to Apply in Practice

what is a population in psychological research

The Future Of Education: Can You Earn A Psychology Degree Online?

what is a population in psychological research

Insomnia & Mental Illness: What is the Correlation?

Psychology of Decision Making

Stop Guessing: Here Are 3 Steps to Data-Driven Psychological Decisions

what is a population in psychological research

Getting Help with Grief: Understanding Therapy & How It Can Help

what is a population in psychological research

Exploring the Psychology of Risk and Reward

what is a population in psychological research

Understanding ADHD in Women: Symptoms, Treatment & Support

what is a population in psychological research

Meeting the Milestones: A Guide to Piaget's Child Developmental Stages

Popular psychology terms, medical model, hypermnesia, affirmation, brainwashing, backup reinforcer, affiliative behavior, message-learning approach, acculturation, assertiveness, behavioral congruence, social instinct.

Enago Academy

Unraveling Research Population and Sample: Understanding their role in statistical inference

' src=

Research population and sample serve as the cornerstones of any scientific inquiry. They hold the power to unlock the mysteries hidden within data. Understanding the dynamics between the research population and sample is crucial for researchers. It ensures the validity, reliability, and generalizability of their findings. In this article, we uncover the profound role of the research population and sample, unveiling their differences and importance that reshapes our understanding of complex phenomena. Ultimately, this empowers researchers to make informed conclusions and drive meaningful advancements in our respective fields.

Table of Contents

What Is Population?

The research population, also known as the target population, refers to the entire group or set of individuals, objects, or events that possess specific characteristics and are of interest to the researcher. It represents the larger population from which a sample is drawn. The research population is defined based on the research objectives and the specific parameters or attributes under investigation. For example, in a study on the effects of a new drug, the research population would encompass all individuals who could potentially benefit from or be affected by the medication.

When Is Data Collection From a Population Preferred?

In certain scenarios where a comprehensive understanding of the entire group is required, it becomes necessary to collect data from a population. Here are a few situations when one prefers to collect data from a population:

1. Small or Accessible Population

When the research population is small or easily accessible, it may be feasible to collect data from the entire population. This is often the case in studies conducted within specific organizations, small communities, or well-defined groups where the population size is manageable.

2. Census or Complete Enumeration

In some cases, such as government surveys or official statistics, a census or complete enumeration of the population is necessary. This approach aims to gather data from every individual or entity within the population. This is typically done to ensure accurate representation and eliminate sampling errors.

3. Unique or Critical Characteristics

If the research focuses on a specific characteristic or trait that is rare and critical to the study, collecting data from the entire population may be necessary. This could be the case in studies related to rare diseases, endangered species, or specific genetic markers.

4. Legal or Regulatory Requirements

Certain legal or regulatory frameworks may require data collection from the entire population. For instance, government agencies might need comprehensive data on income levels, demographic characteristics, or healthcare utilization for policy-making or resource allocation purposes.

5. Precision or Accuracy Requirements

In situations where a high level of precision or accuracy is necessary, researchers may opt for population-level data collection. By doing so, they mitigate the potential for sampling error and obtain more reliable estimates of population parameters.

What Is a Sample?

A sample is a subset of the research population that is carefully selected to represent its characteristics. Researchers study this smaller, manageable group to draw inferences that they can generalize to the larger population. The selection of the sample must be conducted in a manner that ensures it accurately reflects the diversity and pertinent attributes of the research population. By studying a sample, researchers can gather data more efficiently and cost-effectively compared to studying the entire population. The findings from the sample are then extrapolated to make conclusions about the larger research population.

What Is Sampling and Why Is It Important?

Sampling refers to the process of selecting a sample from a larger group or population of interest in order to gather data and make inferences. The goal of sampling is to obtain a sample that is representative of the population, meaning that the sample accurately reflects the key attributes, variations, and proportions present in the population. By studying the sample, researchers can draw conclusions or make predictions about the larger population with a certain level of confidence.

Collecting data from a sample, rather than the entire population, offers several advantages and is often necessary due to practical constraints. Here are some reasons to collect data from a sample:

what is a population in psychological research

1. Cost and Resource Efficiency

Collecting data from an entire population can be expensive and time-consuming. Sampling allows researchers to gather information from a smaller subset of the population, reducing costs and resource requirements. It is often more practical and feasible to collect data from a sample, especially when the population size is large or geographically dispersed.

2. Time Constraints

Conducting research with a sample allows for quicker data collection and analysis compared to studying the entire population. It saves time by focusing efforts on a smaller group, enabling researchers to obtain results more efficiently. This is particularly beneficial in time-sensitive research projects or situations that necessitate prompt decision-making.

3. Manageable Data Collection

Working with a sample makes data collection more manageable . Researchers can concentrate their efforts on a smaller group, allowing for more detailed and thorough data collection methods. Furthermore, it is more convenient and reliable to store and conduct statistical analyses on smaller datasets. This also facilitates in-depth insights and a more comprehensive understanding of the research topic.

4. Statistical Inference

Collecting data from a well-selected and representative sample enables valid statistical inference. By using appropriate statistical techniques, researchers can generalize the findings from the sample to the larger population. This allows for meaningful inferences, predictions, and estimation of population parameters, thus providing insights beyond the specific individuals or elements in the sample.

5. Ethical Considerations

In certain cases, collecting data from an entire population may pose ethical challenges, such as invasion of privacy or burdening participants. Sampling helps protect the privacy and well-being of individuals by reducing the burden of data collection. It allows researchers to obtain valuable information while ensuring ethical standards are maintained .

Key Steps Involved in the Sampling Process

Sampling is a valuable tool in research; however, it is important to carefully consider the sampling method, sample size, and potential biases to ensure that the findings accurately represent the larger population and are valid for making conclusions and generalizations. While the specific steps may vary depending on the research context, here is a general outline of the sampling process:

what is a population in psychological research

1. Define the Population

Clearly define the target population for your research study. The population should encompass the group of individuals, elements, or units that you want to draw conclusions about.

2. Define the Sampling Frame

Create a sampling frame, which is a list or representation of the individuals or elements in the target population. The sampling frame should be comprehensive and accurately reflect the population you want to study.

3. Determine the Sampling Method

Select an appropriate sampling method based on your research objectives, available resources, and the characteristics of the population. You can perform sampling by either utilizing probability-based or non-probability-based techniques. Common sampling methods include random sampling, stratified sampling, cluster sampling, and convenience sampling.

4. Determine Sample Size

Determine the desired sample size based on statistical considerations, such as the level of precision required, desired confidence level, and expected variability within the population. Larger sample sizes generally reduce sampling error but may be constrained by practical limitations.

5. Collect Data

Once the sample is selected using the appropriate technique, collect the necessary data according to the research design and data collection methods . Ensure that you use standardized and consistent data collection process that is also appropriate for your research objectives.

6. Analyze the Data

Perform the necessary statistical analyses on the collected data to derive meaningful insights. Use appropriate statistical techniques to make inferences, estimate population parameters, test hypotheses, or identify patterns and relationships within the data.

Population vs Sample — Differences and examples

While the population provides a comprehensive overview of the entire group under study, the sample, on the other hand, allows researchers to draw inferences and make generalizations about the population. Researchers should employ careful sampling techniques to ensure that the sample is representative and accurately reflects the characteristics and variability of the population.

what is a population in psychological research

Research Study: Investigating the prevalence of stress among high school students in a specific city and its impact on academic performance.

Population: All high school students in a particular city

Sampling Frame: The sampling frame would involve obtaining a comprehensive list of all high schools in the specific city. A random selection of schools would be made from this list to ensure representation from different areas and demographics of the city.

Sample: Randomly selected 500 high school students from different schools in the city

The sample represents a subset of the entire population of high school students in the city.

Research Study: Assessing the effectiveness of a new medication in managing symptoms and improving quality of life in patients with the specific medical condition.

Population: Patients diagnosed with a specific medical condition

Sampling Frame: The sampling frame for this study would involve accessing medical records or databases that include information on patients diagnosed with the specific medical condition. Researchers would select a convenient sample of patients who meet the inclusion criteria from the sampling frame.

Sample: Convenient sample of 100 patients from a local clinic who meet the inclusion criteria for the study

The sample consists of patients from the larger population of individuals diagnosed with the medical condition.

Research Study: Investigating community perceptions of safety and satisfaction with local amenities in the neighborhood.

Population: Residents of a specific neighborhood

Sampling Frame: The sampling frame for this study would involve obtaining a list of residential addresses within the specific neighborhood. Various sources such as census data, voter registration records, or community databases offer the means to obtain this information. From the sampling frame, researchers would randomly select a cluster sample of households to ensure representation from different areas within the neighborhood.

Sample: Cluster sample of 50 households randomly selected from different blocks within the neighborhood

The sample represents a subset of the entire population of residents living in the neighborhood.

To summarize, sampling allows for cost-effective data collection, easier statistical analysis, and increased practicality compared to studying the entire population. However, despite these advantages, sampling is subject to various challenges. These challenges include sampling bias, non-response bias, and the potential for sampling errors.

To minimize bias and enhance the validity of research findings , researchers should employ appropriate sampling techniques, clearly define the population, establish a comprehensive sampling frame, and monitor the sampling process for potential biases. Validating findings by comparing them to known population characteristics can also help evaluate the generalizability of the results. Properly understanding and implementing sampling techniques ensure that research findings are accurate, reliable, and representative of the larger population. By carefully considering the choice of population and sample, researchers can draw meaningful conclusions and, consequently, make valuable contributions to their respective fields of study.

Now, it’s your turn! Take a moment to think about a research question that interests you. Consider the population that would be relevant to your inquiry. Who would you include in your sample? How would you go about selecting them? Reflecting on these aspects will help you appreciate the intricacies involved in designing a research study. Let us know about it in the comment section below or reach out to us using  #AskEnago  and tag  @EnagoAcademy  on  Twitter ,  Facebook , and  Quora .

' src=

Thank you very much, this is helpful

Very impressive and helpful and also easy to understand….. Thanks to the Author and Publisher….

Rate this article Cancel Reply

Your email address will not be published.

what is a population in psychological research

Enago Academy's Most Popular Articles

retractions and research integrity

  • Publishing Research
  • Trending Now
  • Understanding Ethics

Understanding the Impact of Retractions on Research Integrity – A global study

As we reach the midway point of 2024, ‘Research Integrity’ remains one of the hot…

Gender Bias in Science Funding

  • Diversity and Inclusion

The Silent Struggle: Confronting gender bias in science funding

In the 1990s, Dr. Katalin Kariko’s pioneering mRNA research seemed destined for obscurity, doomed by…

Content Analysis vs Thematic Analysis: What's the difference?

  • Reporting Research

Choosing the Right Analytical Approach: Thematic analysis vs. content analysis for data interpretation

In research, choosing the right approach to understand data is crucial for deriving meaningful insights.…

Addressing Biases in the Journey of PhD

Addressing Barriers in Academia: Navigating unconscious biases in the Ph.D. journey

In the journey of academia, a Ph.D. marks a transitional phase, like that of a…

Cross-sectional and Longitudinal Study Design

Comparing Cross Sectional and Longitudinal Studies: 5 steps for choosing the right approach

The process of choosing the right research design can put ourselves at the crossroads of…

Choosing the Right Analytical Approach: Thematic analysis vs. content analysis for…

Comparing Cross Sectional and Longitudinal Studies: 5 steps for choosing the right…

what is a population in psychological research

Sign-up to read more

Subscribe for free to get unrestricted access to all our resources on research writing and academic publishing including:

  • 2000+ blog articles
  • 50+ Webinars
  • 10+ Expert podcasts
  • 50+ Infographics
  • 10+ Checklists
  • Research Guides

We hate spam too. We promise to protect your privacy and never spam you.

  • Industry News
  • AI in Academia
  • Promoting Research
  • Career Corner
  • Infographics
  • Expert Video Library
  • Other Resources
  • Enago Learn
  • Upcoming & On-Demand Webinars
  • Peer Review Week 2024
  • Open Access Week 2023
  • Conference Videos
  • Enago Report
  • Journal Finder
  • Enago Plagiarism & AI Grammar Check
  • Editing Services
  • Publication Support Services
  • Research Impact
  • Translation Services
  • Publication solutions
  • AI-Based Solutions
  • Thought Leadership
  • Call for Articles
  • Call for Speakers
  • Author Training
  • Edit Profile

I am looking for Editing/ Proofreading services for my manuscript Tentative date of next journal submission:

what is a population in psychological research

In your opinion, what is the most effective way to improve integrity in the peer review process?

Research Methods In Psychology

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

Research methods in psychology are systematic procedures used to observe, describe, predict, and explain behavior and mental processes. They include experiments, surveys, case studies, and naturalistic observations, ensuring data collection is objective and reliable to understand and explain psychological phenomena.

research methods3

Hypotheses are statements about the prediction of the results, that can be verified or disproved by some investigation.

There are four types of hypotheses :
  • Null Hypotheses (H0 ) – these predict that no difference will be found in the results between the conditions. Typically these are written ‘There will be no difference…’
  • Alternative Hypotheses (Ha or H1) – these predict that there will be a significant difference in the results between the two conditions. This is also known as the experimental hypothesis.
  • One-tailed (directional) hypotheses – these state the specific direction the researcher expects the results to move in, e.g. higher, lower, more, less. In a correlation study, the predicted direction of the correlation can be either positive or negative.
  • Two-tailed (non-directional) hypotheses – these state that a difference will be found between the conditions of the independent variable but does not state the direction of a difference or relationship. Typically these are always written ‘There will be a difference ….’

All research has an alternative hypothesis (either a one-tailed or two-tailed) and a corresponding null hypothesis.

Once the research is conducted and results are found, psychologists must accept one hypothesis and reject the other. 

So, if a difference is found, the Psychologist would accept the alternative hypothesis and reject the null.  The opposite applies if no difference is found.

Sampling techniques

Sampling is the process of selecting a representative group from the population under study.

Sample Target Population

A sample is the participants you select from a target population (the group you are interested in) to make generalizations about.

Representative means the extent to which a sample mirrors a researcher’s target population and reflects its characteristics.

Generalisability means the extent to which their findings can be applied to the larger population of which their sample was a part.

  • Volunteer sample : where participants pick themselves through newspaper adverts, noticeboards or online.
  • Opportunity sampling : also known as convenience sampling , uses people who are available at the time the study is carried out and willing to take part. It is based on convenience.
  • Random sampling : when every person in the target population has an equal chance of being selected. An example of random sampling would be picking names out of a hat.
  • Systematic sampling : when a system is used to select participants. Picking every Nth person from all possible participants. N = the number of people in the research population / the number of people needed for the sample.
  • Stratified sampling : when you identify the subgroups and select participants in proportion to their occurrences.
  • Snowball sampling : when researchers find a few participants, and then ask them to find participants themselves and so on.
  • Quota sampling : when researchers will be told to ensure the sample fits certain quotas, for example they might be told to find 90 participants, with 30 of them being unemployed.

Experiments always have an independent and dependent variable .

  • The independent variable is the one the experimenter manipulates (the thing that changes between the conditions the participants are placed into). It is assumed to have a direct effect on the dependent variable.
  • The dependent variable is the thing being measured, or the results of the experiment.

variables

Operationalization of variables means making them measurable/quantifiable. We must use operationalization to ensure that variables are in a form that can be easily tested.

