• Type 2 Diabetes
  • Heart Disease
  • Digestive Health
  • Multiple Sclerosis
  • Diet & Nutrition
  • Health Insurance
  • Public Health
  • Patient Rights
  • Caregivers & Loved Ones
  • End of Life Concerns
  • Health News
  • Thyroid Test Analyzer
  • Doctor Discussion Guides
  • Hemoglobin A1c Test Analyzer
  • Lipid Test Analyzer
  • Complete Blood Count (CBC) Analyzer
  • What to Buy
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Medical Expert Board

Cyberbullying: Everything You Need to Know

  • Cyberbullying
  • How to Respond

Cyberbullying is the act of intentionally and consistently mistreating or harassing someone through the use of electronic devices or other forms of electronic communication (like social media platforms).

Because cyberbullying mainly affects children and adolescents, many brush it off as a part of growing up. However, cyberbullying can have dire mental and emotional consequences if left unaddressed.

This article discusses cyberbullying, its adverse effects, and what can be done about it.

FangXiaNuo / Getty Images

Cyberbullying Statistics and State Laws

The rise of digital communication methods has paved the way for a new type of bullying to form, one that takes place outside of the schoolyard. Cyberbullying follows kids home, making it much more difficult to ignore or cope.

Statistics 

As many as 15% of young people between 12 and 18 have been cyberbullied at some point. However, over 25% of children between 13 and 15 were cyberbullied in one year alone.

About 6.2% of people admitted that they’ve engaged in cyberbullying at some point in the last year. The age at which a person is most likely to cyberbully one of their peers is 13.

Those subject to online bullying are twice as likely to self-harm or attempt suicide . The percentage is much higher in young people who identify as LGBTQ, at 56%.

Cyberbullying by Sex and Sexual Orientation

Cyberbullying statistics differ among various groups, including:

  • Girls and boys reported similar numbers when asked if they have been cyberbullied, at 23.7% and 21.9%, respectively.
  • LGBTQ adolescents report cyberbullying at higher rates, at 31.7%. Up to 56% of young people who identify as LGBTQ have experienced cyberbullying.
  • Transgender teens were the most likely to be cyberbullied, at a significantly high rate of 35.4%.

State Laws 

The laws surrounding cyberbullying vary from state to state. However, all 50 states have developed and implemented specific policies or laws to protect children from being cyberbullied in and out of the classroom.

The laws were put into place so that students who are being cyberbullied at school can have access to support systems, and those who are being cyberbullied at home have a way to report the incidents.

Legal policies or programs developed to help stop cyberbullying include:

  • Bullying prevention programs
  • Cyberbullying education courses for teachers
  • Procedures designed to investigate instances of cyberbullying
  • Support systems for children who have been subject to cyberbullying 

Are There Federal Laws Against Cyberbullying?

There are no federal laws or policies that protect people from cyberbullying. However, federal involvement may occur if the bullying overlaps with harassment. Federal law will get involved if the bullying concerns a person’s race, ethnicity, national origin, sex, disability, or religion.

Examples of Cyberbullying 

There are several types of bullying that can occur online, and they all look different.

Harassment can include comments, text messages, or threatening emails designed to make the cyberbullied person feel scared, embarrassed, or ashamed of themselves.

Other forms of harassment include:

  • Using group chats as a way to gang up on one person
  • Making derogatory comments about a person based on their race, gender, sexual orientation, economic status, or other characteristics
  • Posting mean or untrue things on social media sites, such as Twitter, Facebook, or Instagram, as a way to publicly hurt the person experiencing the cyberbullying  

Impersonation

A person may try to pretend to be the person they are cyberbullying to attempt to embarrass, shame, or hurt them publicly. Some examples of this include:

  • Hacking into someone’s online profile and changing any part of it, whether it be a photo or their "About Me" portion, to something that is either harmful or inappropriate
  • Catfishing, which is when a person creates a fake persona to trick someone into a relationship with them as a joke or for their own personal gain
  • Making a fake profile using the screen name of their target to post inappropriate or rude remarks on other people’s pages

Other Examples

Not all forms of cyberbullying are the same, and cyberbullies use other tactics to ensure that their target feels as bad as possible. Some tactics include:

  • Taking nude or otherwise degrading photos of a person without their consent
  • Sharing or posting nude pictures with a wide audience to embarrass the person they are cyberbullying
  • Sharing personal information about a person on a public website that could cause them to feel unsafe
  • Physically bullying someone in school and getting someone else to record it so that it can be watched and passed around later
  • Circulating rumors about a person

How to Know When a Joke Turns Into Cyberbullying

People may often try to downplay cyberbullying by saying it was just a joke. However, any incident that continues to make a person feel shame, hurt, or blatantly disrespected is not a joke and should be addressed. People who engage in cyberbullying tactics know that they’ve crossed these boundaries, from being playful to being harmful.

Effects and Consequences of Cyberbullying 

Research shows many negative effects of cyberbullying, some of which can lead to severe mental health issues. Cyberbullied people are twice as likely to experience suicidal thoughts, actions, or behaviors and engage in self-harm as those who are not.

Other negative health consequences of cyberbullying are:

  • Stomach pain and digestive issues
  • Sleep disturbances
  • Difficulties with academics
  • Violent behaviors
  • High levels of stress
  • Inability to feel safe
  • Feelings of loneliness and isolation
  • Feelings of powerlessness and hopelessness

If You’ve Been Cyberbullied 

Being on the receiving end of cyberbullying is hard to cope with. It can feel like you have nowhere to turn and no escape. However, some things can be done to help overcome cyberbullying experiences.

Advice for Preteens and Teenagers

The best thing you can do if you’re being cyberbullied is tell an adult you trust. It may be challenging to start the conversation because you may feel ashamed or embarrassed. However, if it is not addressed, it can get worse.

Other ways you can cope with cyberbullying include:

  • Walk away : Walking away online involves ignoring the bullies, stepping back from your computer or phone, and finding something you enjoy doing to distract yourself from the bullying.
  • Don’t retaliate : You may want to defend yourself at the time. But engaging with the bullies can make matters worse.
  • Keep evidence : Save all copies of the cyberbullying, whether it be posts, texts, or emails, and keep them if the bullying escalates and you need to report them.
  • Report : Social media sites take harassment seriously, and reporting them to site administrators may block the bully from using the site.
  • Block : You can block your bully from contacting you on social media platforms and through text messages.

In some cases, therapy may be a good option to help cope with the aftermath of cyberbullying.