For instance, we can’t really measure ‘happiness’, but we can measure how many times a person smiles within a two-hour period. 

By operationalizing variables, we make it easy for someone else to replicate our research. Remember, this is important because we can check if our findings are reliable.

Extraneous variables are all variables which are not independent variable but could affect the results of the experiment.

It can be a natural characteristic of the participant, such as intelligence levels, gender, or age for example, or it could be a situational feature of the environment such as lighting or noise.

Demand characteristics are a type of extraneous variable that occurs if the participants work out the aims of the research study, they may begin to behave in a certain way.

For example, in Milgram’s research , critics argued that participants worked out that the shocks were not real and they administered them as they thought this was what was required of them. 

Extraneous variables must be controlled so that they do not affect (confound) the results.

Randomly allocating participants to their conditions or using a matched pairs experimental design can help to reduce participant variables. 

Situational variables are controlled by using standardized procedures, ensuring every participant in a given condition is treated in the same way

Experimental Design

Experimental design refers to how participants are allocated to each condition of the independent variable, such as a control or experimental group.
  • Independent design ( between-groups design ): each participant is selected for only one group. With the independent design, the most common way of deciding which participants go into which group is by means of randomization. 
  • Matched participants design : each participant is selected for only one group, but the participants in the two groups are matched for some relevant factor or factors (e.g. ability; sex; age).
  • Repeated measures design ( within groups) : each participant appears in both groups, so that there are exactly the same participants in each group.
  • The main problem with the repeated measures design is that there may well be order effects. Their experiences during the experiment may change the participants in various ways.
  • They may perform better when they appear in the second group because they have gained useful information about the experiment or about the task. On the other hand, they may perform less well on the second occasion because of tiredness or boredom.
  • Counterbalancing is the best way of preventing order effects from disrupting the findings of an experiment, and involves ensuring that each condition is equally likely to be used first and second by the participants.

If we wish to compare two groups with respect to a given independent variable, it is essential to make sure that the two groups do not differ in any other important way. 

Experimental Methods

All experimental methods involve an iv (independent variable) and dv (dependent variable)..

The researcher decides where the experiment will take place, at what time, with which participants, in what circumstances,  using a standardized procedure.

  • Field experiments are conducted in the everyday (natural) environment of the participants. The experimenter still manipulates the IV, but in a real-life setting. It may be possible to control extraneous variables, though such control is more difficult than in a lab experiment.
  • Natural experiments are when a naturally occurring IV is investigated that isn’t deliberately manipulated, it exists anyway. Participants are not randomly allocated, and the natural event may only occur rarely.

Case studies are in-depth investigations of a person, group, event, or community. It uses information from a range of sources, such as from the person concerned and also from their family and friends.

Many techniques may be used such as interviews, psychological tests, observations and experiments. Case studies are generally longitudinal: in other words, they follow the individual or group over an extended period of time. 

Case studies are widely used in psychology and among the best-known ones carried out were by Sigmund Freud . He conducted very detailed investigations into the private lives of his patients in an attempt to both understand and help them overcome their illnesses.

Case studies provide rich qualitative data and have high levels of ecological validity. However, it is difficult to generalize from individual cases as each one has unique characteristics.

Correlational Studies

Correlation means association; it is a measure of the extent to which two variables are related. One of the variables can be regarded as the predictor variable with the other one as the outcome variable.

Correlational studies typically involve obtaining two different measures from a group of participants, and then assessing the degree of association between the measures. 

The predictor variable can be seen as occurring before the outcome variable in some sense. It is called the predictor variable, because it forms the basis for predicting the value of the outcome variable.

Relationships between variables can be displayed on a graph or as a numerical score called a correlation coefficient.

types of correlation. Scatter plot. Positive negative and no correlation

  • If an increase in one variable tends to be associated with an increase in the other, then this is known as a positive correlation .
  • If an increase in one variable tends to be associated with a decrease in the other, then this is known as a negative correlation .
  • A zero correlation occurs when there is no relationship between variables.

After looking at the scattergraph, if we want to be sure that a significant relationship does exist between the two variables, a statistical test of correlation can be conducted, such as Spearman’s rho.

The test will give us a score, called a correlation coefficient . This is a value between 0 and 1, and the closer to 1 the score is, the stronger the relationship between the variables. This value can be both positive e.g. 0.63, or negative -0.63.

Types of correlation. Strong, weak, and perfect positive correlation, strong, weak, and perfect negative correlation, no correlation. Graphs or charts ...

A correlation between variables, however, does not automatically mean that the change in one variable is the cause of the change in the values of the other variable. A correlation only shows if there is a relationship between variables.

Correlation does not always prove causation, as a third variable may be involved. 

causation correlation

Interview Methods

Interviews are commonly divided into two types: structured and unstructured.

A fixed, predetermined set of questions is put to every participant in the same order and in the same way. 

Responses are recorded on a questionnaire, and the researcher presets the order and wording of questions, and sometimes the range of alternative answers.

The interviewer stays within their role and maintains social distance from the interviewee.

There are no set questions, and the participant can raise whatever topics he/she feels are relevant and ask them in their own way. Questions are posed about participants’ answers to the subject

Unstructured interviews are most useful in qualitative research to analyze attitudes and values.

Though they rarely provide a valid basis for generalization, their main advantage is that they enable the researcher to probe social actors’ subjective point of view. 

Questionnaire Method

Questionnaires can be thought of as a kind of written interview. They can be carried out face to face, by telephone, or post.

The choice of questions is important because of the need to avoid bias or ambiguity in the questions, ‘leading’ the respondent or causing offense.

  • Open questions are designed to encourage a full, meaningful answer using the subject’s own knowledge and feelings. They provide insights into feelings, opinions, and understanding. Example: “How do you feel about that situation?”
  • Closed questions can be answered with a simple “yes” or “no” or specific information, limiting the depth of response. They are useful for gathering specific facts or confirming details. Example: “Do you feel anxious in crowds?”

Its other practical advantages are that it is cheaper than face-to-face interviews and can be used to contact many respondents scattered over a wide area relatively quickly.

Observations

There are different types of observation methods :
  • Covert observation is where the researcher doesn’t tell the participants they are being observed until after the study is complete. There could be ethical problems or deception and consent with this particular observation method.
  • Overt observation is where a researcher tells the participants they are being observed and what they are being observed for.
  • Controlled : behavior is observed under controlled laboratory conditions (e.g., Bandura’s Bobo doll study).
  • Natural : Here, spontaneous behavior is recorded in a natural setting.
  • Participant : Here, the observer has direct contact with the group of people they are observing. The researcher becomes a member of the group they are researching.  
  • Non-participant (aka “fly on the wall): The researcher does not have direct contact with the people being observed. The observation of participants’ behavior is from a distance

Pilot Study

A pilot  study is a small scale preliminary study conducted in order to evaluate the feasibility of the key s teps in a future, full-scale project.

A pilot study is an initial run-through of the procedures to be used in an investigation; it involves selecting a few people and trying out the study on them. It is possible to save time, and in some cases, money, by identifying any flaws in the procedures designed by the researcher.

A pilot study can help the researcher spot any ambiguities (i.e. unusual things) or confusion in the information given to participants or problems with the task devised.

Sometimes the task is too hard, and the researcher may get a floor effect, because none of the participants can score at all or can complete the task – all performances are low.

The opposite effect is a ceiling effect, when the task is so easy that all achieve virtually full marks or top performances and are “hitting the ceiling”.

Research Design

In cross-sectional research , a researcher compares multiple segments of the population at the same time

Sometimes, we want to see how people change over time, as in studies of human development and lifespan. Longitudinal research is a research design in which data-gathering is administered repeatedly over an extended period of time.

In cohort studies , the participants must share a common factor or characteristic such as age, demographic, or occupation. A cohort study is a type of longitudinal study in which researchers monitor and observe a chosen population over an extended period.

Triangulation means using more than one research method to improve the study’s validity.

Reliability

Reliability is a measure of consistency, if a particular measurement is repeated and the same result is obtained then it is described as being reliable.

  • Test-retest reliability :  assessing the same person on two different occasions which shows the extent to which the test produces the same answers.
  • Inter-observer reliability : the extent to which there is an agreement between two or more observers.

Meta-Analysis

Meta-analysis is a statistical procedure used to combine and synthesize findings from multiple independent studies to estimate the average effect size for a particular research question.

Meta-analysis goes beyond traditional narrative reviews by using statistical methods to integrate the results of several studies, leading to a more objective appraisal of the evidence.

This is done by looking through various databases, and then decisions are made about what studies are to be included/excluded.

  • Strengths : Increases the conclusions’ validity as they’re based on a wider range.
  • Weaknesses : Research designs in studies can vary, so they are not truly comparable.

Peer Review

A researcher submits an article to a journal. The choice of the journal may be determined by the journal’s audience or prestige.

The journal selects two or more appropriate experts (psychologists working in a similar field) to peer review the article without payment. The peer reviewers assess: the methods and designs used, originality of the findings, the validity of the original research findings and its content, structure and language.

Feedback from the reviewer determines whether the article is accepted. The article may be: Accepted as it is, accepted with revisions, sent back to the author to revise and re-submit or rejected without the possibility of submission.

The editor makes the final decision whether to accept or reject the research report based on the reviewers comments/ recommendations.

Peer review is important because it prevent faulty data from entering the public domain, it provides a way of checking the validity of findings and the quality of the methodology and is used to assess the research rating of university departments.

Peer reviews may be an ideal, whereas in practice there are lots of problems. For example, it slows publication down and may prevent unusual, new work being published. Some reviewers might use it as an opportunity to prevent competing researchers from publishing work.

Some people doubt whether peer review can really prevent the publication of fraudulent research.

The advent of the internet means that a lot of research and academic comment is being published without official peer reviews than before, though systems are evolving on the internet where everyone really has a chance to offer their opinions and police the quality of research.

Types of Data

  • Quantitative data is numerical data e.g. reaction time or number of mistakes. It represents how much or how long, how many there are of something. A tally of behavioral categories and closed questions in a questionnaire collect quantitative data.
  • Qualitative data is virtually any type of information that can be observed and recorded that is not numerical in nature and can be in the form of written or verbal communication. Open questions in questionnaires and accounts from observational studies collect qualitative data.
  • Primary data is first-hand data collected for the purpose of the investigation.
  • Secondary data is information that has been collected by someone other than the person who is conducting the research e.g. taken from journals, books or articles.

Validity means how well a piece of research actually measures what it sets out to, or how well it reflects the reality it claims to represent.

Validity is whether the observed effect is genuine and represents what is actually out there in the world.

  • Concurrent validity is the extent to which a psychological measure relates to an existing similar measure and obtains close results. For example, a new intelligence test compared to an established test.
  • Face validity : does the test measure what it’s supposed to measure ‘on the face of it’. This is done by ‘eyeballing’ the measuring or by passing it to an expert to check.
  • Ecological validit y is the extent to which findings from a research study can be generalized to other settings / real life.
  • Temporal validity is the extent to which findings from a research study can be generalized to other historical times.

Features of Science

  • Paradigm – A set of shared assumptions and agreed methods within a scientific discipline.
  • Paradigm shift – The result of the scientific revolution: a significant change in the dominant unifying theory within a scientific discipline.
  • Objectivity – When all sources of personal bias are minimised so not to distort or influence the research process.
  • Empirical method – Scientific approaches that are based on the gathering of evidence through direct observation and experience.
  • Replicability – The extent to which scientific procedures and findings can be repeated by other researchers.
  • Falsifiability – The principle that a theory cannot be considered scientific unless it admits the possibility of being proved untrue.

Statistical Testing

A significant result is one where there is a low probability that chance factors were responsible for any observed difference, correlation, or association in the variables tested.

If our test is significant, we can reject our null hypothesis and accept our alternative hypothesis.

If our test is not significant, we can accept our null hypothesis and reject our alternative hypothesis. A null hypothesis is a statement of no effect.

In Psychology, we use p < 0.05 (as it strikes a balance between making a type I and II error) but p < 0.01 is used in tests that could cause harm like introducing a new drug.

A type I error is when the null hypothesis is rejected when it should have been accepted (happens when a lenient significance level is used, an error of optimism).

A type II error is when the null hypothesis is accepted when it should have been rejected (happens when a stringent significance level is used, an error of pessimism).

Ethical Issues

  • Informed consent is when participants are able to make an informed judgment about whether to take part. It causes them to guess the aims of the study and change their behavior.
  • To deal with it, we can gain presumptive consent or ask them to formally indicate their agreement to participate but it may invalidate the purpose of the study and it is not guaranteed that the participants would understand.
  • Deception should only be used when it is approved by an ethics committee, as it involves deliberately misleading or withholding information. Participants should be fully debriefed after the study but debriefing can’t turn the clock back.
  • All participants should be informed at the beginning that they have the right to withdraw if they ever feel distressed or uncomfortable.
  • It causes bias as the ones that stayed are obedient and some may not withdraw as they may have been given incentives or feel like they’re spoiling the study. Researchers can offer the right to withdraw data after participation.
  • Participants should all have protection from harm . The researcher should avoid risks greater than those experienced in everyday life and they should stop the study if any harm is suspected. However, the harm may not be apparent at the time of the study.
  • Confidentiality concerns the communication of personal information. The researchers should not record any names but use numbers or false names though it may not be possible as it is sometimes possible to work out who the researchers were.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Sweepstakes
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

How and Why Sampling Is Used in Psychology Research

Verywell / Nusha Ashjaee  

  • Why Use Samples
  • Probability Samples
  • Nonprobability Samples

Sampling Errors

In statistics, a sample is a subset of a population that is used to represent the entire group as a whole. When doing psychology research, it is often impractical to survey every member of a particular population because the number of people is simply too large. To make inferences about the characteristics of a population, psychology researchers use a random sample .

Keep reading to learn about how samples are used in psychology research, the different types of samples, and the errors that may occur when using samples.

Why Psychology Researchers Use Samples

When researching an aspect of the human mind or ​ human behavior , psychology researchers can rarely collect data from every single affected individual. Instead, they use a smaller sample of individuals to represent the larger group.

The goal when choosing a sample is to make sure it represents the entire group accurately. This means that the sample should reflect the diverse characteristics present in the total population. The sample must accurately represent the population in question so researchers can generalize their results to the larger group with statistical analysis .

In psychological research and other types of social science research , experimenters typically rely on a few different sampling methods. These can be grouped into probability and nonprobability samples.

Types of Probability Samples

Probability sampling means every individual in a population stands a chance of being selected. Because probability sampling uses random selection , every subset of the population has an equal chance of being represented in the sample.

Probability samples are more representative of large populations and researchers are better able to generalize their results to the group as a whole when they use probability samples.

Simple Random Sampling

Simple random sampling is, as the name suggests, the simplest type of probability sampling. Psychology researchers take every individual in a population and randomly select individuals to compose their sample, often by using some type of computer program or random number generator.

Stratified Random Sampling

Stratified random sampling involves separating the population into subgroups and then taking a simple random sample from each of these subgroups. For example, researchers may divide the population into subgroups based on race, sex, or age, and then take a simple random sample of each of these groups.