Advice for Parents

As a parent, watching your child experience cyberbullying can be difficult. To help in the right ways, you can:

  • Offer support and comfort : Listening to your child explain what's happening can be helpful. If you've experienced bullying as a child, sharing that experience may provide some perspective on how it can be overcome and that the feelings don't last forever.
  • Make sure they know they are not at fault : Whatever the bully uses to target your child can make them feel like something is wrong with them. Offer praise to your child for speaking up and reassure them that it's not their fault.
  • Contact the school : Schools have policies to protect children from bullying, but to help, you have to inform school officials.
  • Keep records : Ask your child for all the records of the bullying and keep a copy for yourself. This evidence will be helpful to have if the bullying escalates and further action needs to be taken.
  • Try to get them help : In many cases, cyberbullying can lead to mental stress and sometimes mental health disorders. Getting your child a therapist gives them a safe place to work through their experience.

In the Workplace 

Although cyberbullying more often affects children and adolescents, it can also happen to adults in the workplace. If you are dealing with cyberbullying at your workplace, you can:

  • Let your bully know how what they said affected you and that you expect it to stop.
  • Keep copies of any harassment that goes on in the workplace.
  • Report your cyberbully to your human resources (HR) department.
  • Report your cyberbully to law enforcement if you are being threatened.
  • Close off all personal communication pathways with your cyberbully.
  • Maintain a professional attitude at work regardless of what is being said or done.
  • Seek out support through friends, family, or professional help.

Effective Action Against Cyberbullying

If cyberbullying continues, actions will have to be taken to get it to stop, such as:

  • Talking to a school official : Talking to someone at school may be difficult, but once you do, you may be grateful that you have some support. Schools have policies to address cyberbullying.
  • Confide in parents or trusted friends : Discuss your experience with your parents or others you trust. Having support on your side will make you feel less alone.
  • Report it on social media : Social media sites have strict rules on the types of interactions and content sharing allowed. Report your aggressor to the site to get them banned and eliminate their ability to contact you.
  • Block the bully : Phones, computers, and social media platforms contain options to block correspondence from others. Use these blocking tools to help free yourself from cyberbullying.

Help Is Available

If you or someone you know are having suicidal thoughts, dial  988  to contact the  988 Suicide & Crisis Lifeline  and connect with a trained counselor. To find mental health resources in your area, contact the  Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) National Helpline  at  800-662-4357  for information.

Cyberbullying occurs over electronic communication methods like cell phones, computers, social media, and other online platforms. While anyone can be subject to cyberbullying, it is most likely to occur between the ages of 12 and 18.

Cyberbullying can be severe and lead to serious health issues, such as new or worsened mental health disorders, sleep issues, or thoughts of suicide or self-harm. There are laws to prevent cyberbullying, so it's essential to report it when it happens. Coping strategies include stepping away from electronics, blocking bullies, and getting.

Alhajji M, Bass S, Dai T. Cyberbullying, mental health, and violence in adolescents and associations with sex and race: data from the 2015 youth risk behavior survey . Glob Pediatr Health. 2019;6:2333794X19868887. doi:10.1177/2333794X19868887

Cyberbullying Research Center. Cyberbullying in 2021 by age, gender, sexual orientation, and race .

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: StopBullying.gov. Facts about bullying .

John A, Glendenning AC, Marchant A, et al. Self-harm, suicidal behaviours, and cyberbullying in children and young people: systematic review .  J Med Internet Res . 2018;20(4):e129. doi:10.2196/jmir.9044

Cyberbullying Research Center. Bullying, cyberbullying, and LGBTQ students .

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: StopBullying.gov. Laws, policies, and regulations .

Wolke D, Lee K, Guy A. Cyberbullying: a storm in a teacup? . Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(8):899-908. doi:10.1007/s00787-017-0954-6

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: StopBullying.gov. Cyberbullying tactics .

Garett R, Lord LR, Young SD. Associations between social media and cyberbullying: a review of the literature . mHealth . 2016;2:46-46. doi:10.21037/mhealth.2016.12.01

Nemours Teens Health. Cyberbullying .

Nixon CL. Current perspectives: the impact of cyberbullying on adolescent health . Adolesc Health Med Ther. 2014;5:143-58. doi:10.2147/AHMT.S36456

Nemours Kids Health. Cyberbullying (for parents) .

By Angelica Bottaro Bottaro has a Bachelor of Science in Psychology and an Advanced Diploma in Journalism. She is based in Canada.

Journal of Youth Development

Preventing Bullying: Consequences, Prevention, and Intervention

  • Suzanne Le Menestrel National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine

Bullying is considered to be a significant public health problem with both short- and long-term physical and social-emotional consequences for youth. A large body of research indicates that youth who have been bullied are at increased risk of subsequent mental, emotional, health, and behavioral problems, especially internalizing problems, such as low self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and loneliness. Given the growing awareness of bullying as a public health problem and the increasing evidence of short- and long-term physical, mental, emotional, and behavioral health and academic consequences of bullying behavior, there have been significant efforts at the practice, program, and policy levels to address bullying behavior. This article summarizes a recent consensus report from the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, Preventing Bullying Through Science, Policy, and Practice , and what is known about the consequences of bullying behavior and interventions that attempt to prevent and respond to it.

Author Biography

Suzanne le menestrel, national academies of sciences, engineering, and medicine.

American Psychological Association Zero Tolerance Task Force. (2008). Are zero tolerance policies effective in the schools?: An evidentiary review and recommendations. American Psychologist, 63(9) 852-862. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.9.852

Beran, T. (2009). Correlates of peer victimization and achievement: An exploratory model. Psychology in the Schools, 46(4), 348-361. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20380

Beran, T. N., Hughes, G., & Lupart, J. (2008). A model of achievement and bullying: Analyses of the Canadian National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth data. Educational Research, 50(1), 25-39. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131880801920379

Beran, T. N., & Lupart, J. (2009). The relationship between school achievement and peer harassment in Canadian adolescents: The importance of mediating factors. School Psychology International, 30(1), 75-91. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034308101851

Boccanfuso, C., & Kuhfeld, M. (2011, March). Multiple responses, promising results: Evidence-based nonpunitive alternatives to zero tolerance (Research to Results Brief, Child Trends Publication No. 2011-09). http://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2011/03/Child_Trends-2011_03_01_RB_AltToZeroTolerance.pdf

Bradshaw, C. P. (2015). Translating research to practice in bullying prevention. American Psychologist, 70(4), 322. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039114

de Bruyn, E., Cillessen, A., & Wissink, I. (2010). Associations of peer acceptance and perceived popularity with bullying and victimization in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 30(4), 543-566. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431609340517

Dijkstra, J. K., Lindenberg, S., & Veenstra, R. (2008). Beyond the class norm: Bullying behavior of popular adolescents and its relation to peer acceptance and rejection. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(8), 1289-1299. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9251-7

Faris, R., & Ennett, S. (2012). Adolescent aggression: The role of peer group status motives, peer aggression, and group characteristics. Social Networks, 34(4), 371-378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2010.06.003

Farmer, T. W., Estell, D. B., Bishop, J. L., O’Neal, K. K., & Cairns, B. D. (2003). Rejected bullies or popular leaders? The social relations of aggressive subtypes of rural African American early adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 39(6), 992-1004. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.6.992

Farrington, D. P., & Ttofi, M. (2009). School-based programs to reduce bullying and victimization: A systematic review. Campbell Systematic Reviews, 5(6), 1-148. https://doi.org/10.4073/csr.2009.6

Gini, G., & Pozzoli, T. (2009). Association between bullying and psychosomatic problems: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 123(3), 1059-1065. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1215

Gladden, R. M., Vivolo-Kantor, A. M., Hamburger, M. E., & Lumpkin, C. D. (2014). Bullying surveillance among youths: Uniform definitions for public health and recommended data elements (Version 1.0). Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and U.S. Department of Education. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED575477.pdf

Glew, G. M., Fan, M.-Y., Katon, W., Rivara, F. P., & Kernic, M. A. (2005). Bullying, psychosocial adjustment, and academic performance in elementary school. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 159(11), 1026-1031. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.159.11.1026

Hatzenbuehler, M. L., & Keyes, K. M. (2013). Inclusive anti-bullying policies and reduced risk of suicide attempts in lesbian and gay youth. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53(1), S21-S26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.08.010

Institute of Medicine. (1994). Reducing risks for mental disorders: Frontiers for preventive intervention research. National Academy Press.

Jiménez-Barbero, J. A., Ruiz-Hernández, J. A., Llor-Zaragoza, L., Pérez-García, M., & Llor- Esteban, B. (2016). Effectiveness of anti-bullying school programs: A meta-analysis. Children and Youth Services Review, 61, 165-175. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2015.12.015

Juvonen, J., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2000). Peer harassment, psychological adjustment, and school functioning in early adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 349-359. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.2.349

Klomek, A. B., Kleinman, M., Altschuler, E., Marrocco, F., Amakawa, L., & Gould, M. S. (2011). High school bullying as a risk for later depression and suicidality. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 41(5), 501-516. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1943-278X.2011.00046.x

Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. W. (1996). Peer victimization: Cause or consequence of school maladjustment? Child Development, 67(4), 1305-1317. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01797.x

Kowalski, R. M., and Limber, S. P. (2013). Psychological, physical, and academic correlates of cyberbullying and traditional bullying. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53(1), S13-S20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.09.018

Lereya, S. T., Copeland, W. E., Costello, E. J., & Wolke, D. (2015). Adult mental health consequences of peer bullying and maltreatment in childhood: Two cohorts in two countries. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(6), 524-531. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00165-0

Limber, S. B. (2014). Bullying Among Children and Youth. Unpublished manuscript of a study commissioned by the Planning Committee on Increasing Capacity for Reducing Bullying and Its Impact on the Lifecourse of Youth Involved. Department of Psychology and Institute on Family and Neighborhood Life, Clemson University.

Marsh, H. W., Nagengast, B., Morin, A. J., Parada, R. H., Craven, R. G., & Hamilton, L. R. (2011). Construct validity of the multidimensional structure of bullying and victimization: An application of exploratory structural equation modeling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 701-732. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024122

McDougall, P., & Vaillancourt, T. (2015). Long-term adult outcomes of peer victimization in childhood and adolescence: Pathways to adjustment and maladjustment. American Psychologist, 70(4), 300. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039174

Minton, S. J. (2014). Prejudice and effective anti-bullying intervention: Evidence from the bullying of “minorities.” Nordic Psychology, 66(2), 108-120. https://doi.org/10.1080/19012276.2014.928485

Musher-Eizenman, D. R., Boxer, P., Danner, S., Dubow, E. F., Goldstein, S. E., & Heretick, D. M. L. (2004). Social-cognitive mediators of the relation of environmental and emotion regulation factors to children’s aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 30(5), 389-408.

Nakamoto, J., & Schwartz, D. (2010). Is peer victimization associated with academic achievement? A meta-analytic review. Social Development, 19(2), 221-242.

National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2016. Preventing bullying through science, policy, and practice. The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/23482

Neary, A., & Joseph, S. (1994). Peer victimization and its relationship to self-concept and depression among schoolgirls. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(1), 183-186. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)90122-8

Nixon, C. (2014). Current perspectives: The impact of cyberbullying on adolescent health. Adolescent Health, Medicine and Therapeutics, 143-158. https://doi.org/10.2147/AHMT.S36456

Olweus, D. (1993a). Bullying at school. What we know and what we can do. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Olweus, D. (1993b). Victimization by peers: Antecedents and long-term outcomes. In K. H. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social Withdrawal, Inhibition, and Shyness in Childhood (pp. 315-341). Psychology Press.

Olweus, D. (2001). Peer harassment: A critical analysis and some important issues. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 3-20). Guilford Press.

Peeters, M., Cillessen, A. H., & Scholte, R. H. (2010). Clueless or powerful? Identifying subtypes of bullies in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(9), 1041-1052. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9478-9

Polanin, J. R., Espelage, D. L., & Pigott, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of school-based bullying prevention programs’ effects on bystander intervention behavior. School Psychology Review, 41(1), 47-65.

Pöyhönen, V., Juvonen, J., & Salmivalli, C. (2010). What does it take to stand up for the victim of bullying? The interplay between personal and social factors. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 56(2), 143-163.

Reijntjes, A., Kamphuis, J. H., Prinzie, P., & Telch, M. J. (2010). Peer victimization and internalizing problems in children: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Child Abuse & Neglect, 34(4), 244-252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.07.009

Rigby, K., & Slee, P. T. (1993). Dimensions of interpersonal relation among Australian children and implications for psychological well-being. Journal of Social Psychology, 133(1), 33-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1993.9712116

Rigby, K., & Slee, P. (2008). Interventions to reduce bullying. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 20(2), 165-183. https://doi.org/10.1515/IJAMH.2008.20.2.165

Rivers, I., & Noret, N. (2013). Potential suicide ideation and its association with observing bullying at school. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53(1), S32-S36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.10.279

Rivers, I., Poteat, V. P., Noret, N., & Ashurst, N. (2009). Observing bullying at school: The mental health implications of witness status. School Psychology Quarterly, 24(4), 211-223. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018164

Rodkin, P. C., Espelage, D. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2015). A relational framework for understanding bullying: Developmental antecedents and outcomes. American Psychologist, 70(4), 311-321. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038658

Salmivalli, C. (2001). Group view on victimization: Empirical findings and their implications. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school (pp. 398-419). Guilford Press.

Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: A review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 15(2), 112-120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2009.08.007

Salmivalli, C. (2014). Participant roles in bullying: How can peer bystanders be utilized in interventions? Theory Into Practice, 53(4), 286-292. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2014.947222

Sijtsema, J. J., Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., & Salmivalli, C. (2009). Empirical test of bullies’ status goals: Assessing direct goals, aggression, and prestige. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 57-67. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20282

Thunfors, P., & Cornell, D. (2008). The popularity of middle school bullies. Journal of School Violence, 7(1), 65-82. https://doi.org/10.1300/J202v07n01_05

Trach, J., Hymel, S., Waterhouse, T., & Neale, K. (2010). Bystander responses to school bullying: A cross-sectional investigation of grade and sex differences. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 25(1), 114-130. https://doi.org/10.1177/0829573509357553

Ttofi, M. M., & Farrington, D. P. (2009). What works in preventing bullying: Effective elements of anti-bullying programmes. Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research, 1(1), 13-24. https://doi.org/10.1108/17596599200900003

Ttofi, M. M., & Farrington, D. P. (2011). Effectiveness of school-based programs to reduce bullying: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 7(1), 27-56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-010-9109-1

Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., Lösel, F., & Loeber, R. (2011). Do the victims of school bullies tend to become depressed later in life? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research, 3(2), 63-73. https://doi.org/10.1108/17596591111132873/full/html

Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., & McDougall, P. (2003). Bullying is power: Implications for school-based intervention strategies. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 19(2), 157-176. https://doi.org/10.1300/J008v19n02_10

Vaillancourt, T., McDougall, P., Hymel, S., & Sunderani, S. (2010). The relationship between power and bullying behavior. In S. R. Jimerson, S. M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of Bullying in Schools: An International Perspective (pp. 211-222). Routledge.

Vaillancourt, T., Brittain, H. L., McDougall, P., & Duku, E. (2013). Longitudinal links between childhood peer victimization, internalizing and externalizing problems, and academic functioning: Developmental cascades. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(8), 1203-1215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9781-5

Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011). Online communication among adolescents: An integrated model of its attraction, opportunities, and risks. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48(2), 121-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.08.020

Vreeman, R. C., & Carroll, A. E. (2007). A systematic review of school-based interventions to prevent bullying. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 161(1), 78-88.

Wolke, D., & Lereya, S. T. (2015). Long-term effects of bullying. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 100(9), 879-85. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2014-306667

Wolke, D., Lereya, S. T., Fisher, H., Lewis, G., & Zammit, S. (2014). Bullying in elementary school and psychotic experiences at 18 years: A longitudinal, population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 44(10, 2199-2211. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002912

Ybarra, M. L., & Mitchell, K. L. (2004). Online aggressor/targets, aggressors, and targets: A comparison of associated youth characteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(7), 1308-1316. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00328.x

Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:

  • The Author retains copyright in the Work, where the term “Work” shall include all digital objects that may result in subsequent electronic publication or distribution.
  • Upon acceptance of the Work, the author shall grant to the Publisher the right of first publication of the Work.
  • Attribution—other users must attribute the Work in the manner specified by the author as indicated on the journal Web site;
  • The Author is able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the nonexclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the Work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), as long as there is provided in the document an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
  • Authors are permitted and encouraged to post online a prepublication manuscript (but not the Publisher’s final formatted PDF version of the Work) in institutional repositories or on their Websites prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work. Any such posting made before acceptance and publication of the Work shall be updated upon publication to include a reference to the Publisher-assigned DOI (Digital Object Identifier) and a link to the online abstract for the final published Work in the Journal.
  • Upon Publisher’s request, the Author agrees to furnish promptly to Publisher, at the Author’s own expense, written evidence of the permissions, licenses, and consents for use of third-party material included within the Work, except as determined by Publisher to be covered by the principles of Fair Use.
  • the Work is the Author’s original work;
  • the Author has not transferred, and will not transfer, exclusive rights in the Work to any third party;
  • the Work is not pending review or under consideration by another publisher;
  • the Work has not previously been published;
  • the Work contains no misrepresentation or infringement of the Work or property of other authors or third parties; and
  • the Work contains no libel, invasion of privacy, or other unlawful matter.
  • The Author agrees to indemnify and hold Publisher harmless from Author’s breach of the representations and warranties contained in Paragraph 6 above, as well as any claim or proceeding relating to Publisher’s use and publication of any content contained in the Work, including third-party content.

Revised 7/16/2018. Revision Description: Removed outdated link. 

Developed By

Information.

  • For Readers
  • For Authors
  • For Librarians

More information about the publishing system, Platform and Workflow by OJS/PKP.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Here’s how you know

Official websites use .gov

A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS

A lock ( ) or https:// means you’ve safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

StopBullying.gov

  • Research Resources

Consequences of Bullying

Print

It is important for parents and people who work with children and adolescents to understand that bullying can have both short- and long-term effects on everyone involved. While most research on bullying has been about children and adolescents who have been bullied, those who bully others are also negatively impacted, as are those who are both bullied and bully others, and even those who are not directly involved but witness bullying.

Children Who Have Been Bullied

Research has found that children and adolescents who have been bullied can experience negative psychological, physical, and academic effects.

Psychological Effects

Consequences of bullying

The psychological effects of bullying include depression, anxiety, low self-esteem, self-harming behavior (especially for girls), alcohol and drug use and dependence, aggression, and involvement in violence or crime (especially for boys). While bullying can lead to mental health problems for any child, those who already have mental health difficulties are even more likely to be bullied and to experience its negative effects.

Cyberbullying – bullying that happens with computers or mobile devices – has also been linked to mental health problems. Compared with peers who were not cyberbullied, children who were cyberbullied report higher levels of depression and thoughts of suicide, as well as greater emotional distress, hostility, and delinquency.

Physical Effects

Bullying and Suicide

Bullying is a risk factor for depression and thinking about suicide. Children who bully others, are bullied, or both bully and are bullied are more likely to think about or attempt suicide than those who are not involved in bullying at all.

The physical effects of bullying can be obvious and immediate, such as being injured from a physical attack. However, the ongoing stress and trauma of being bullied can also lead to physical problems over time. A child who is bullied could develop sleep disorders - such as difficulty falling asleep or staying asleep - stomachaches, headaches, heart palpitations, dizziness, bedwetting, and chronic pain and somatization (i.e., a syndrome of distressful, physical symptoms that cannot be explained by a medical cause).

Being bullied also increases cortisol levels – a stress hormone – in the body, which typically happens after a stressful event. Stress from bullying can impact the immune system and hormones. Imaging studies show that brain activity and functioning can be affected by bullying, which may help explain the behavior of children who have been bullied.

Academic Effects

Research has consistently shown that bullying can have a negative impact on how well children and adolescents do in school. It has a negative impact on both grades and standardized test scores starting as early as kindergarten and continuing through high school.

Children Who Bully and Those Who Witness Bullying

Very little research has been done to understand the effects of bullying on children who bully, and those who witness bullying (e.g., bystanders). More research is needed to understand the consequences of bullying on the individuals who bully others, particularly to understand the differences between those who are generally aggressive and those who bully others.

Studies of children who witness bullying usually focus on their role in the bullying situation (e.g., if they backed up the child who bullied, or defended the victim) and why they did or did not intervene. While studies rarely assess the effects of bullying exposure on the witness, some research has found that bullying witnesses experience anxiety and insecurity based on their own fears of retaliation.