Stratified random sampling often provides greater statistical accuracy than simple random sampling because it ensures each of the subgroups is accurately represented in the sample.

Cluster Sampling

Cluster sampling involves dividing a population into smaller clusters, often based on geographic location. A random sample of these clusters is then selected, and all of the subjects within the cluster are measured.

For example, imagine you are doing a study on school principals in your state. Collecting data from every single school principal would be cost-prohibitive and time-consuming. But, if you were to use a cluster sampling method, you would randomly select five counties from your state and then collect data from every subject in each of those five counties to create a representative sample.

Probability sampling methods allow psychology researchers to get a more representative sample. Techniques that might be used include simple random sampling, stratified random sampling, and cluster sampling.

Types of Nonprobability Samples

Nonprobability sampling involves selecting participants using methods that do not give every subset of a population an equal chance of being represented. For example, a study may recruit participants from an already established group of volunteers.

One problem with this type of sample is that volunteers might differ from non-volunteers on certain variables, which can make it difficult to generalize the results to the entire population.

Convenience Sampling

Convenience sampling involves selecting participants for a study based on what is most convenient–people who are easily accessible and have the time. If you have ever volunteered for a psychology study conducted through your university's psychology department, then you have participated in a study that relied on a convenience sample.

Studies that rely on asking for volunteers or using clinical samples available to the researcher are also examples of convenience samples.

Purposive Sampling

Purposive sampling involves seeking out individuals who meet certain criteria. For example, a researcher might be interested in learning how college graduates between the ages of 20 and 35 feel about a topic. In purposive sampling, the researcher might conduct telephone interviews to intentionally seek out people who meet their criteria.

Quota Sampling

Quota sampling involves intentionally sampling specific proportions of each subgroup within a population. For example, political pollsters might be interested in researching the opinions of a population on a certain political issue. If they use simple random sampling, they might miss certain subsets of the population by chance.

Instead, they establish criteria to assign each subgroup a certain percentage of the sample. This differs from stratified sampling because, to find individuals within each subgroup, researchers use non-random methods to fill the quotas for each subgroup.

Nonprobability sampling can also be used when selecting a sample in psychology research. Such methods are less representative of the general population. Techniques include convenience sampling, purposive sampling, and quota sampling.

Sampling errors are differences between what is present in a population and what is present in a sample. Because sampling cannot include every single individual in a population, errors can occur. This can ultimately have an impact on the results of psychology research.

While it is impossible to know exactly how great the difference between the population and sample may be, researchers can statistically estimate the size of the sampling errors. In political polls, for example, you might often hear of the margin of errors expressed by certain confidence levels.

In general, the larger the sample size, the smaller the level of error. This is simply because the closer the sample is to the size of the total population, the more likely it is to accurately capture all of the characteristics of the population.

The only way to completely eliminate sampling error is to collect data from the entire population, which is often simply too costly and time-consuming. Sampling errors can be minimized, however, by using randomized probability testing and large sample size.

Samples are important in psychology research because they allow scientists to study what is happening in a larger population without having to reach every individual in the entire group.

Different types of samples can be used depending on what researchers are studying and the resources they have available to collect data. Probability samples tend to be more representative of the larger group. Nonprobability samples, on the other hand, tend to involve selecting participants based on availability and studying specific subsets of a larger group, which is less representative of the larger group.

Sampling errors can occur, however, with any type of sampling. To minimize errors, researchers strive to use large, representative samples.

Valliant R, Dever J. Estimating propensity adjustments for volunteer web surveys . Sociol Methods Res . 2011;40(1):105-137. doi:10.1177/0049124110392533

Lin L. Bias caused by sampling error in meta-analysis with small sample sizes . PLoS ONE . 2018;13(9):e0204056. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0204056

Goodwin CJ. Research In Psychology: Methods and Design, 12th ed . John Wiley and Sons.

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

APS

Misinformation vs. Disinformation: What the Terms Mean and the Effects They Have

  • misinformation
  • Social Media

Loose lips sink ships − especially when they’re not telling the truth. Spotting misinformation can be difficult, especially on an information super-highway like the internet. 

“There is a myriad of consequences: From cynicism of government, the media, and science, to behaviors that harm individuals and others … to large scale damage to public property, to insurrection,” Dolores Albarracin , a psychology professor at the University of Illinois who studies attitudes, communication and behavior told USA TODAY in 2021.

“We have a lot more little bits of fiction roaming around in those memory banks than probably people would realize or would like to think is true,” Elizabeth Loftus , a University of California, Irvine professor who has done groundbreaking research on the misinformation effect, explained.

Read the whole story (subscription may be required): USA TODAY

APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines .

Please login with your APS account to comment.

what is a population in psychological research

Visual Memory Distortions Paint a Picture of the Past That Never Was 

Basic research on our imperfect visual memories is bringing to light how and why we may misremember what we have seen.

what is a population in psychological research

Scientists Discuss How to Study the Psychology of Collectives, Not Just Individuals

In a set of articles appearing in Perspectives on Psychological Science, an international array of scientists discusses how the study of neighborhoods, work units, activist groups, and other collectives can help us better understand and respond to societal changes.

what is a population in psychological research

Cattell Fund Projects Include Research on Children’s Executive Function, Empathy Choice, and More

The James McKeen Cattell Fund has recognized APS Fellow Stephanie M. Carlson, C. Daryl Cameron, Robert Hampton, and Kevin Holmes as recipients of its Sabbatical Fund Fellowship for 2023–2024.

Privacy Overview

CookieDurationDescription
__cf_bm30 minutesThis cookie, set by Cloudflare, is used to support Cloudflare Bot Management.
CookieDurationDescription
AWSELBCORS5 minutesThis cookie is used by Elastic Load Balancing from Amazon Web Services to effectively balance load on the servers.
CookieDurationDescription
at-randneverAddThis sets this cookie to track page visits, sources of traffic and share counts.
CONSENT2 yearsYouTube sets this cookie via embedded youtube-videos and registers anonymous statistical data.
uvc1 year 27 daysSet by addthis.com to determine the usage of addthis.com service.
_ga2 yearsThe _ga cookie, installed by Google Analytics, calculates visitor, session and campaign data and also keeps track of site usage for the site's analytics report. The cookie stores information anonymously and assigns a randomly generated number to recognize unique visitors.
_gat_gtag_UA_3507334_11 minuteSet by Google to distinguish users.
_gid1 dayInstalled by Google Analytics, _gid cookie stores information on how visitors use a website, while also creating an analytics report of the website's performance. Some of the data that are collected include the number of visitors, their source, and the pages they visit anonymously.
CookieDurationDescription
loc1 year 27 daysAddThis sets this geolocation cookie to help understand the location of users who share the information.
VISITOR_INFO1_LIVE5 months 27 daysA cookie set by YouTube to measure bandwidth that determines whether the user gets the new or old player interface.
YSCsessionYSC cookie is set by Youtube and is used to track the views of embedded videos on Youtube pages.
yt-remote-connected-devicesneverYouTube sets this cookie to store the video preferences of the user using embedded YouTube video.
yt-remote-device-idneverYouTube sets this cookie to store the video preferences of the user using embedded YouTube video.
yt.innertube::nextIdneverThis cookie, set by YouTube, registers a unique ID to store data on what videos from YouTube the user has seen.
yt.innertube::requestsneverThis cookie, set by YouTube, registers a unique ID to store data on what videos from YouTube the user has seen.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Res Metr Anal

Logo of frontrma

The Use of Research Methods in Psychological Research: A Systematised Review

Salomé elizabeth scholtz.

1 Community Psychosocial Research (COMPRES), School of Psychosocial Health, North-West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa

Werner de Klerk

Leon t. de beer.

2 WorkWell Research Institute, North-West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa

Research methods play an imperative role in research quality as well as educating young researchers, however, the application thereof is unclear which can be detrimental to the field of psychology. Therefore, this systematised review aimed to determine what research methods are being used, how these methods are being used and for what topics in the field. Our review of 999 articles from five journals over a period of 5 years indicated that psychology research is conducted in 10 topics via predominantly quantitative research methods. Of these 10 topics, social psychology was the most popular. The remainder of the conducted methodology is described. It was also found that articles lacked rigour and transparency in the used methodology which has implications for replicability. In conclusion this article, provides an overview of all reported methodologies used in a sample of psychology journals. It highlights the popularity and application of methods and designs throughout the article sample as well as an unexpected lack of rigour with regard to most aspects of methodology. Possible sample bias should be considered when interpreting the results of this study. It is recommended that future research should utilise the results of this study to determine the possible impact on the field of psychology as a science and to further investigation into the use of research methods. Results should prompt the following future research into: a lack or rigour and its implication on replication, the use of certain methods above others, publication bias and choice of sampling method.

Introduction

Psychology is an ever-growing and popular field (Gough and Lyons, 2016 ; Clay, 2017 ). Due to this growth and the need for science-based research to base health decisions on (Perestelo-Pérez, 2013 ), the use of research methods in the broad field of psychology is an essential point of investigation (Stangor, 2011 ; Aanstoos, 2014 ). Research methods are therefore viewed as important tools used by researchers to collect data (Nieuwenhuis, 2016 ) and include the following: quantitative, qualitative, mixed method and multi method (Maree, 2016 ). Additionally, researchers also employ various types of literature reviews to address research questions (Grant and Booth, 2009 ). According to literature, what research method is used and why a certain research method is used is complex as it depends on various factors that may include paradigm (O'Neil and Koekemoer, 2016 ), research question (Grix, 2002 ), or the skill and exposure of the researcher (Nind et al., 2015 ). How these research methods are employed is also difficult to discern as research methods are often depicted as having fixed boundaries that are continuously crossed in research (Johnson et al., 2001 ; Sandelowski, 2011 ). Examples of this crossing include adding quantitative aspects to qualitative studies (Sandelowski et al., 2009 ), or stating that a study used a mixed-method design without the study having any characteristics of this design (Truscott et al., 2010 ).

The inappropriate use of research methods affects how students and researchers improve and utilise their research skills (Scott Jones and Goldring, 2015 ), how theories are developed (Ngulube, 2013 ), and the credibility of research results (Levitt et al., 2017 ). This, in turn, can be detrimental to the field (Nind et al., 2015 ), journal publication (Ketchen et al., 2008 ; Ezeh et al., 2010 ), and attempts to address public social issues through psychological research (Dweck, 2017 ). This is especially important given the now well-known replication crisis the field is facing (Earp and Trafimow, 2015 ; Hengartner, 2018 ).

Due to this lack of clarity on method use and the potential impact of inept use of research methods, the aim of this study was to explore the use of research methods in the field of psychology through a review of journal publications. Chaichanasakul et al. ( 2011 ) identify reviewing articles as the opportunity to examine the development, growth and progress of a research area and overall quality of a journal. Studies such as Lee et al. ( 1999 ) as well as Bluhm et al. ( 2011 ) review of qualitative methods has attempted to synthesis the use of research methods and indicated the growth of qualitative research in American and European journals. Research has also focused on the use of research methods in specific sub-disciplines of psychology, for example, in the field of Industrial and Organisational psychology Coetzee and Van Zyl ( 2014 ) found that South African publications tend to consist of cross-sectional quantitative research methods with underrepresented longitudinal studies. Qualitative studies were found to make up 21% of the articles published from 1995 to 2015 in a similar study by O'Neil and Koekemoer ( 2016 ). Other methods in health psychology, such as Mixed methods research have also been reportedly growing in popularity (O'Cathain, 2009 ).

A broad overview of the use of research methods in the field of psychology as a whole is however, not available in the literature. Therefore, our research focused on answering what research methods are being used, how these methods are being used and for what topics in practice (i.e., journal publications) in order to provide a general perspective of method used in psychology publication. We synthesised the collected data into the following format: research topic [areas of scientific discourse in a field or the current needs of a population (Bittermann and Fischer, 2018 )], method [data-gathering tools (Nieuwenhuis, 2016 )], sampling [elements chosen from a population to partake in research (Ritchie et al., 2009 )], data collection [techniques and research strategy (Maree, 2016 )], and data analysis [discovering information by examining bodies of data (Ktepi, 2016 )]. A systematised review of recent articles (2013 to 2017) collected from five different journals in the field of psychological research was conducted.

Grant and Booth ( 2009 ) describe systematised reviews as the review of choice for post-graduate studies, which is employed using some elements of a systematic review and seldom more than one or two databases to catalogue studies after a comprehensive literature search. The aspects used in this systematised review that are similar to that of a systematic review were a full search within the chosen database and data produced in tabular form (Grant and Booth, 2009 ).

Sample sizes and timelines vary in systematised reviews (see Lowe and Moore, 2014 ; Pericall and Taylor, 2014 ; Barr-Walker, 2017 ). With no clear parameters identified in the literature (see Grant and Booth, 2009 ), the sample size of this study was determined by the purpose of the sample (Strydom, 2011 ), and time and cost constraints (Maree and Pietersen, 2016 ). Thus, a non-probability purposive sample (Ritchie et al., 2009 ) of the top five psychology journals from 2013 to 2017 was included in this research study. Per Lee ( 2015 ) American Psychological Association (APA) recommends the use of the most up-to-date sources for data collection with consideration of the context of the research study. As this research study focused on the most recent trends in research methods used in the broad field of psychology, the identified time frame was deemed appropriate.

Psychology journals were only included if they formed part of the top five English journals in the miscellaneous psychology domain of the Scimago Journal and Country Rank (Scimago Journal & Country Rank, 2017 ). The Scimago Journal and Country Rank provides a yearly updated list of publicly accessible journal and country-specific indicators derived from the Scopus® database (Scopus, 2017b ) by means of the Scimago Journal Rank (SJR) indicator developed by Scimago from the algorithm Google PageRank™ (Scimago Journal & Country Rank, 2017 ). Scopus is the largest global database of abstracts and citations from peer-reviewed journals (Scopus, 2017a ). Reasons for the development of the Scimago Journal and Country Rank list was to allow researchers to assess scientific domains, compare country rankings, and compare and analyse journals (Scimago Journal & Country Rank, 2017 ), which supported the aim of this research study. Additionally, the goals of the journals had to focus on topics in psychology in general with no preference to specific research methods and have full-text access to articles.

The following list of top five journals in 2018 fell within the abovementioned inclusion criteria (1) Australian Journal of Psychology, (2) British Journal of Psychology, (3) Europe's Journal of Psychology, (4) International Journal of Psychology and lastly the (5) Journal of Psychology Applied and Interdisciplinary.

Journals were excluded from this systematised review if no full-text versions of their articles were available, if journals explicitly stated a publication preference for certain research methods, or if the journal only published articles in a specific discipline of psychological research (for example, industrial psychology, clinical psychology etc.).

The researchers followed a procedure (see Figure 1 ) adapted from that of Ferreira et al. ( 2016 ) for systematised reviews. Data collection and categorisation commenced on 4 December 2017 and continued until 30 June 2019. All the data was systematically collected and coded manually (Grant and Booth, 2009 ) with an independent person acting as co-coder. Codes of interest included the research topic, method used, the design used, sampling method, and methodology (the method used for data collection and data analysis). These codes were derived from the wording in each article. Themes were created based on the derived codes and checked by the co-coder. Lastly, these themes were catalogued into a table as per the systematised review design.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is frma-05-00001-g0001.jpg

Systematised review procedure.