Children Who Bully and Are Also Bullied

Children and adolescents who bully others and who are also bullied are at the greatest risk for negative mental and physical health consequences, compared to those who only bully or are only being bullied. These children and adolescents may experience a combination of psychological problems, a negative perception of themselves and others, poor social skills, conduct problems, and rejection by their peer group.

Compared with non-involved peers, those who have bullied others and have also been bullied have been found to be at increased risk for serious mental illness, be at high risk for thinking about and attempting suicide, and demonstrate heightened aggression.

Exposure to bullying in any manner – by being bullied, bullying others, or witnessing peers being bullied – has long-term, negative effects on children. The School Crime Supplement to the National Crime Victimization Survey found that in 2015, about 21 percent of students ages 12-18 reported being bullied at school during the school year. Given the prevalence of youth exposed to bullying across the nation, it is important to understand the consequences of bullying on children and adolescents, how it relates to other violent behaviors and mental health challenges, in order to effectively address them.

Download PDF

Source and Research Limitations

The information discussed in this fact sheet is based on the comprehensive review of bullying research presented in the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine’s report entitled Preventing Bullying Through Science, Policy, and Practice .

This report includes the most up to date research on bullying, but it is important to note that this research has several important limitations. Most of the research is cross-sectional, which means it took place at one point in time. This type of research shows us what things are related to each other at that time, but cannot tell us which thing came first or if one of those things caused the other to occur.

Defining school bullying and its implications on education, teachers and learners

defining school bullying

Contributing to UNESCO’s work on fostering safe learning environments , which addresses many different forms of violence, the UNESCO Chair on Bullying and Cyberbullying, in collaboration with the World Anti-Bullying Forum (WABF), led an international working group to create a more holistic and inclusive definition of school bullying. Professor James O’Higgins Norman, UNESCO Chair on Bullying and Cyberbullying, shares his insights on this work.

Why revisit the definition of bullying?

Many current anti-bullying programmes in schools are rooted in early definitions characterizing bullying as an “unwanted aggressive behavior that is repeated over time and involves an imbalance of power or strength”. While this was groundbreaking at the time and advanced the work of researchers, policy makers, educators and others, evolving perspectives have deepened our understanding of bullying.

Research shows that progress in reducing school bullying has been slow, with only a 19% decrease in perpetration and a 15% drop in the rate of learners facing bullying. This means we must reassess our understanding and approaches to bullying, especially in our increasingly complex world, where both in-person and online bullying intertwine with personal and societal issues.

How are you revisiting the definition of bullying?

As a UNESCO Chair, my role involves facilitating interdisciplinary research and dialogue, and working towards a more holistic approach to bullying. Our recommendation for a ‘whole-education’ approach to tackle bullying recognizes individual, contextual, and societal dimensions.

With support from UNESCO and the WABF, I facilitated the working group to revisit the definition of bullying, consulting scholars, policymakers and practitioners worldwide. We gathered feedback from a diverse group and have conducted wide consultations. This working group was launched following the recommendations by a Scientific Committee on preventing and addressing school bullying and cyberbullying, convened by UNESCO and the French Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports.

What would a revised definition mean for education policymakers and practitioners, for school communities and learners?

The proposed definition promotes a holistic and inclusion-driven approach to tackling bullying and violence in schools and in online spaces. 

Crafting a more inclusive definition has the potential to break down academic and professional barriers, encouraging cooperation between sectors, and among scholars, policymakers, educators, and learners. It provides a solid foundation to better understand bullying particularly regarding those most marginalized due to appearance, ethnicity, gender, social class, or sexuality, among others. Bullying is a complex issue tied to individual, contextual, and structural factors, making collaboration essential.

Together, we can deepen our understanding and address not only the behavior but also the underlying systems and ideologies supporting bullying.

What is your vision for this improved definition of school bullying?

My vision aligns with United Nations Sustainable Development Goal 4, on education, in that our work on bullying, and all other forms of school violence, is aimed at ensuring an inclusive and equitable quality education and the promotion of lifelong learning opportunities for all. 

What message do you have for teachers and learners?

To teachers and school staff: Do not accept bullying as normal. Create a safe classroom environment by setting clear expectations for kindness and respect, remain vigilant for signs of bullying, stay informed about effective prevention strategies, and promptly address any incidents. Implement a robust anti-bullying policy. Under the idea of a ‘whole-education’ approach, collaborate with colleagues and parents, incorporate empathy and anti-bullying content into the curriculum, and use collaborative learning methods.

To learners: Report bullying, be confident in recognizing and responding to it, and encourage bystander intervention. You have the power to stop bullying.

New definition and what’s next?

The working group presented its proposed revised definition of school bullying at the WABF held in October 2023. The proposed definition reads:

School bullying is a damaging social process that is characterized by an imbalance of power driven by social (societal) and institutional norms. It is often repeated and manifests as unwanted interpersonal behaviour among students or school personnel that causes physical, social, and emotional harm to the targeted individuals or groups, and the wider school community.

This new inclusive definition of school bullying was largely welcomed by delegates at the Forum. The UNESCO Chair and WABF hope that this revised definition will contribute to opening a new chapter in the global conversation on the nature of and responses to bullying and cyberbullying. 

For UNESCO, the new definition of bullying reflects our approach and work to ensure that schools are safe and supportive learning environments. This means that to end all forms of school violence, including bullying, we must understand that these behaviours do not happen in isolation, that there are different drivers of violence, and that a ‘whole-education’ approach is needed. 

More information

Related items.

  • International Day Against Violence and Bullying at School, including Cyberbullying
  • Health education
  • Topics: Display
  • See more add

More on this subject

Youth voices shaping the future: Highlights from the Regional Policy Dialogue in Bangkok

Other recent news

Empowering Youth in Rwanda: The Impact of Peer Education on Sexual Reproductive Health

Bullying Essay for Students and Children

500+ words essay on bullying.

Bullying refers to aggressive behavior so as to dominate the other person. It refers to the coercion of power over others so that one individual can dominate others. It is an act that is not one time, instead, it keeps on repeating over frequent intervals.  The person(s) who bullies others can be termed as bullies, who make fun of others due to several reasons. Bullying is a result of someone’s perception of the imbalance of power.

bullying essay

Types of bullying :

There can be various types of bullying, like:

  • Physical bullying:  When the bullies try to physically hurt or torture someone, or even touch someone without his/her consent can be termed as physical bullying .
  • Verbal bullying:  It is when a person taunts or teases the other person.
  • Psychological bullying:  When a person or group of persons gossip about another person or exclude them from being part of the group, can be termed as psychological bullying.
  • Cyber bullying:  When bullies make use of social media to insult or hurt someone. They may make comments bad and degrading comments on the person at the public forum and hence make the other person feel embarrassed. Bullies may also post personal information, pictures or videos on social media to deteriorate some one’s public image.