According to Johnston et al. ( 2019 ), “literature screening, selection, and data extraction/analyses” (p. 7) are specifically tailored to the aim of a review. Therefore, the steps followed in a systematic review must be reported in a comprehensive and transparent manner. The chosen systematised design adhered to the rigour expected from systematic reviews with regard to full search and data produced in tabular form (Grant and Booth, 2009 ). The rigorous application of the systematic review is, therefore discussed in relation to these two elements.

Firstly, to ensure a comprehensive search, this research study promoted review transparency by following a clear protocol outlined according to each review stage before collecting data (Johnston et al., 2019 ). This protocol was similar to that of Ferreira et al. ( 2016 ) and approved by three research committees/stakeholders and the researchers (Johnston et al., 2019 ). The eligibility criteria for article inclusion was based on the research question and clearly stated, and the process of inclusion was recorded on an electronic spreadsheet to create an evidence trail (Bandara et al., 2015 ; Johnston et al., 2019 ). Microsoft Excel spreadsheets are a popular tool for review studies and can increase the rigour of the review process (Bandara et al., 2015 ). Screening for appropriate articles for inclusion forms an integral part of a systematic review process (Johnston et al., 2019 ). This step was applied to two aspects of this research study: the choice of eligible journals and articles to be included. Suitable journals were selected by the first author and reviewed by the second and third authors. Initially, all articles from the chosen journals were included. Then, by process of elimination, those irrelevant to the research aim, i.e., interview articles or discussions etc., were excluded.

To ensure rigourous data extraction, data was first extracted by one reviewer, and an independent person verified the results for completeness and accuracy (Johnston et al., 2019 ). The research question served as a guide for efficient, organised data extraction (Johnston et al., 2019 ). Data was categorised according to the codes of interest, along with article identifiers for audit trails such as authors, title and aims of articles. The categorised data was based on the aim of the review (Johnston et al., 2019 ) and synthesised in tabular form under methods used, how these methods were used, and for what topics in the field of psychology.

The initial search produced a total of 1,145 articles from the 5 journals identified. Inclusion and exclusion criteria resulted in a final sample of 999 articles ( Figure 2 ). Articles were co-coded into 84 codes, from which 10 themes were derived ( Table 1 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is frma-05-00001-g0002.jpg

Journal article frequency.

Codes used to form themes (research topics).

Social Psychology31Aggression SP, Attitude SP, Belief SP, Child abuse SP, Conflict SP, Culture SP, Discrimination SP, Economic, Family illness, Family, Group, Help, Immigration, Intergeneration, Judgement, Law, Leadership, Marriage SP, Media, Optimism, Organisational and Social justice, Parenting SP, Politics, Prejudice, Relationships, Religion, Romantic Relationships SP, Sex and attraction, Stereotype, Violence, Work
Experimental Psychology17Anxiety, stress and PTSD, Coping, Depression, Emotion, Empathy, Facial research, Fear and threat, Happiness, Humor, Mindfulness, Mortality, Motivation and Achievement, Perception, Rumination, Self, Self-efficacy
Cognitive Psychology12Attention, Cognition, Decision making, Impulse, Intelligence, Language, Math, Memory, Mental, Number, Problem solving, Reading
Health Psychology7Addiction, Body, Burnout, Health, Illness (Health Psychology), Sleep (Health Psychology), Suicide and Self-harm
Physiological Psychology6Gender, Health (Physiological psychology), Illness (Physiological psychology), Mood disorders, Sleep (Physiological psychology), Visual research
Developmental Psychology3Attachment, Development, Old age
Personality3Machiavellian, Narcissism, Personality
Psychological Psychology3Programme, Psychology practice, Theory
Education and Learning1Education and Learning
Psychometrics1Measure
Code Total84

These 10 themes represent the topic section of our research question ( Figure 3 ). All these topics except, for the final one, psychological practice , were found to concur with the research areas in psychology as identified by Weiten ( 2010 ). These research areas were chosen to represent the derived codes as they provided broad definitions that allowed for clear, concise categorisation of the vast amount of data. Article codes were categorised under particular themes/topics if they adhered to the research area definitions created by Weiten ( 2010 ). It is important to note that these areas of research do not refer to specific disciplines in psychology, such as industrial psychology; but to broader fields that may encompass sub-interests of these disciplines.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is frma-05-00001-g0003.jpg

Topic frequency (international sample).

In the case of developmental psychology , researchers conduct research into human development from childhood to old age. Social psychology includes research on behaviour governed by social drivers. Researchers in the field of educational psychology study how people learn and the best way to teach them. Health psychology aims to determine the effect of psychological factors on physiological health. Physiological psychology , on the other hand, looks at the influence of physiological aspects on behaviour. Experimental psychology is not the only theme that uses experimental research and focuses on the traditional core topics of psychology (for example, sensation). Cognitive psychology studies the higher mental processes. Psychometrics is concerned with measuring capacity or behaviour. Personality research aims to assess and describe consistency in human behaviour (Weiten, 2010 ). The final theme of psychological practice refers to the experiences, techniques, and interventions employed by practitioners, researchers, and academia in the field of psychology.

Articles under these themes were further subdivided into methodologies: method, sampling, design, data collection, and data analysis. The categorisation was based on information stated in the articles and not inferred by the researchers. Data were compiled into two sets of results presented in this article. The first set addresses the aim of this study from the perspective of the topics identified. The second set of results represents a broad overview of the results from the perspective of the methodology employed. The second set of results are discussed in this article, while the first set is presented in table format. The discussion thus provides a broad overview of methods use in psychology (across all themes), while the table format provides readers with in-depth insight into methods used in the individual themes identified. We believe that presenting the data from both perspectives allow readers a broad understanding of the results. Due a large amount of information that made up our results, we followed Cichocka and Jost ( 2014 ) in simplifying our results. Please note that the numbers indicated in the table in terms of methodology differ from the total number of articles. Some articles employed more than one method/sampling technique/design/data collection method/data analysis in their studies.

What follows is the results for what methods are used, how these methods are used, and which topics in psychology they are applied to . Percentages are reported to the second decimal in order to highlight small differences in the occurrence of methodology.

Firstly, with regard to the research methods used, our results show that researchers are more likely to use quantitative research methods (90.22%) compared to all other research methods. Qualitative research was the second most common research method but only made up about 4.79% of the general method usage. Reviews occurred almost as much as qualitative studies (3.91%), as the third most popular method. Mixed-methods research studies (0.98%) occurred across most themes, whereas multi-method research was indicated in only one study and amounted to 0.10% of the methods identified. The specific use of each method in the topics identified is shown in Table 2 and Figure 4 .

Research methods in psychology.

Quantitative4011626960525248283813
Qualitative28410523501
Review115203411301
Mixed Methods7000101100
Multi-method0000000010
Total4471717260615853473915

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is frma-05-00001-g0004.jpg

Research method frequency in topics.

Secondly, in the case of how these research methods are employed , our study indicated the following.

Sampling −78.34% of the studies in the collected articles did not specify a sampling method. From the remainder of the studies, 13 types of sampling methods were identified. These sampling methods included broad categorisation of a sample as, for example, a probability or non-probability sample. General samples of convenience were the methods most likely to be applied (10.34%), followed by random sampling (3.51%), snowball sampling (2.73%), and purposive (1.37%) and cluster sampling (1.27%). The remainder of the sampling methods occurred to a more limited extent (0–1.0%). See Table 3 and Figure 5 for sampling methods employed in each topic.

Sampling use in the field of psychology.

Not stated3311534557494343383114
Convenience sampling558101689261
Random sampling15391220211
Snowball sampling14441200300
Purposive sampling6020020310
Cluster sampling8120020000
Stratified sampling4120110000
Non-probability sampling4010000010
Probability sampling3100000000
Quota sampling1010000000
Criterion sampling1000000000
Self-selection sampling1000000000
Unsystematic sampling0100000000
Total4431727660605852484016

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is frma-05-00001-g0005.jpg

Sampling method frequency in topics.

Designs were categorised based on the articles' statement thereof. Therefore, it is important to note that, in the case of quantitative studies, non-experimental designs (25.55%) were often indicated due to a lack of experiments and any other indication of design, which, according to Laher ( 2016 ), is a reasonable categorisation. Non-experimental designs should thus be compared with experimental designs only in the description of data, as it could include the use of correlational/cross-sectional designs, which were not overtly stated by the authors. For the remainder of the research methods, “not stated” (7.12%) was assigned to articles without design types indicated.

From the 36 identified designs the most popular designs were cross-sectional (23.17%) and experimental (25.64%), which concurred with the high number of quantitative studies. Longitudinal studies (3.80%), the third most popular design, was used in both quantitative and qualitative studies. Qualitative designs consisted of ethnography (0.38%), interpretative phenomenological designs/phenomenology (0.28%), as well as narrative designs (0.28%). Studies that employed the review method were mostly categorised as “not stated,” with the most often stated review designs being systematic reviews (0.57%). The few mixed method studies employed exploratory, explanatory (0.09%), and concurrent designs (0.19%), with some studies referring to separate designs for the qualitative and quantitative methods. The one study that identified itself as a multi-method study used a longitudinal design. Please see how these designs were employed in each specific topic in Table 4 , Figure 6 .

Design use in the field of psychology.

Experimental design828236010128643
Non-experimental design1153051013171313143
Cross-sectional design123311211917215132
Correlational design5612301022042
Not stated377304241413
Longitudinal design21621122023
Quasi-experimental design4100002100
Systematic review3000110100
Cross-cultural design3001000100
Descriptive design2000003000
Ethnography4000000000
Literature review1100110000
Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA)2000100000
Narrative design1000001100
Case-control research design0000020000
Concurrent data collection design1000100000
Grounded Theory1000100000
Narrative review0100010000
Auto-ethnography1000000000
Case series evaluation0000000100
Case study1000000000
Comprehensive review0100000000
Descriptive-inferential0000000010
Explanatory sequential design1000000000
Exploratory mixed-method0000100100
Grounded ethnographic design0100000000
Historical cohort design0100000000
Historical research0000000100
interpretivist approach0000000100
Meta-review1000000100
Prospective design1000000000
Qualitative review0000000100
Qualitative systematic review0000010000
Short-term prospective design0100000000
Total4611757463635856483916

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is frma-05-00001-g0006.jpg

Design frequency in topics.

Data collection and analysis —data collection included 30 methods, with the data collection method most often employed being questionnaires (57.84%). The experimental task (16.56%) was the second most preferred collection method, which included established or unique tasks designed by the researchers. Cognitive ability tests (6.84%) were also regularly used along with various forms of interviewing (7.66%). Table 5 and Figure 7 represent data collection use in the various topics. Data analysis consisted of 3,857 occurrences of data analysis categorised into ±188 various data analysis techniques shown in Table 6 and Figures 1 – 7 . Descriptive statistics were the most commonly used (23.49%) along with correlational analysis (17.19%). When using a qualitative method, researchers generally employed thematic analysis (0.52%) or different forms of analysis that led to coding and the creation of themes. Review studies presented few data analysis methods, with most studies categorising their results. Mixed method and multi-method studies followed the analysis methods identified for the qualitative and quantitative studies included.

Data collection in the field of psychology.

Questionnaire3641136542405139243711
Experimental task68663529511551
Cognitive ability test957112615110
Physiological measure31216253010
Interview19301302201
Online scholarly literature104003401000
Open-ended questions15301312300
Semi-structured interviews10300321201
Observation10100000020
Documents5110000120
Focus group6120100000
Not stated2110001401
Public data6100000201
Drawing task0201110200
In-depth interview6000100000
Structured interview0200120010
Writing task1000400100
Questionnaire interviews1010201000
Non-experimental task4000000000
Tests2200000000
Group accounts2000000100
Open-ended prompts1100000100
Field notes2000000000
Open-ended interview2000000000
Qualitative questions0000010001
Social media1000000010
Assessment procedure0001000000
Closed-ended questions0000000100
Open discussions1000000000
Qualitative descriptions1000000000
Total55127375116797365605017

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is frma-05-00001-g0007.jpg

Data collection frequency in topics.

Data analysis in the field of psychology.