Read Essay on Cyber Bullying

Bullying can happen at any stage of life, such as school bullying, College bullying, Workplace bullying, Public Place bullying, etc. Many times not only the other persons but the family members or parents also unknowingly bully an individual by making constant discouraging remarks. Hence the victim gradually starts losing his/her self-esteem, and may also suffer from psychological disorders.

A UNESCO report says that 32% of students are bullied at schools worldwide. In our country as well, bullying is becoming quite common. Instead, bullying is becoming a major problem worldwide. It has been noted that physical bullying is prevalent amongst boys and psychological bullying is prevalent amongst girls.

Prevention strategies:

In the case of school bullying, parents and teachers can play an important role. They should try and notice the early symptoms of children/students such as behavioral change, lack of self-esteem, concentration deficit, etc. Early recognition of symptoms, prompt action and timely counseling can reduce the after-effects of bullying on the victim.

Get the huge list of more than 500 Essay Topics and Ideas

Anti-bullying laws :

One should be aware of the anti-bullying laws in India. Awareness about such laws may also create discouragement to the act of bullying amongst children and youngsters. Some information about anti-bullying laws is as follows:

  • Laws in School: To put a notice on the notice board that if any student is found bullying other students then he/she can be rusticated. A committee should be formed which can have representatives from school, parents, legal, etc.
  • Laws in Colleges: The government of India, in order to prevent ragging , has created guideline called “UGC regulations on curbing the menace of ragging in Higher Education Institutions,2009”.
  • Cyber Bullying Laws: The victim can file a complaint under the Indian Penal Code .

Conclusion:

It is the duty of the parents to constantly preach their children about not bullying anyone and that it is wrong. Hence, if we, as a society need to grow and develop then we have to collectively work towards discouraging the act of bullying and hence make our children feel secure.

Customize your course in 30 seconds

Which class are you in.

tutor

  • Travelling Essay
  • Picnic Essay
  • Our Country Essay
  • My Parents Essay
  • Essay on Favourite Personality
  • Essay on Memorable Day of My Life
  • Essay on Knowledge is Power
  • Essay on Gurpurab
  • Essay on My Favourite Season
  • Essay on Types of Sports

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Download the App

Google Play

  • Skip to content
  • Skip to secondary menu
  • Skip to primary sidebar
  • Skip to footer

Health and Medical Blog

Pros and Cons of Cyber Bullying

In today’s world young people have access to the entire world through the use of computers, cell phones, and other electronic devices. The benefits of this have been great, but with the good there has to be the bad. Cyber Bullying is defined as any form of repeated harassment to a person using electronic devices and the internet. It is a growing issue that youth are facing today. Studies have shown that one third of students have faced some form of cyber bullying in their life, that is a startling high amount. While it is hard to imagine any good coming from something so hurtful, could there be pros to go with the cons?

The Pros of Cyberbullying

1. Stand Up For Themselves Cyber bullying often attacks personal traits of an individual, such as their weight, appearance, voice, or values. This can be extremely damaging to a developing child. Cyber bullies often choose this avenue because it can be done from the safety of their home, with no real confrontation. This works both ways, however. Children that are experiencing cyber bullying feel much more empowered to stand up for themselves, because they are also in the comfort of their home.

2. It’s In Writing Social media, texts, and emails are all in writing, and once they are sent they cannot be taken back. This gives hard and undeniable proof that this bullying is occurring, as well as exactly who is involved. It can tremendously help parents and schools to identify who the problem students are and the proper actions to take.

3. From The Bullies Perspective If you are the person who is doing the bullying online, you may feel very confident and courageous when bullying someone from behind a computer. This also often makes people develop an inflated ego and feel “cool”.

The Cons of Cyber Bullying

1. Spreads To Day To Day Life Cyber bullying is very commonly just a side product of real life, face to face bullying that is occurring. This may make the victim feel scared and unsure in there normal days, especially at school or other social situations.

2. Risk of Depression And Suicide An increasing number of young kids are falling into a depressive state, and sadly, committing suicide as a result from this cyber bullying. The abuse is so severe and inescapable that they feel it is their only way out. Young adults and kids are very susceptible to criticism and cruelty because they are still unsure of themselves.

3. Out Of Schools Hands Because cyber bullying doesn’t occur in the school in a literal sense, many schools do not do anything about it. It is not because they don’t care, but the guildelines for discipline and boundaries of when their control stops is unclear when it comes to student’s online lives.

4. It Follows You Traditional bullying, or face to face altercations, doesn’t follow the same rules that cyber bullying does. With traditional bullying the victim can go home and get away from it at some point, they have safety zones and times. However, when it comes to cyber bullying all bets are off. Cell phones and computers are always there, the attacks can happen at any time of the day or night and anywhere. This makes the victims feel completely helpless.

5. Larger Audience Cyber bullying is very much public, and because it is published it has the ability to reach a very broad audience. Furthering the humiliation of the person being harassed.

Cyberbullying Trends

  • 13 ANC Nails Pros and Cons
  • 15 Artificial Sphincter Pros and Cons
  • 14 Hysterectomy for Fibroids Pros and Cons
  • 15 Monovision Lasik Pros and Cons
  • 12 Pros and Cons of the Da Vinci Robotic Surgery
  • 14 Peritoneal Dialysis Pros and Cons
  • 14 Pros and Cons of the Cataract Surgery Multifocal Lens
  • 19 Dermaplaning Pros and Cons
  • 15 Mirena IUD Pros and Cons
  • 11 Pros and Cons of Monovision Cataract Surgery
  • Calories Burned
  • Cancer Articles and Infographics
  • Definitions and Examples of Theory
  • Definitions for Kids
  • Dental Articles and Infographics
  • Elder Care Articles and Infographics
  • Environmental
  • Health Research Funding
  • Healthcare Articles and Infographics
  • ICD 9 Codes
  • Major Accomplishments
  • Medical Articles and Infographics
  • Nutrition Articles and Infographics
  • Pharmaceutical Articles and Infographics
  • Psychological Articles and Infographics
  • Skin Articles and Infographics
  • Surgery Articles and Infographics
  • Theories and Models
  • Uncategorized
  • Videos on How to Get Research Funding

Advertisement

Advertisement

Bullying: issues and challenges in prevention and intervention

  • Published: 12 August 2023
  • Volume 43 , pages 9270–9279, ( 2024 )

Cite this article

disadvantages of bullying essay

  • Muhammad Waseem   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-8720-955X 1 , 2 , 3 &
  • Amanda B. Nickerson 4  

1419 Accesses

3 Citations

68 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

Bullying is a public health issue that persists and occurs across several contexts. In this narrative review, we highlight issues and challenges in addressing bullying prevention. Specifically, we discuss issues related to defining, measuring, and screening for bullying. These include discrepancies in the interpretation and measurement of power imbalance, repetition of behavior, and perceptions of the reporter. The contexts of bullying, both within and outside of the school setting (including the online environment), are raised as an important issue relevant for identification and prevention. The role of medical professionals in screening for bullying is also noted. Prevention and intervention approaches are reviewed, and we highlight the need and evidence for social architectural interventions that involve multiple stakeholders, including parents, in these efforts. Areas in need are identified, such as understanding and intervening in cyberbullying, working more specifically with perpetrators as a heterogeneous group, and providing more intensive interventions for the most vulnerable youth who remain at risk despite universal prevention efforts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

disadvantages of bullying essay

Updated Perspectives on Linking School Bullying and Related Youth Violence Research to Effective Prevention Strategies

disadvantages of bullying essay

Bullying and Cyberbullying Throughout Adolescence

disadvantages of bullying essay

Explore related subjects

  • Medical Ethics

Data availability

This manuscript does not involve patient data. This is a review based on the published literature.