Not stated5120011501
Actor-Partner Interdependence Model (APIM)4000000000
Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA)17813421001
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)112601629151715653
Auto-regressive path coefficients0010000000
Average variance extracted (AVE)1000010000
Bartholomew's classification system1000000000
Bayesian analysis3000100000
Bibliometric analysis1100000100
Binary logistic regression1100141000
Binary multilevel regression0001000000
Binomial and Bernoulli regression models2000000000
Binomial mixed effects model1000000000
Bivariate Correlations321030435111
Bivariate logistic correlations1000010000
Bootstrapping391623516121
Canonical correlations0000000020
Cartesian diagram1000000000
Case-wise diagnostics0100001000
Casual network analysis0001000000
Categorisation5200110400
Categorisation of responses2000000000
Category codes3100010000
Cattell's scree-test0010000000
Chi-square tests52201756118743
Classic Parallel Analysis (PA)0010010010
Cluster analysis7000111101
Coded15312111210
Cohen d effect size14521323101
Common method variance (CMV)5010000000
Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA)0000000010
Confidence Interval (CI)2000010000
Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA)5713400247131
Content analysis9100210100
Convergent validity1000000000
Cook's distance0100100000
Correlated-trait-correlated-method minus one model1000000000
Correlational analysis2598544182731348338
Covariance matrix3010000000
Covariance modelling0110000000
Covariance structure analyses2000000000
Cronbach's alpha61141865108375
Cross-validation0020000001
Cross-lagged analyses1210001000
Dependent t-test1200110100
Descriptive statistics3241324349414336282910
Differentiated analysis0000001000
Discriminate analysis1020000001
Discursive psychology1000000000
Dominance analysis1000000000
Expectation maximisation2100000100
Exploratory data Analysis1100110000
Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA)145240114040
Exploratory structural equation modelling (ESEM)0010000010
Factor analysis124160215020
Measurement invariance testing0000000000
Four-way mixed ANOVA0101000000
Frequency rate20142122200
Friedman test1000000000
Games-Howell 2200010000
General linear model analysis1200001100
Greenhouse-Geisser correction2500001111
Grounded theory method0000000001
Grounded theory methodology using open and axial coding1000000000
Guttman split-half0010000000
Harman's one-factor test13200012000
Herman's criteria of experience categorisation0000000100
Hierarchical CFA (HCFA)0010000000
Hierarchical cluster analysis1000000000
Hierarchical Linear Modelling (HLM)762223767441
Huynh-Felt correction1000000000
Identified themes3000100000
Independent samples t-test38944483311
Inductive open coding1000000000
Inferential statistics2000001000
Interclass correlation3010000000
Internal consistency3120000000
Interpreted and defined0000100000
Interpretive Phenomenological Analysis (IPA)2100100000
Item fit analysis1050000000
K-means clustering0000000100
Kaiser-meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy2080002020
Kendall's coefficients3100000000
Kolmogorov-Smirnov test1211220010
Lagged-effects multilevel modelling1100000000
Latent class differentiation (LCD)1000000000
Latent cluster analysis0000010000
Latent growth curve modelling (LGCM)1000000110
Latent means1000000000
Latent Profile Analysis (LPA)1100000000
Linear regressions691941031253130
Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count0000100000
Listwise deletion method0000010000
Log-likelihood ratios0000010000
Logistic mixed-effects model1000000000
Logistic regression analyses17010421001
Loglinear Model2000000000
Mahalanobis distances0200010000
Mann-Whitney U tests6421202400
Mauchly's test0102000101
Maximum likelihood method11390132310
Maximum-likelihood factor analysis with promax rotation0100000000
Measurement invariance testing4110100000
Mediation analysis29712435030
Meta-analysis3010000100
Microanalysis1000000000
Minimum significant difference (MSD) comparison0100000000
Mixed ANOVAs196010121410
Mixed linear model0001001000
Mixed-design ANCOVA1100000000
Mixed-effects multiple regression models1000000000
Moderated hierarchical regression model1000000000
Moderated regression analysis8400101010
Monte Carlo Markov Chains2010000000
Multi-group analysis3000000000
Multidimensional Random Coefficient Multinomial Logit (MRCML)0010000000
Multidimensional Scaling2000000000
Multiple-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MGCFA)3000020000
Multilevel latent class analysis1000010000
Multilevel modelling7211100110
Multilevel Structural Equation Modelling (MSEM)2000000000
Multinominal logistic regression (MLR)1000000000
Multinominal regression analysis1000020000
Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC)0000110000
Multiple mediation analysis2600221000
Multiple regression341530345072
Multivariate analysis of co-variance (MANCOVA)12211011010
Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA)38845569112
Multivariate hierarchical linear regression1100000000
Multivariate linear regression0100001000
Multivariate logistic regression analyses1000000000
Multivariate regressions2100001000
Nagelkerke's R square0000010000
Narrative analysis1000001000
Negative binominal regression with log link0000010000
Newman-Keuls0100010000
Nomological Validity Analysis0010000000
One sample t-test81017464010
Ordinary Least-Square regression (OLS)2201000000
Pairwise deletion method0000010000
Pairwise parameter comparison4000002000
Parametric Analysis0001000000
Partial Least Squares regression method (PLS)1100000000
Path analysis21901245120
Path-analytic model test1000000000
Phenomenological analysis0010000100
Polynomial regression analyses1000000000
Fisher LSD0100000000
Principal axis factoring2140001000
Principal component analysis (PCA)81121103251
Pseudo-panel regression1000000000
Quantitative content analysis0000100000
Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analysis2001000000
Relative weight analysis1000000000
Repeated measures analyses of variances (rANOVA)182217521111
Ryan-Einot-Gabriel-Welsch multiple F test1000000000
Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square statistic0030000000
Scheffe's test3000010000
Sequential multiple mediation analysis1000000000
Shapiro-Wilk test2302100000
Sobel Test13501024000
Squared multiple correlations1000000000
Squared semi-partial correlations (sr2)2000000000
Stepwise regression analysis3200100020
Structural Equation Modelling (SEM)562233355053
Structure analysis0000001000
Subsequent t-test0000100000
Systematic coding- Gemeinschaft-oriented1000100000
Task analysis2000000000
Thematic analysis11200302200
Three (condition)-way ANOVA0400101000
Three-way hierarchical loglinear analysis0200000000
Tukey-Kramer corrections0001010000
Two-paired sample t-test7611031101
Two-tailed related t-test0110100000
Unadjusted Logistic regression analysis0100000000
Univariate generalized linear models (GLM)2000000000
Variance inflation factor (VIF)3100000010
Variance-covariance matrix1000000100
Wald test1100000000
Ward's hierarchical cluster method0000000001
Weighted least squares with corrections to means and variances (WLSMV)2000000000
Welch and Brown-Forsythe F-ratios0100010000
Wilcoxon signed-rank test3302000201
Wilks' Lamba6000001000
Word analysis0000000100
Word Association Analysis1000000000
scores5610110100
Total173863532919219823722511715255

Results of the topics researched in psychology can be seen in the tables, as previously stated in this article. It is noteworthy that, of the 10 topics, social psychology accounted for 43.54% of the studies, with cognitive psychology the second most popular research topic at 16.92%. The remainder of the topics only occurred in 4.0–7.0% of the articles considered. A list of the included 999 articles is available under the section “View Articles” on the following website: https://methodgarden.xtrapolate.io/ . This website was created by Scholtz et al. ( 2019 ) to visually present a research framework based on this Article's results.

This systematised review categorised full-length articles from five international journals across the span of 5 years to provide insight into the use of research methods in the field of psychology. Results indicated what methods are used how these methods are being used and for what topics (why) in the included sample of articles. The results should be seen as providing insight into method use and by no means a comprehensive representation of the aforementioned aim due to the limited sample. To our knowledge, this is the first research study to address this topic in this manner. Our discussion attempts to promote a productive way forward in terms of the key results for method use in psychology, especially in the field of academia (Holloway, 2008 ).

With regard to the methods used, our data stayed true to literature, finding only common research methods (Grant and Booth, 2009 ; Maree, 2016 ) that varied in the degree to which they were employed. Quantitative research was found to be the most popular method, as indicated by literature (Breen and Darlaston-Jones, 2010 ; Counsell and Harlow, 2017 ) and previous studies in specific areas of psychology (see Coetzee and Van Zyl, 2014 ). Its long history as the first research method (Leech et al., 2007 ) in the field of psychology as well as researchers' current application of mathematical approaches in their studies (Toomela, 2010 ) might contribute to its popularity today. Whatever the case may be, our results show that, despite the growth in qualitative research (Demuth, 2015 ; Smith and McGannon, 2018 ), quantitative research remains the first choice for article publication in these journals. Despite the included journals indicating openness to articles that apply any research methods. This finding may be due to qualitative research still being seen as a new method (Burman and Whelan, 2011 ) or reviewers' standards being higher for qualitative studies (Bluhm et al., 2011 ). Future research is encouraged into the possible biasness in publication of research methods, additionally further investigation with a different sample into the proclaimed growth of qualitative research may also provide different results.

Review studies were found to surpass that of multi-method and mixed method studies. To this effect Grant and Booth ( 2009 ), state that the increased awareness, journal contribution calls as well as its efficiency in procuring research funds all promote the popularity of reviews. The low frequency of mixed method studies contradicts the view in literature that it's the third most utilised research method (Tashakkori and Teddlie's, 2003 ). Its' low occurrence in this sample could be due to opposing views on mixing methods (Gunasekare, 2015 ) or that authors prefer publishing in mixed method journals, when using this method, or its relative novelty (Ivankova et al., 2016 ). Despite its low occurrence, the application of the mixed methods design in articles was methodologically clear in all cases which were not the case for the remainder of research methods.

Additionally, a substantial number of studies used a combination of methodologies that are not mixed or multi-method studies. Perceived fixed boundaries are according to literature often set aside, as confirmed by this result, in order to investigate the aim of a study, which could create a new and helpful way of understanding the world (Gunasekare, 2015 ). According to Toomela ( 2010 ), this is not unheard of and could be considered a form of “structural systemic science,” as in the case of qualitative methodology (observation) applied in quantitative studies (experimental design) for example. Based on this result, further research into this phenomenon as well as its implications for research methods such as multi and mixed methods is recommended.

Discerning how these research methods were applied, presented some difficulty. In the case of sampling, most studies—regardless of method—did mention some form of inclusion and exclusion criteria, but no definite sampling method. This result, along with the fact that samples often consisted of students from the researchers' own academic institutions, can contribute to literature and debates among academics (Peterson and Merunka, 2014 ; Laher, 2016 ). Samples of convenience and students as participants especially raise questions about the generalisability and applicability of results (Peterson and Merunka, 2014 ). This is because attention to sampling is important as inappropriate sampling can debilitate the legitimacy of interpretations (Onwuegbuzie and Collins, 2017 ). Future investigation into the possible implications of this reported popular use of convenience samples for the field of psychology as well as the reason for this use could provide interesting insight, and is encouraged by this study.

Additionally, and this is indicated in Table 6 , articles seldom report the research designs used, which highlights the pressing aspect of the lack of rigour in the included sample. Rigour with regards to the applied empirical method is imperative in promoting psychology as a science (American Psychological Association, 2020 ). Omitting parts of the research process in publication when it could have been used to inform others' research skills should be questioned, and the influence on the process of replicating results should be considered. Publications are often rejected due to a lack of rigour in the applied method and designs (Fonseca, 2013 ; Laher, 2016 ), calling for increased clarity and knowledge of method application. Replication is a critical part of any field of scientific research and requires the “complete articulation” of the study methods used (Drotar, 2010 , p. 804). The lack of thorough description could be explained by the requirements of certain journals to only report on certain aspects of a research process, especially with regard to the applied design (Laher, 20). However, naming aspects such as sampling and designs, is a requirement according to the APA's Journal Article Reporting Standards (JARS-Quant) (Appelbaum et al., 2018 ). With very little information on how a study was conducted, authors lose a valuable opportunity to enhance research validity, enrich the knowledge of others, and contribute to the growth of psychology and methodology as a whole. In the case of this research study, it also restricted our results to only reported samples and designs, which indicated a preference for certain designs, such as cross-sectional designs for quantitative studies.

Data collection and analysis were for the most part clearly stated. A key result was the versatile use of questionnaires. Researchers would apply a questionnaire in various ways, for example in questionnaire interviews, online surveys, and written questionnaires across most research methods. This may highlight a trend for future research.

With regard to the topics these methods were employed for, our research study found a new field named “psychological practice.” This result may show the growing consciousness of researchers as part of the research process (Denzin and Lincoln, 2003 ), psychological practice, and knowledge generation. The most popular of these topics was social psychology, which is generously covered in journals and by learning societies, as testaments of the institutional support and richness social psychology has in the field of psychology (Chryssochoou, 2015 ). The APA's perspective on 2018 trends in psychology also identifies an increased amount of psychology focus on how social determinants are influencing people's health (Deangelis, 2017 ).

This study was not without limitations and the following should be taken into account. Firstly, this study used a sample of five specific journals to address the aim of the research study, despite general journal aims (as stated on journal websites), this inclusion signified a bias towards the research methods published in these specific journals only and limited generalisability. A broader sample of journals over a different period of time, or a single journal over a longer period of time might provide different results. A second limitation is the use of Excel spreadsheets and an electronic system to log articles, which was a manual process and therefore left room for error (Bandara et al., 2015 ). To address this potential issue, co-coding was performed to reduce error. Lastly, this article categorised data based on the information presented in the article sample; there was no interpretation of what methodology could have been applied or whether the methods stated adhered to the criteria for the methods used. Thus, a large number of articles that did not clearly indicate a research method or design could influence the results of this review. However, this in itself was also a noteworthy result. Future research could review research methods of a broader sample of journals with an interpretive review tool that increases rigour. Additionally, the authors also encourage the future use of systematised review designs as a way to promote a concise procedure in applying this design.

Our research study presented the use of research methods for published articles in the field of psychology as well as recommendations for future research based on these results. Insight into the complex questions identified in literature, regarding what methods are used how these methods are being used and for what topics (why) was gained. This sample preferred quantitative methods, used convenience sampling and presented a lack of rigorous accounts for the remaining methodologies. All methodologies that were clearly indicated in the sample were tabulated to allow researchers insight into the general use of methods and not only the most frequently used methods. The lack of rigorous account of research methods in articles was represented in-depth for each step in the research process and can be of vital importance to address the current replication crisis within the field of psychology. Recommendations for future research aimed to motivate research into the practical implications of the results for psychology, for example, publication bias and the use of convenience samples.

Ethics Statement

This study was cleared by the North-West University Health Research Ethics Committee: NWU-00115-17-S1.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

  • Aanstoos C. M. (2014). Psychology . Available online at: http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.nwulib.nwu.ac.za/eds/detail/detail?sid=18de6c5c-2b03-4eac-94890145eb01bc70%40sessionmgr4006&vid$=$1&hid$=$4113&bdata$=$JnNpdGU9ZWRzL~WxpdmU%3d#AN$=$93871882&db$=$ers
  • American Psychological Association (2020). Science of Psychology . Available online at: https://www.apa.org/action/science/
  • Appelbaum M., Cooper H., Kline R. B., Mayo-Wilson E., Nezu A. M., Rao S. M. (2018). Journal article reporting standards for quantitative research in psychology: the APA Publications and Communications Board task force report . Am. Psychol. 73 :3. 10.1037/amp0000191 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandara W., Furtmueller E., Gorbacheva E., Miskon S., Beekhuyzen J. (2015). Achieving rigor in literature reviews: insights from qualitative data analysis and tool-support . Commun. Ass. Inform. Syst. 37 , 154–204. 10.17705/1CAIS.03708 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barr-Walker J. (2017). Evidence-based information needs of public health workers: a systematized review . J. Med. Libr. Assoc. 105 , 69–79. 10.5195/JMLA.2017.109 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bittermann A., Fischer A. (2018). How to identify hot topics in psychology using topic modeling . Z. Psychol. 226 , 3–13. 10.1027/2151-2604/a000318 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bluhm D. J., Harman W., Lee T. W., Mitchell T. R. (2011). Qualitative research in management: a decade of progress . J. Manage. Stud. 48 , 1866–1891. 10.1111/j.1467-6486.2010.00972.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Breen L. J., Darlaston-Jones D. (2010). Moving beyond the enduring dominance of positivism in psychological research: implications for psychology in Australia . Aust. Psychol. 45 , 67–76. 10.1080/00050060903127481 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Burman E., Whelan P. (2011). Problems in / of Qualitative Research . Maidenhead: Open University Press/McGraw Hill. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chaichanasakul A., He Y., Chen H., Allen G. E. K., Khairallah T. S., Ramos K. (2011). Journal of Career Development: a 36-year content analysis (1972–2007) . J. Career. Dev. 38 , 440–455. 10.1177/0894845310380223 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chryssochoou X. (2015). Social Psychology . Inter. Encycl. Soc. Behav. Sci. 22 , 532–537. 10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.24095-6 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cichocka A., Jost J. T. (2014). Stripped of illusions? Exploring system justification processes in capitalist and post-Communist societies . Inter. J. Psychol. 49 , 6–29. 10.1002/ijop.12011 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clay R. A. (2017). Psychology is More Popular Than Ever. Monitor on Psychology: Trends Report . Available online at: https://www.apa.org/monitor/2017/11/trends-popular
  • Coetzee M., Van Zyl L. E. (2014). A review of a decade's scholarly publications (2004–2013) in the South African Journal of Industrial Psychology . SA. J. Psychol . 40 , 1–16. 10.4102/sajip.v40i1.1227 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Counsell A., Harlow L. (2017). Reporting practices and use of quantitative methods in Canadian journal articles in psychology . Can. Psychol. 58 , 140–147. 10.1037/cap0000074 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deangelis T. (2017). Targeting Social Factors That Undermine Health. Monitor on Psychology: Trends Report . Available online at: https://www.apa.org/monitor/2017/11/trend-social-factors
  • Demuth C. (2015). New directions in qualitative research in psychology . Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 49 , 125–133. 10.1007/s12124-015-9303-9 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Denzin N. K., Lincoln Y. (2003). The Landscape of Qualitative Research: Theories and Issues , 2nd Edn. London: Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drotar D. (2010). A call for replications of research in pediatric psychology and guidance for authors . J. Pediatr. Psychol. 35 , 801–805. 10.1093/jpepsy/jsq049 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dweck C. S. (2017). Is psychology headed in the right direction? Yes, no, and maybe . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12 , 656–659. 10.1177/1745691616687747 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Earp B. D., Trafimow D. (2015). Replication, falsification, and the crisis of confidence in social psychology . Front. Psychol. 6 :621. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00621 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ezeh A. C., Izugbara C. O., Kabiru C. W., Fonn S., Kahn K., Manderson L., et al.. (2010). Building capacity for public and population health research in Africa: the consortium for advanced research training in Africa (CARTA) model . Glob. Health Action 3 :5693. 10.3402/gha.v3i0.5693 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ferreira A. L. L., Bessa M. M. M., Drezett J., De Abreu L. C. (2016). Quality of life of the woman carrier of endometriosis: systematized review . Reprod. Clim. 31 , 48–54. 10.1016/j.recli.2015.12.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fonseca M. (2013). Most Common Reasons for Journal Rejections . Available online at: http://www.editage.com/insights/most-common-reasons-for-journal-rejections
  • Gough B., Lyons A. (2016). The future of qualitative research in psychology: accentuating the positive . Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 50 , 234–243. 10.1007/s12124-015-9320-8 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grant M. J., Booth A. (2009). A typology of reviews: an analysis of 14 review types and associated methodologies . Health Info. Libr. J. 26 , 91–108. 10.1111/j.1471-1842.2009.00848.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grix J. (2002). Introducing students to the generic terminology of social research . Politics 22 , 175–186. 10.1111/1467-9256.00173 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gunasekare U. L. T. P. (2015). Mixed research method as the third research paradigm: a literature review . Int. J. Sci. Res. 4 , 361–368. Available online at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=2735996 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hengartner M. P. (2018). Raising awareness for the replication crisis in clinical psychology by focusing on inconsistencies in psychotherapy Research: how much can we rely on published findings from efficacy trials? Front. Psychol. 9 :256. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00256 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holloway W. (2008). Doing intellectual disagreement differently . Psychoanal. Cult. Soc. 13 , 385–396. 10.1057/pcs.2008.29 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ivankova N. V., Creswell J. W., Plano Clark V. L. (2016). Foundations and Approaches to mixed methods research , in First Steps in Research , 2nd Edn. K. Maree (Pretoria: Van Schaick Publishers; ), 306–335. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Johnson M., Long T., White A. (2001). Arguments for British pluralism in qualitative health research . J. Adv. Nurs. 33 , 243–249. 10.1046/j.1365-2648.2001.01659.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Johnston A., Kelly S. E., Hsieh S. C., Skidmore B., Wells G. A. (2019). Systematic reviews of clinical practice guidelines: a methodological guide . J. Clin. Epidemiol. 108 , 64–72. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.11.030 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ketchen D. J., Jr., Boyd B. K., Bergh D. D. (2008). Research methodology in strategic management: past accomplishments and future challenges . Organ. Res. Methods 11 , 643–658. 10.1177/1094428108319843 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ktepi B. (2016). Data Analytics (DA) . Available online at: https://eds-b-ebscohost-com.nwulib.nwu.ac.za/eds/detail/detail?vid=2&sid=24c978f0-6685-4ed8-ad85-fa5bb04669b9%40sessionmgr101&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWRzLWxpdmU%3d#AN=113931286&db=ers
  • Laher S. (2016). Ostinato rigore: establishing methodological rigour in quantitative research . S. Afr. J. Psychol. 46 , 316–327. 10.1177/0081246316649121 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee C. (2015). The Myth of the Off-Limits Source . Available online at: http://blog.apastyle.org/apastyle/research/
  • Lee T. W., Mitchell T. R., Sablynski C. J. (1999). Qualitative research in organizational and vocational psychology, 1979–1999 . J. Vocat. Behav. 55 , 161–187. 10.1006/jvbe.1999.1707 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leech N. L., Anthony J., Onwuegbuzie A. J. (2007). A typology of mixed methods research designs . Sci. Bus. Media B. V Qual. Quant 43 , 265–275. 10.1007/s11135-007-9105-3 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levitt H. M., Motulsky S. L., Wertz F. J., Morrow S. L., Ponterotto J. G. (2017). Recommendations for designing and reviewing qualitative research in psychology: promoting methodological integrity . Qual. Psychol. 4 , 2–22. 10.1037/qup0000082 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lowe S. M., Moore S. (2014). Social networks and female reproductive choices in the developing world: a systematized review . Rep. Health 11 :85. 10.1186/1742-4755-11-85 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maree K. (2016). Planning a research proposal , in First Steps in Research , 2nd Edn, ed Maree K. (Pretoria: Van Schaik Publishers; ), 49–70. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maree K., Pietersen J. (2016). Sampling , in First Steps in Research, 2nd Edn , ed Maree K. (Pretoria: Van Schaik Publishers; ), 191–202. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ngulube P. (2013). Blending qualitative and quantitative research methods in library and information science in sub-Saharan Africa . ESARBICA J. 32 , 10–23. Available online at: http://hdl.handle.net/10500/22397 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nieuwenhuis J. (2016). Qualitative research designs and data-gathering techniques , in First Steps in Research , 2nd Edn, ed Maree K. (Pretoria: Van Schaik Publishers; ), 71–102. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nind M., Kilburn D., Wiles R. (2015). Using video and dialogue to generate pedagogic knowledge: teachers, learners and researchers reflecting together on the pedagogy of social research methods . Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 18 , 561–576. 10.1080/13645579.2015.1062628 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O'Cathain A. (2009). Editorial: mixed methods research in the health sciences—a quiet revolution . J. Mix. Methods 3 , 1–6. 10.1177/1558689808326272 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O'Neil S., Koekemoer E. (2016). Two decades of qualitative research in psychology, industrial and organisational psychology and human resource management within South Africa: a critical review . SA J. Indust. Psychol. 42 , 1–16. 10.4102/sajip.v42i1.1350 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Onwuegbuzie A. J., Collins K. M. (2017). The role of sampling in mixed methods research enhancing inference quality . Köln Z Soziol. 2 , 133–156. 10.1007/s11577-017-0455-0 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perestelo-Pérez L. (2013). Standards on how to develop and report systematic reviews in psychology and health . Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 13 , 49–57. 10.1016/S1697-2600(13)70007-3 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pericall L. M. T., Taylor E. (2014). Family function and its relationship to injury severity and psychiatric outcome in children with acquired brain injury: a systematized review . Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 56 , 19–30. 10.1111/dmcn.12237 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peterson R. A., Merunka D. R. (2014). Convenience samples of college students and research reproducibility . J. Bus. Res. 67 , 1035–1041. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2013.08.010 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ritchie J., Lewis J., Elam G. (2009). Designing and selecting samples , in Qualitative Research Practice: A Guide for Social Science Students and Researchers , 2nd Edn, ed Ritchie J., Lewis J. (London: Sage; ), 1–23. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sandelowski M. (2011). When a cigar is not just a cigar: alternative perspectives on data and data analysis . Res. Nurs. Health 34 , 342–352. 10.1002/nur.20437 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sandelowski M., Voils C. I., Knafl G. (2009). On quantitizing . J. Mix. Methods Res. 3 , 208–222. 10.1177/1558689809334210 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scholtz S. E., De Klerk W., De Beer L. T. (2019). A data generated research framework for conducting research methods in psychological research .
  • Scimago Journal & Country Rank (2017). Available online at: http://www.scimagojr.com/journalrank.php?category=3201&year=2015
  • Scopus (2017a). About Scopus . Available online at: https://www.scopus.com/home.uri (accessed February 01, 2017).
  • Scopus (2017b). Document Search . Available online at: https://www.scopus.com/home.uri (accessed February 01, 2017).
  • Scott Jones J., Goldring J. E. (2015). ‘I' m not a quants person'; key strategies in building competence and confidence in staff who teach quantitative research methods . Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 18 , 479–494. 10.1080/13645579.2015.1062623 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith B., McGannon K. R. (2018). Developing rigor in quantitative research: problems and opportunities within sport and exercise psychology . Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 11 , 101–121. 10.1080/1750984X.2017.1317357 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stangor C. (2011). Introduction to Psychology . Available online at: http://www.saylor.org/books/
  • Strydom H. (2011). Sampling in the quantitative paradigm , in Research at Grass Roots; For the Social Sciences and Human Service Professions , 4th Edn, eds de Vos A. S., Strydom H., Fouché C. B., Delport C. S. L. (Pretoria: Van Schaik Publishers; ), 221–234. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tashakkori A., Teddlie C. (2003). Handbook of Mixed Methods in Social & Behavioural Research . Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE publications. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Toomela A. (2010). Quantitative methods in psychology: inevitable and useless . Front. Psychol. 1 :29. 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00029 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Truscott D. M., Swars S., Smith S., Thornton-Reid F., Zhao Y., Dooley C., et al.. (2010). A cross-disciplinary examination of the prevalence of mixed methods in educational research: 1995–2005 . Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 13 , 317–328. 10.1080/13645570903097950 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weiten W. (2010). Psychology Themes and Variations . Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. [ Google Scholar ]

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

medicina-logo

Article Menu

what is a population in psychological research

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Impulsivity and its association with depression and anxiety in the normal egyptian population post covid-19 pandemic.

what is a population in psychological research

1. Introduction

2. materials and methods, 2.1. subjects and procedures, 2.2. study measures, 2.2.1. the arab short urgency–premeditation–perseverance–sensation-seeking—positive urgency impulsive behavior scale (short upps-p) [ 26 ], 2.2.2. hamilton anxiety rating scale (ham-a) [ 27 ], 2.2.3. hamilton depression rating scale (hdrs) [ 28 ], 2.2.4. covid-19-related disruption, 2.2.5. the sheehan disability scale (sds) [ 29 ], 2.3. statistical analysis, 3.1. socio-demographic characteristics, 3.2. covid-19 pandemic-related disruption, 3.3. psychometric scales for anxiety, depression, and functional impairment, 3.5. correlation and regression analyses, 4. discussion, 5. conclusions, author contributions, institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, acknowledgments, conflicts of interest.