Allen, K. P. (2010a). A bullying intervention system in high school: A two-year school-wide follow-up. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 36 (3), 83–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2011.01.002

Article   Google Scholar  

Allen, K. P. (2010b). A bullying intervention system: Reducing risk and creating support for aggressive students. Preventing School Failure, 54 (3), 199–209. https://doi.org/10.1080/10459880903496289

American Educational Research Association (2013). Prevention of bullying in schools, colleges, and universities: Research report and recommendations . https://www.aera.net/Education-Research/Issues-and-Initiatives/Bullying-Prevention-and-School-Safety/Bullying-Prevention

Axford, N., Bjornstad, G., Clarkson, S., Ukoumunne, O. C., Wrigley, Z., Matthews, J., Berry, V., & Hutchings, J. (2020). The effectiveness of the KiVa bullying prevention program in Wales, UK: Results from a pragmatic cluster randomized controlled trial. Prevention Science, 21 (5), 615–626. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-020-01103-9

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Baldry, A. C., & Farrington, D. P. (2007). Effectiveness of programs to prevent school bullying. Victims & Offenders, 2 (2), 183–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/15564880701263155

Barboza, G. E., Schiamberg, L. B., Oehmke, J., Korzeniewski, S. J., Post, L. A., & Heraux, C. G. (2009). Individual characteristics and the multiple contexts of adolescent bullying: An ecological perspective. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38 (1), 101–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9271-1

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Bauman, S. (2016). Do we need more measures of bullying? The Journal of Adolescent Health, 59 (5), 487–488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.08.021

Bear, G. G., Mantz, L. S., Glutting, J. J., Yang, C., & Boyer, D. E. (2015). Differences in bullying victimization between students with and without disabilities. School Psychology Review, 44 (1), 98–116. https://doi.org/10.17105/SPR44-1.98-116

Blake, J. J., Lund, E. M., Zhou, Q., Kwok, O. M., & Benz, M. R. (2012). National prevalence rates of bully victimization among students with disabilities in the United States. School Psychology Quarterly, 27 (4), 210–222. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000008

Borgen, N. T., Olweus, D., Kirkebøen, L. J., Breivik, K., Solberg, M. E., Frønes, I., Cross, D., & Raaum, O. (2021). The potential of anti-bullying efforts to prevent academic failure and youth crime. A case using the Olweus bullying prevention program (OBPP). Prevention Science, 22 , 1147–1158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-021-01254-3

Bostic, J. Q., & Brunt, C. C. (2011). Cornered: An approach to school bullying and cyberbullying, and forensic implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20 (3), 447–465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2011.03.004

Bradshaw, C. P. (2015). Translating research to practice in bullying prevention. American Psychologist, 70 (4), 322–332. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039114

Camodeca, M., & Nava, E. (2022). The long-term effects of bullying, victimization, and bystander behavior on emotion regulation and its physiological correlates. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 37 (3–4), NP2056–NP2075. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260520934438

Carter, S. (2011). Bullies and power: A look at the research. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 34 (2), 97–102. https://doi.org/10.3109/01460862.2011.574455

Cascardi, M., Brown, C., Iannarone, M., & Cardona, N. (2014). The problem with overly broad definitions of bullying: Implications for the schoolhouse, the statehouse, and the ivory tower. Journal of School Violence, 13 (3), 253–276. https://doi.org/10.1080/15388220.2013.846861

Chen, Q., Zhu, Y., & Chui, W. H. (2021). A meta-analysis on the effects of parenting programs on bullying prevention. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 22 (5), 1209–1220. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838020915619

Cheng, Y. Y., Chen, L. M., Ho, H. C., & Cheng, C. L. (2011). Definitions of school bullying in Taiwan: A comparison of multiple perspectives. School Psychology International, 32 (3), 227–243. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034313479694

Cook, C. R., Williams, K. R., Guerra, N. G., Kim, T. E., & Sadek, S. (2010). Predictors of bullying and victimization in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic investigation. School Psychology Quarterly, 25 (2), 65–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020149

Cosgrove, H., & Nickerson, A. B. (2015). Anti-bullying/harassment legislation and educator perceptions of severity, effectiveness, and school climate: A cross-sectional analysis. Educational Policy, 31 (4), 518–545. https://doi.org/10.1177/0895904815604217

Dale, J., Russell, R., & Wolke, D. (2014). Intervening in primary care against childhood bullying: An increasingly pressing public health need. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 107 (6), 219–223. https://doi.org/10.1177/0141076814525071

Deshmukh, P., & Kaplan, S. (2018). Bullying as a risk factor for inpatient hospitalization: Let’s change it! Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57 (10), S260–S260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2018.09.397

Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of enhancing students' social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development, 82 (1), 405–432. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x

Espelage, D. L., & Swearer, S. M. (2003). Research on school bullying and victimization: What have we learned and where do we go from here? School Psychology Review, 32 (3), 365–383. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2003.12086206

Espelage, D. L., Low, S., Polanin, J. R., & Brown, E. C. (2013). The impact of a middle school program to reduce aggression, victimization, and sexual violence. The Journal of Adolescent Health, 53 (2), 180–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.02.021

Espelage, D. L., Low, S., Van Ryzin, M. J., & Polanin, J. R. (2015). Clinical trial of second step middle school program: Impact on bullying, cyberbullying, homophobic teasing, and sexual harassment perpetration. School Psychology Review, 44 (4), 464–479. https://doi.org/10.17105/spr-15-0052.1

Farrington, D. P., & Ttofi, M. M. (2009). School-based programs to reduce bullying and victimization. Campbell Systematic Reviews, 5 (1), 148. https://doi.org/10.4073/csr.2009.6

Farrington, D. P., & Ttofi, M. M. (2011). Bullying as a predictor of offending, violence and later life outcomes. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 21 (2), 90–98. https://doi.org/10.1002/cbm.801