  • Abdelrahman, M.A.; Zaki, A.; Salem, S.A.; Salem, H.F.; Ibrahim, A.R.; Hassan, A.; Elgendy, M.O. The Impact of Cefepime and Ampicillin/Sulbactam on Preventing Post-Cesarean Surgical Site Infections, Randomized Controlled Trail. Antibiotics 2023 , 12 , 1666. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Salah, H.; Sinan, I.; Alsamani, O.; Abdelghani, L.S.; ElLithy, M.H.; Bukamal, N.; Jawad, H.; Hussein, R.R.; Elgendy, M.O.; Rabie, A.s.I. COVID-19 booster doses: A multi-center study reflecting healthcare providers’ perceptions. Vaccines 2023 , 11 , 1061. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Eid, R.A.; Elgendy, M.O.; Sayed, A.M.; Abdallah, A.M.; Mostafa, H.M.; Elsisi, A.M.M.; Hamed, A.M.; Shaker, M.A. Efficacy of Linezolid in the management of pneumonic COVID-19 patients. Bioinformatics-based clinical study. J. Infect. Dev. Ctries. 2024 , 18 , 326–331. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Alatawi, A.D.; Elgendy, M.O.; Sayed, A.M.; Shafiq, S.N.; El-Bahrawy, A.H.; Mallhi, T.H.; Khan, Y.H.; Alzarea, A.I.; Alotaibi, N.H.; Alanazi, A.S. Local and Systemic side effects of COVID-19 Vaccines. Int. J. Clin. Med. Res. 2023 , 2 , 11–20. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sharma, K.; Saji, J.; Kumar, R.; Raju, A. Psychological and anxiety/depression level assessment among quarantine people during Covid19 outbreak. J. Drug Deliv. Ther. 2020 , 10 , 198–201. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Elgendy, M.O.; Abdelrahman, M.A.; Abdelrahim, M.E. What should be learned from the COVID-19 pandemic for the next coronavirus pandemics? Int. J. Clin. Med. Res. 2023 , 1 , 9–11. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Khalaf, A.M.; Elgendy, M.O.; Fahmy, A.M.; El Gendy, S.O.; El-Gendy, A.O.; Abdelrahman, M.A.; El-Bahrawy, A.H.; Elsisi, A.M.M.; Shafiq, S.N. Relation between ABO and RhD and prevalence and severity of COVID-19 disease. Int. J. Clin. Med. Res. 2024 , 2 , 78–86. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mazza, C.; Ricci, E.; Biondi, S.; Colasanti, M.; Ferracuti, S.; Napoli, C.; Roma, P. A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Italian people during the COVID-19 pandemic: Immediate psychological responses and associated factors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020 , 17 , 3165. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wang, C.; Pan, R.; Wan, X.; Tan, Y.; Xu, L.; Ho, C.S.; Ho, R.C. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020 , 17 , 1729. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Abo-Elela, M.M.; ElKasabgy, N.A. COVID-19 a global crisis: Features, complications and suggested treatments. Int. J. Clin. Med. Res. 2023 , 1 , 43–55. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Islam, M.S.; Tasnim, R.; Sujan, M.S.H.; Ferdous, M.Z.; Sikder, M.T.; Masud, J.H.B.; Kundu, S.; Tahsin, P.; Mosaddek, A.S.M.; Griffiths, M.D. Depressive symptoms associated with COVID-19 preventive practice measures, daily activities in home quarantine and suicidal behaviors: Findings from a large-scale online survey in Bangladesh. BMC Psychiatry 2021 , 21 , 273. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Hoerger, M.; Kim, S.; Mossman, B.; Alonzi, S.; Xu, K.; Coward, J.C.; Whalen, K.; Nauman, E.; Miller, J.; De La Cerda, T. Cultivating community-based participatory research (CBPR) to respond to the COVID-19 pandemic: An illustrative example of partnership and topic prioritization in the food services industry. BMC Public Health 2023 , 23 , 1939. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Osama, H.; Rabea, H.M.; Abdelrahman, M.A. The impact of mindfulness-based stress reduction on psychological health among patients with chronic diseases during COVID-19 outbreak lockdown. Beni Suef Univ. J. Basic Appl. Sci. 2023 , 12 , 50. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Giménez-Palomo, A.; Fico, G.; Borràs, R.; Sagué-Vilavella, M.; Gómez-Ramiro, M.; Fortea, A.; Fortea, L.; Vázquez, M.; Vieta, E. Increasing prevalence of self-harm, suicidal behavior and suicidal ideation throughout a three-year period in the context of COVID-19 pandemic in Spain. Psychiatry Res. 2024 , 339 , 116015. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Elhawary, A.E.; Lashin, H.I.; Fnoon, N.F.; Sagah, G.A. Evaluation of the rate and pattern of suicide attempts and deaths by self-poisoning among Egyptians before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Toxicol. Res. 2023 , 12 , 1113–1125. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Xiang, Y.-T.; Yang, Y.; Li, W.; Zhang, L.; Zhang, Q.; Cheung, T.; Ng, C.H. Timely mental health care for the 2019 novel coronavirus outbreak is urgently needed. Lancet Psychiatry 2020 , 7 , 228–229. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Shaaban, M.; Mohamed, A. Determining the efficacy of N-acetyl cysteine in treatment of pneumonia in COVID-19 hospitalized patients: A meta-analysis. Int. J. Clin. Med. Res. 2023 , 1 , 36–42. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Elgendy, M.O.; Saeed, H.; Abou-Taleb, H.A. Assessment of educated people awareness level and sources about COVID-19. Int. J. Clin. Med. Res. 2023 , 1 , 19–27. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Elgendy, S.O.; Elgendy, M.O.; El-Gendy, A.O.; Hamied, A.M.A.; Al Amir, K.; Gad, R.A.; Fahmy, A.M. Health Care Workers’ Awareness about the Post-COVID Syndrome and Different Types of COVID-19 Vaccines in Egypt. NeuroQuantology 2022 , 20 , 3830. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boshra, M.S.; Elgendy, M.O.; Abdelaty, L.N.; Tammam, M.; Alanazi, A.S.; Alzarea, A.I.; Alsahali, S.; Sarhan, R.M. Knowledge, Attitude, and Acceptance of Sinopharm and AstraZeneca’s COVID-19 Vaccines among Egyptian Population: A Cross-Sectional Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022 , 19 , 16803. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Moustafa, A.A.; Tindle, R.; Frydecka, D.; Misiak, B. Impulsivity and its relationship with anxiety, depression and stress. Compr. Psychiatry 2017 , 74 , 173–179. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Adzrago, D.; Chiangong, J.; Ormiston, C.K.; Dada, O.M.; Jones, A.; Williams, F. Social distancing stress, anxiety/depression, COVID-19 diagnosis, gender identity, and immigration status. Arch. Public Health 2024 , 82 , 86. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Elgendy, M.O.; El-Gendy, A.O.; Abdelrahim, M.E. Public awareness in Egypt about COVID-19 spread in the early phase of the pandemic. Patient Educ. Couns. 2020 , 103 , 2598–2601. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhang, J.; Lu, H.; Zeng, H.; Zhang, S.; Du, Q.; Jiang, T.; Du, B. The differential psychological distress of populations affected by the COVID-19 pandemic. Brain Behav. Immun. 2020 , 87 , 49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Elgendy, M.O.; El-Gendy, A.O.; Elgendy, S.O.; Abdelaty, L.N.; Abdelrahim, M.E.; Abdelrahman, M.A. Perceptions, Knowledge, and Experiences of Using Face Masks among Egyptian Healthcare Workers during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study. Healthcare 2023 , 11 , 838. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Bteich, G.; Berbiche, D.; Khazaal, Y. Validation of the short Arabic UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale. BMC Psychiatry 2017 , 17 , 244. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Hamilton, M. Hamilton anxiety rating scale (HAM-A). J. Med. 1959 , 61 , 81–82. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Obeid, S.; Hallit, C.A.E.; Haddad, C.; Hany, Z.; Hallit, S. Validation of the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) and sociodemographic factors associated with Lebanese depressed patients. L’encephale 2018 , 44 , 397–402. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Arbuckle, R.; Frye, M.A.; Brecher, M.; Paulsson, B.; Rajagopalan, K.; Palmer, S.; Degl’Innocenti, A. The psychometric validation of the Sheehan Disability Scale (SDS) in patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2009 , 165 , 163–174. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sheehan, K.H.; Sheehan, D.V. Assessing treatment effects in clinical trials with the discan metric of the Sheehan Disability Scale. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2008 , 23 , 70–83. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Alzueta, E.; Perrin, P.; Baker, F.C.; Caffarra, S.; Ramos-Usuga, D.; Yuksel, D.; Arango-Lasprilla, J.C. How the COVID-19 pandemic has changed our lives: A study of psychological correlates across 59 countries. J. Clin. Psychol. 2021 , 77 , 556–570. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yamada, Y.; Ćepulić, D.-B.; Coll-Martín, T.; Debove, S.; Gautreau, G.; Han, H.; Rasmussen, J.; Tran, T.P.; Travaglino, G.A. COVIDiSTRESS Global Survey dataset on psychological and behavioural consequences of the COVID-19 outbreak. Sci. Data 2021 , 8 , 3. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Mencacci, C.; Salvi, V. Expected effects of COVID-19 outbreak on depression incidence in Italy. J. Affect. Disord. 2021 , 278 , 66. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Wilson, J.J.; Smith, L.; Yakkundi, A.; Jacob, L.; Martin, S.; Grabovac, I.; McDermott, D.T.; López-Bueno, R.; Barnett, Y.; Butler, L.T. Changes in health-related Behaviours and mental health in a UK public sample during the first set of COVID-19 public health restrictions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022 , 19 , 3959. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Poyraz, B.Ç.; Poyraz, C.A.; Olgun, Y.; Gürel, Ö.; Alkan, S.; Özdemir, Y.E.; Balkan, İ.İ.; Karaali, R. Psychiatric morbidity and protracted symptoms after COVID-19. Psychiatry Res. 2021 , 295 , 113604. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Liu, D.; Baumeister, R.F.; Veilleux, J.C.; Chen, C.; Liu, W.; Yue, Y.; Zhang, S. Risk factors associated with mental illness in hospital discharged patients infected with COVID-19 in Wuhan, China. Psychiatry Res. 2020 , 292 , 113297. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mehandru, S.; Merad, M. Pathological sequelae of long-haul COVID. Nat. Immunol. 2022 , 23 , 194–202. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Terrier, C.; Chen, D.L.; Sutter, M. COVID-19 within families amplifies the prosociality gap between adolescents of high and low socioeconomic status. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021 , 118 , e2110891118. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Park, Y.-j.; Jung, Y.-E.; Kim, M.-D.; Bahk, W.-M. COVID-19–Related Functional Impairment in a Community Sample of Korean Adults: Associations With Depression, COVID-19 Infection Fear, and Resilience. Psychiatry Investig. 2022 , 19 , 919. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gallagher, M.W.; Zvolensky, M.J.; Long, L.J.; Rogers, A.H.; Garey, L. The impact of Covid-19 experiences and associated stress on anxiety, depression, and functional impairment in American adults. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2020 , 44 , 1043–1051. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gecaite-Stonciene, J.; Saudargiene, A.; Pranckeviciene, A.; Liaugaudaite, V.; Griskova-Bulanova, I.; Simkute, D.; Naginiene, R.; Dainauskas, L.L.; Ceidaite, G.; Burkauskas, J. Impulsivity mediates associations between problematic internet use, anxiety, and depressive symptoms in students: A cross-sectional COVID-19 study. Front. Psychiatry 2021 , 12 , 634464. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yu, Y.; Yu, Y.; Lin, Y. Anxiety and depression aggravate impulsiveness: The mediating and moderating role of cognitive flexibility. Psychol. Health Med. 2020 , 25 , 25–36. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Altan-Atalay, A.; Zeytun, D. The association of negative urgency with psychological distress: Moderating role of proactive coping strategies. J. Psychol. 2020 , 154 , 487–498. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Pautrat, M.; Le Guen, A.; Barrault, S.; Ribadier, A.; Ballon, N.; Lebeau, J.-P.; Brunault, P. Impulsivity as a risk factor for addictive disorder severity during the COVID-19 lockdown: Results from a mixed quantitative and qualitative study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022 , 20 , 705. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Schaefer, K.E.; Esposito-Smythers, C.; Riskind, J.H. The role of impulsivity in the relationship between anxiety and suicidal ideation. J. Affect. Disord. 2012 , 143 , 95–101. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Cyders, M.A.; Coskunpinar, A. Is urgency emotionality? Separating urgent behaviors from effects of emotional experiences. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2010 , 48 , 839–844. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • López Steinmetz, L.C.; Fong, S.B.; Godoy, J.C. Suicidal risk and impulsivity-related traits among young Argentinean college students during a quarantine of up to 103-day duration: Longitudinal evidence from the COVID-19 pandemic. Suicide Life-Threat. Behav. 2021 , 51 , 1175–1188. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Guenther, B.; Galizzi, M.M.; Sanders, J.G. Heterogeneity in risk-taking during the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence from the UK lockdown. Front. Psychol. 2021 , 12 , 643653. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Byrne, K.A.; Six, S.G.; Anaraky, R.G.; Harris, M.W.; Winterlind, E.L. Risk-taking unmasked: Using risky choice and temporal discounting to explain COVID-19 preventative behaviors. PLoS ONE 2021 , 16 , e0251073. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Reynolds, B.; Ortengren, A.; Richards, J.B.; De Wit, H. Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2006 , 40 , 305–315. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Marazziti, D.; Baroni, S.; Masala, I.; Golia, F.; Consoli, G.; Massimetti, G.; Picchetti, M.; Dell’Osso, M.C.; Giannaccini, G.; Betti, L. Impulsivity, gender, and the platelet serotonin transporter in healthy subjects. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2010 , 6 , 9–15. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Johnson, S.L.; Carver, C.S.; Joormann, J. Impulsive responses to emotion as a transdiagnostic vulnerability to internalizing and externalizing symptoms. J. Affect. Disord. 2013 , 150 , 872–878. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Curtin, R.; Presser, S.; Singer, E. The effects of response rate changes on the index of consumer sentiment. Public Opin. Q. 2000 , 64 , 413–428. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Singer, E.; Van Hoewyk, J.; Maher, M.P. Experiments with incentives in telephone surveys. Public Opin. Q. 2000 , 64 , 171–188. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]

Click here to enlarge figure

CharacteristicN, %
1. Gender (n, %)
Male61, 29.9%
Female140, 70.1%
2. Marital state (n, %)
Single 171, 85.1%
Married30, 14.9%
3. COVID-19-related disruption (yes, %)
Positive infection with coronavirus109, 54.2%
Family member infection with the coronavirus126, 62.7%
Hospital admission as a result of infection with the coronavirus13, 6.5%
Family member death as a result of corona infection 33, 16.4%
History or currently suffers from depression110, 54.7%
History or currently suffers from a state of fear and panic94, 46.8%
Suffers from obsessive–compulsive disorder48, 23.9%
Became unemployed since the pandemic23, 11.4%
Possibility of becoming unemployed due to the pandemic 37, 18.4%
Corona-related events affect the ability to pay rent or mortgage 44, 21.9%
Feeling that there are people available to talk to about problems (including online)102, 50.7%
Suffers from poor concentration123, 61.2%
Key VariablesHAM-AHDRS
Mean (SD)p-ValueMean (SD)p-Value
Overall15.46 (10.17)<0.001 *13.89 (8.56)0.005 *
Male (29.9%)10.6 (7.97)11.28 (8.04)
Female (70.14%)17.51 (10.33)14.99 (8.57)
Married (%) 0.03 * 0.306
Yes (14.9)19.17 (10.98)15.37 (8.43)
No (85.1)14.81 (9.91)13.63 (8.59)
COVID-19 infection (%) 0.079 0.104
Yes (54.2)16.61 ± 9.9514.79 (9.12)
No (45.8)14.09 ± 10.3212.82 (7.76)
History of panic or fear state (%) <0.001 * <0.001 *
Yes (46.8)19.36 (9.61)16.78 (8.39)
No (53.2)12.03 (9.42)11.35 (7.92)
History of depression (%) <0.001 * <0.001 *
Yes (54.7)18.86 (9.72)16.55 (8.52)
No (45.3)11.34 (9.17)10.63 (7.46)
Poor concentration (%) <0.001 * <0.001 *
Yes (61.2)19.05 (9.72)16.61 (8.28)
No (38.8)9.79 (8.12)9.59 (7.15)
Sheehan Disability Scale
Work/Study DomainSocial Life DomainFamily Domain
Mean (SD) p-ValueMean (SD) p-ValueMean (SD) p-Value
Overall2.81 (2.3) 3.12 (2.5) 3.40 (2.7)0.079
Male (29.9%)2.2 (1.8)2.47 (2.2)2.87 (2.7)
Female (70.14%)3.06 (2.4)3.40 (2.6)3.62 (2.8)
Married (%) 0.620 0.833 0.829
Yes (14.9)3.0 (1.9)3.03 (2.2)3.5 (2.6)
No (85.1)2.77 (2.3)3.14 (2.6)3.38 (2.8)
COVID-19 infection (%) 0.175 0.214
Yes (54.2)3.12 (2.2)3.35 (2.6)3.62 (2.7)
No (45.8)2.43 (2.3)2.86 (2.5)3.13 (2.9)
History of panic or fear state (%) 0.073
Yes (46.8)3.29 (2.6)3.47 (2.6)4.04 (2.9)
No (53.2)2.38 (2.0)2.82 (2.4)2.83 (2.5)
History of depression (%)
Yes (54.7)3.28 (2.5)3.61 (2.7)3.85 (2.9)
No (45.3)2.23 (1.9)2.54 (2.3)2.85 (2.6)
Poor concentration (%)
Yes (61.2)3.3 (2.4)3.57 (2.6)3.91 (2.8)
No (38.8)2.03 (1.9)2.42 (2.3)2.59 (2.6)
S-UPPSMean (SD)Male
Mean (SD)
Female
Mean (SD)
p-Value
Total42.97 (8.49)40.77 (9.26)43.91 (7.99)
Negative urgency8.89 (2.52)8.37 (2.59)9.11 (2.47)0.057
Lack of premeditation7.99 (2.03)7.67 (2.04)8.12 (2.02)0.148
Lack of perseverance 8.33 (2.25)7.97 (2.18)8.48 (2.27)0.138
Positive urgency8.26 (2.24)8.03 (2.09)8.36 (2.30)0.343
Sensation seeking9.51 (2.71)8.73 (2.83)9.84 (2.59)
1234567891011121314
1. S-UPPS total1
r
P
2. Negative urgency 1
r0.684
P
3. Lack of premeditation 1
r0.507
P
4. Lack of perseverance 1
r0.6340.2670.519
P
5. Positive urgency 1
r0.6950.5740.1400.340
P
6. Sensation seeking 1
r0.6890.4210.2080.2950.408
P
7. HAM-A score 1
r0.2470.2600.3080.2640.1780.159
P 0.024
8. HDRS score 1
r0.2260.1630.2890.2530.1770.1710.764
P 0.015
9. SDS work/study domain 1
r0.1250.0200.1580.046−0.0180.0470.4780.449
P0.0770.779 0.5160.8000.344
10. SDS social domain 1
r0.1570.031 0.116 0.0760.4470.5050.687
P 0.661 0.110 0.281
11. Family domain 1
r0.0720.0130.114−0.002−0.0200.0800.3950.4250.5780.690
P0.3070.9860.1080.9820.7830.260
12. SDS total 1
r0.1100.022 0.0410.0520.0840.4840.5090.8140.9020.874
P0.1220.753 0.5630.4660.238
13. Gender 1
r0.141 0.1240.0910.1030.0660.1760.3120.2090.1660.202 0.200
P 0.0680.2010.1470.354
14. COVID-19-related disruption 1
r0.0820.1450.0440.0830.3740.0520.4800.4970.3910.3550.3720.4160.078
p0.442 0.5390.241 0.461 0.272
PredictorsB95% CIp
HAM-A score0.3070.0820.430
HDRS0.1460.0570.435
SDS−0.010−0.2040.1840.092
COVID-19-related disruption1.0241.5110.536
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Ismael, M.S.; Elgendy, M.O.; Binsaleh, A.Y.; Saleh, A.; Abdelrahim, M.E.A.; Osama, H. Impulsivity and Its Association with Depression and Anxiety in the Normal Egyptian Population Post COVID-19 Pandemic. Medicina 2024 , 60 , 1367. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60081367

Ismael MS, Elgendy MO, Binsaleh AY, Saleh A, Abdelrahim MEA, Osama H. Impulsivity and Its Association with Depression and Anxiety in the Normal Egyptian Population Post COVID-19 Pandemic. Medicina . 2024; 60(8):1367. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60081367

Ismael, Marwa S., Marwa O. Elgendy, Ammena Y. Binsaleh, Asmaa Saleh, Mohamed E. A. Abdelrahim, and Hasnaa Osama. 2024. "Impulsivity and Its Association with Depression and Anxiety in the Normal Egyptian Population Post COVID-19 Pandemic" Medicina 60, no. 8: 1367. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60081367

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

Justin James Kennedy, Ph.D., D.Prof.

How to Train Your Brain to Limit Pain

How placebo effects deliver pain releif..

Posted August 23, 2024 | Reviewed by Gary Drevitch

Co-authored by neuroplastician, speaker, and author Jasmine Benson.

Imagine you take a sugar pill, believing it to be a powerful painkiller, and your pain actually diminishes. Is it all in your head, or is there a deeper neurological basis for this phenomenon? The intriguing realm of placebo effects, in which belief itself can lead to real health improvements, raises profound questions about how our brains process pain and how we attach "value" to experience.

The placebo effect is a fascinating example of how our brain's expectations can significantly alter our physical and mental health. Contrary to what one might think, the improvement in health from a placebo isn't about tricking the brain into not feeling pain. Instead, it's about engaging brain systems associated with value and motivation . This distinction is crucial as it changes how we approach pain management and treatment.

The Science Behind the Magic

Pain perception is largely a top-down process, in which higher-level brain regions combine sensory and emotional information to predict pain. The placebo effect, in which health improves after taking an inert treatment, highlights this process.

Research by Botvinik-Nezer (2020) in Nature used fMRI to study how the brain’s activity changes with placebo treatments. Advances in neuroplasticity show that belief in a placebo can significantly alter pain perception by activating neural pathways linked to value and motivation. This reveals how belief and perception can reshape physical responses and has broader implications.

A Story of Exploration and Discovery

In one of the most comprehensive studies to date, 395 participants were part of an intriguing experiment. They received two creams on different fingers: one a control cream with no effects and the other presented as a pain-relieving drug, though it was a placebo. To amplify the placebo effect, researchers used conditioning paradigms in which participants were exposed to varying thermal stimuli. Unbeknownst to them, the intensity was lower on the placebo-treated finger.

Participants were then subjected to thermal and mechanical pain tests while their brain activity was monitored. They rated the intensity and unpleasantness of the pain stimuli. The researchers focused on two specific brain signatures: the Neurologic Pain Signature (NPS), associated with the initial sensory perception of pain, and the Stimulus Intensity Independent Pain Signature (SIIPS), linked to higher-order, value-based processing of pain (Wager et al., 2004).

Participants reported less pain in the placebo condition compared to the control. The NPS score, reflecting immediate pain perception, increased with stimulus intensity but showed no significant placebo effect. However, the SIIPS score, representing higher-order pain processing, was significantly lower in the placebo condition for both thermal and mechanical pain. This indicates that the placebo effect is mediated by brain systems involved in value and motivation, not just the raw sensory experience of pain.

Furthermore, the study found that the placebo effect transferred to unconditioned pain modalities. Participants experienced less mechanical pain, even though only thermal pain was conditioned. This suggests that the brain's valuation system can generalize the placebo effect across different types of pain.

Empowering Change

Traditional pain management often relies on pharmaceuticals and rigid protocols that may not fully address the complexity of pain and brain function. Embracing the placebo effect marks a shift toward integrating belief and brain function into treatment. Unlike conventional methods, which may overlook these factors, understanding the placebo effect promotes a more comprehensive approach that includes psychological and physiological aspects.

This approach empowers patients to engage in their own healing through belief and expectation, fostering a positive mindset and active participation in treatment. By recognizing the impact of mental state on recovery, patients can enhance outcomes and align with a broader philosophy of engagement and ownership in both healthcare and personal development.

what is a population in psychological research

This study was groundbreaking because it used a large sample size and two well-validated brain signatures to explore placebo analgesia. It underscores the potential of targeting higher-level cognitive processes in pain management. By understanding that placebo effects engage brain regions associated with value and motivation, healthcare providers can develop more effective treatments that harness these mechanisms.

Consider Sarah, a chronic pain sufferer who tried numerous treatments without success. When she participated in a clinical trial involving a placebo, she was skeptical. Yet, as weeks passed, she noticed a significant reduction in her pain. Sarah's experience wasn't just a figment of her imagination . Her brain had activated higher-order systems that re-evaluated her pain, diminishing its impact.

Expanding Horizons: Placebo Effects in Leadership and Beyond

The placebo effect offers valuable insights for organizational leadership . By understanding placebo mechanisms, leaders can foster environments in which belief and motivation drive performance, enhancing employee engagement and productivity .

This highlights the need for more human-centric management practices that align with evolving work dynamics. Organizations should reinvent their structures and culture to avoid social pain and help employees thrive. The application of placebo models in corporations shows that fostering belief and engagement can significantly improve outcomes, as supported by Eisenberger and Rhoades (2002).

What Lies Ahead

The findings from Botvinik-Nezer and her team, mentioned before, pave the way for future research into how we can harness the power of the placebo effect more effectively. As Rotem Botvinik-Nezer, the first author, expressed, this study represents a significant leap forward in placebo research, made possible by years of meticulous data collection and the efforts of many dedicated researchers, and it can be applied more broadly.

Why Does This Matter to You?

How might understanding the placebo effect change your own approach to pain and treatment? Insights into how our brains process value and motivation reveal the potential to revolutionize pain management and enhance well-being. The power of belief is deeply rooted in our neural pathways, opening new possibilities for healing, health, change, leadership, and human performance (Botvinik-Nezer et al., 2020).

What are your thoughts on integrating placebo effects into everyday treatments? Could this understanding reshape our approach to managing pain and other conditions? Your perspectives could be key in unlocking the full potential of the mind-body connection.

Botvinik-Nezer, R., Holzmeister, F., Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., … & Schonberg, T. (2020). Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset by many teams. Nature , 582(7810), 84-88. doi:10.1038/s41586-020-2314-9

Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The neural bases of social pain: Evidence for shared representations with physical pain. Psychosomatic Medicine, 74 (2), 126–135. DOI: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182464dd1

Wager, T. D., Rilling, J. K., Smith, E. E., Johnston, C. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science, 303 (5661), 1162-1167. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1093065

Justin James Kennedy, Ph.D., D.Prof.

Justin James Kennedy, Ph.D., is a professor of applied neuroscience and organisational behaviour at UGSM-Monarch Business School in Switzerland and the author of Brain Re-Boot.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Self Tests NEW
  • Therapy Center
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

July 2024 magazine cover

Sticking up for yourself is no easy task. But there are concrete skills you can use to hone your assertiveness and advocate for yourself.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

IMAGES

  1. Population vs. Sample

    what is a population in psychological research

  2. What Is Population In Research

    what is a population in psychological research

  3. Population vs Sample in Research

    what is a population in psychological research

  4. PPT

    what is a population in psychological research

  5. Characteristics of psychological variables of the study population

    what is a population in psychological research

  6. Research process sampling from a target population

    what is a population in psychological research

COMMENTS

  1. What Is the Big Deal About Populations in Research?

    A population is a complete set of people with specified characteristics, while a sample is a subset of the population. 1 In general, most people think of the defining characteristic of a population in terms of geographic location. However, in research, other characteristics will define a population.

  2. Population vs. Sample

    A population is the entire group that you want to draw conclusions about. A sample is the specific group that you will collect data from. The size of the sample is always less than the total size of the population. In research, a population doesn't always refer to people. It can mean a group containing elements of anything you want to study ...

  3. Sampling Methods In Reseach: Types, Techniques, & Examples

    Sampling methods in psychology refer to strategies used to select a subset of individuals (a sample) from a larger population, to study and draw inferences about the entire population. Common methods include random sampling, stratified sampling, cluster sampling, and convenience sampling. Proper sampling ensures representative, generalizable, and valid research results.

  4. Population and Target Population in Research Methodology

    Introduction. Research methodology relies heavily on the precise definition and differentiation between the. population under study and the target population, as these concepts serve as the ...

  5. PDF Sampling

    population. The target population is the total group of individuals from which the sample is drawn. In psychological research, we are interested in learning about large groups of people who all have something in common. We call the group that we are interested in studying our population. In some types of research the population might

  6. Population: Psychology Definition, History & Examples

    In psychology, the term 'population' refers to the entire group of individuals or distinct groups that researchers want to study or make conclusions about. It is important to define the population precisely in order to ensure that research findings can be applied to a larger group. The population can be broad, such as all teenagers in a ...

  7. Understanding Population and Sample in Psychological Research

    Psychological research necessitates a clear understanding of the terms 'population' and 'sample' to ensure the validity of study results. A population is the entire set of individuals or observations that are of interest to the researcher's question.

  8. APA Dictionary of Psychology

    population psychology. a subfield of psychology that studies the relationships between the characteristics and dynamics of human populations and the attitudes and behavior of individuals and groups. Representing an interface between psychology and demography, population psychology is particularly concerned with family planning and fertility ...

  9. Psychological Statistics

    Psychological Statistics. Psychological statistics is an applied branch of statistics that involves the application of statistical principles and methods to collect, organize, and analyze data obtained from psychological research. It uses the information conveyed by these data to make scientific inferences and discover patterns in psychological ...

  10. 19 Population Psychology

    The expansionist perspective defines population psychology as any research within the arena of population studies, broadly construed, that includes an individual-level, that is, a psychological, perspective. From this standpoint, most demographers are at least interested in the psychology of demography. There even exists a sub-area of sociology ...

  11. Person, Population, Mechanism. Three Main Branches of Psychological

    The basic suggestion is that psychological science involves research at three different levels: (1) a person-level, (2) a population-level, and (3) a sub-personal mechanism level. The person-level is characterized by a focus on psychological phenomena as experienced and enacted by individual persons in their interaction with other persons and ...

  12. Population definition

    Population. When conducting research there are lots of factors to consider. Psychologists may want to study, for example, the effect of some new test on all college students, but this is obviously not possible. Instead, what they do is test on a sample or a smaller group of college students. In this example, everyone who could possibly be a ...

  13. What is POPULATION? definition of POPULATION ...

    POPULATION. noun. 1. the entire amount of people in a rendered geographical location. 2. with regard to statistics, a theoretically defined, total group of items from which a sampling is taken in effort to attain empirical observations and to which outcomes can be generalized. Commonly referred to as universe.

  14. Population vs. Sample

    The research population, also known as the target population, refers to the entire group or set of individuals, objects, or events that possess specific characteristics and are of interest to the researcher. It represents the larger population from which a sample is drawn. The research population is defined based on the research objectives and ...

  15. Research Methods In Psychology

    Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc. Research methods in psychology are systematic procedures used to observe, describe, predict, and explain behavior and mental processes. They include experiments, surveys, case studies, and naturalistic observations, ensuring data collection is objective and reliable to understand and explain psychological phenomena.

  16. Samples in Psychology Research: Common Types and Errors

    In statistics, a sample is a subset of a population that is used to represent the entire group as a whole. When doing psychology research, it is often impractical to survey every member of a particular population because the number of people is simply too large. To make inferences about the characteristics of a population, psychology researchers use a random sample.

  17. (PDF) CONCEPT OF POPULATION AND SAMPLE

    Abstract. This paper deals with the concept of Population and Sample in research, especially in educational and psychological researches and the researches carried out in the field of Sociology ...

  18. Sample Size and its Importance in Research

    The sample size for a study needs to be estimated at the time the study is proposed; too large a sample is unnecessary and unethical, and too small a sample is unscientific and also unethical. The necessary sample size can be calculated, using statistical software, based on certain assumptions. If no assumptions can be made, then an arbitrary ...

  19. Vulnerable population and methods for their safeguard

    Vulnerable population. There are several definitions available for the term "vulnerable population", the words simply imply the disadvantaged sub-segment of the community[] requiring utmost care, specific ancillary considerations and augmented protections in research.The vulnerable individuals' freedom and capability to protect one-self from intended or inherent risks is variably ...

  20. Considering the whole person: A guide to culturally responsive

    For psychological researchers, this means adapting the methodology to decentralize science. Of note, we offer critiques and culturally responsive practices in psychological research, though these suggestions may be applied to affiliated fields (e.g., sociology, social work, counseling). Further, many of these critiques and recommendations ...

  21. Research with vulnerable populations.

    This chapter considers the ethical issues surrounding research with vulnerable populations. The overarching ethical value of beneficence, as found in Principle A of the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct (the Ethics Code; American Psychological Association [APA], 2010), exhorts psychologists to work for the benefit of individuals and society.

  22. Dealing with small samples in disability research: Do not fret

    Purpose/Objective: Small sample sizes are a common problem in disability research. Here, we show how Bayesian methods can be applied in small sample settings and the advantages that they provide. Method/Design: To illustrate, we provide a Bayesian analysis of employment status (employed vs. unemployed) for those with disability. Specifically, we apply empirically informed priors, based on ...

  23. Misinformation vs. Disinformation: What the Terms Mean and the Effects

    "There is a myriad of consequences: From cynicism of government, the media, and science, to behaviors that harm individuals and others … to large scale damage to public property, to insurrection," Dolores Albarracin, a psychology professor at the University of Illinois who studies attitudes, communication and behavior told USA TODAY in 2021.

  24. APA Dictionary of Psychology

    A trusted reference in the field of psychology, offering more than 25,000 clear and authoritative entries. ... the population that a study is intended to research and to which generalizations from samples are to be made. Also called reference population. Browse Dictionary.

  25. The Use of Research Methods in Psychological Research: A Systematised

    Introduction. Psychology is an ever-growing and popular field (Gough and Lyons, 2016; Clay, 2017).Due to this growth and the need for science-based research to base health decisions on (Perestelo-Pérez, 2013), the use of research methods in the broad field of psychology is an essential point of investigation (Stangor, 2011; Aanstoos, 2014).Research methods are therefore viewed as important ...

  26. Medicina

    Our research highlights the correlation between impulsivity and the psychological distress experienced following the pandemic. Next Article in Journal. ... As a result, the ability to extrapolate our findings to the male population is limited. In future research, it is recommended that researchers expand the participant sample size and ...

  27. How to Train Your Brain to Limit Pain

    Co-authored by neuroplastician, speaker, and author Jasmine Benson. Imagine you take a sugar pill, believing it to be a powerful painkiller, and your pain actually diminishes. Is it all in your ...

  28. Psychology Research Intern Jobs, Employment in Remote

    16 Psychology Research Intern jobs available in Remote 🇮🇱 on Indeed.com. Apply to Research Intern, Human Resources Intern, Licensed Clinical Social Worker and more!