Felix, E. D., Sharkey, J. D., Green, J. G., Furlong, M. J., & Tanigawa, D. (2011). Getting precise and pragmatic about the assessment of bullying: The development of the California bullying victimization scale. Aggressive Behavior, 37 (3), 234–247. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20389

Fredrick, S. S., Nickerson, A. B., & Livingston, J. A. (2021). Family cohesion and the relations among peer victimization and depression: A random intercepts cross-lagged model. Development and Psychopathology, 34 (4), 1429–1446. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942100016X

Frey, K. S., Hirschstein, M. K., Snell, J. L., Edstrom, L. V., MacKenzie, E. P., & Broderick, C. J. (2005). Reducing playground bullying and supporting beliefs: An experimental trial of the steps to respect program. Developmental Psychology, 41 (3), 479–490. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.479

Frey, K. S., Hirschstein, M. K., Edstrom, L. V., & Snell, J. L. (2009). Observed reductions in school bullying, nonbullying aggression and destructive bystander behavior: A longitudinal evaluation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101 (2), 466–481. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013839

Gaffney, H., Farrington, D. P., Espelage, D. L., & Ttofi, M. M. (2019a). Are cyberbullying intervention and prevention programs effective? A systematic and meta-analytical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 45 , 134–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.07.002

Gaffney, H., Ttofi, M., & Farrington, D. (2019b). Evaluating the effectiveness of school-bullying prevention programs: An updated meta-analytical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 45 , 111–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.07.001

Garandeau, C. F., Poskiparta, E., & Salmivalli, C. (2014). Tackling acute cases of school bullying in the KiVa anti-bullying program: A comparison of two approaches. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42 (6), 981–991. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9861-1

Garandeau, C. F., Vartio, A., Poskiparta, E., & Salmivalli, C. (2016). School bullies' intention to change behavior following teacher interventions: Effects of empathy arousal, condemning of bullying, and blaming of the perpetrator. Prevention Science, 17 (8), 1034–1043. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-016-0712-x

Garandeau, C. F., Lee, I. A., & Salmivalli, C. (2018). Decreases in the proportion of bullying victims in the classroom: Effects on the adjustment of remaining victims. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42 (1), 64–72. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025416667492

Gladden, R. M., Vivolo-Kantor, A. M., Hamburger, M. E., & Lumpkin, C. D. (2014). Bullying surveillance among youths: Uniform definitions for public health and recommended data elements, version 1.0 . National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved October 23, 2021, from https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/pdf/bullying-definitions-final-a.pdf

Gómez Baya, D., Rubio Gónzalez, A., & Gaspar de Matos, M. (2018). Online communication, peer relationships and school victimisation: A one-year longitudinal study during middle adolescence. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 24 (2), 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2018.1509793

Good, C. P., McIntosh, K., & Gietz, C. (2011). Integrating bullying prevention into schoolwide positive behavior support. Teaching Exceptional Children, 244 , 48–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/004005991104400106

Hagan, J. F., Shaw, J. S., & Duncan, P. M. (Eds.) (2017). Bright futures: Guidelines for health supervision of infants, children, and adolescents (4th Ed) . American Academy of Pediatrics. https://brightfutures.aap.org/Bright%20Futures%20Documents/BF4_Introduction.pdf

Hall, W. (2017). The effectiveness of policy interventions for school bullying: A systematic review. Journal of the Society for Social Work and Research, 8 (1), 45–69. https://doi.org/10.1086/690565

Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Schwab-Reese, L., Ranapurwala, S. I., Hertz, M. F., & Ramirez, M. R. (2015). Associations between antibullying policies and bullying in 25 states. JAMA Pediatrics, 169 (10), e152411. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.2411

Hensley, V. (2015). Childhood bullying: Assessment practices and predictive factors associated with assessing for bullying by health care providers . University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved October 23, 2021 from https://uknowledge.uky.edu/nursing_etds/25

Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. W. (2015). Bullying beyond the schoolyard: Preventing and responding to cyberbullying (2nd ed.). Sage Publications.

Google Scholar  

Hong, J. S., & Espelage, D. L. (2012). A review of research on bullying and peer victimization in school: An ecological system analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17 (4), 311–322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2012.03.003

Huang, Y., Espelage, D. L., Polanin, J. R., & Hong, J. S. (2019). A meta-analytic review of school-based anti-bullying programs with a parent component. International Journal of Bullying Prevention, 1 (1), 32–44. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42380-018-0002-1

Huitsing, G., Lodder, G. M. A., Browne, W. J., Oldenburg, B., Van der Ploeg, R., & Veenstra, R. (2020). A large-scale replication of the effectiveness of the KiVa Antibullying program: A randomized controlled trial in the Netherlands. Prevention Science, 21 (5), 627–638. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-020-01116-4

Hutson, E., Melnyk, B., Hensley, V., & Sinnott, L. T. (2019). Childhood bullying: Screening and intervening practices of pediatric primary care providers. Journal of Pediatric Health Care, 33 (6), e39–e45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedhc.2019.07.003

Ireland, J. L. (2000). "bullying" among prisoners: A review of research. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5 (2), 201–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(98)00031-7

Kärnä, A., Voeten, M., Little, T. D., Poskiparta, E., Alanen, E., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). Going to scale: A nonrandomized nationwide trial of the KiVa antibullying program for grades 1–9. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79 , 796–805. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025740

Kert, A. S., Codding, R. S., Tryon, G. S., & Shiyko, M. (2010). Impact of the word “bully” on the reported rate of bullying behavior. Psychology in the Schools, 47 (2), 193–204. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20464

Kowalski, R. M., Giumetti, G. W., Schroeder, A. N., & Lattanner, M. R. (2014). Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychological Bulletin, 140 (4), 1073–1137. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035618

Koyanagi, A., Oh, H., Carvalho, A. F., Smith, L., Haro, J. M., Vancampfort, D., Stubbs, B., & DeVylder, J. E. (2019). Bullying victimization and suicide attempt among adolescents aged 12-15 years from 48 countries. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 58 (9), 907–918.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2018.10.018

Lereya, S. T., Copeland, W. E., Costello, E. J., & Wolke, D. (2015). Adult mental health consequences of peer bullying and maltreatment in childhood: Two cohorts in two countries. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2 (6), 524–531. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00165-0

Lund, E. M., & Ross, S. W. (2017). Bullying perpetration, victimization, and demographic differences in college students: A review of the literature. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 18 (3), 348–360. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838015620818

Merrell, K. W., Gueldner, B. A., Ross, S. W., & Isava, D. M. (2008). How effective are school bullying intervention programs? A meta-analysis of intervention research. School Psychology Quarterly, 23 (1), 26–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/1045-3830.23.1.26

Mishna, F. (2004). A qualitative study of bullying from multiple perspectives. Children & Schools, 26 (4), 234–247. https://doi.org/10.1093/cs/26.4.234

Mishna, F., Pepler, D., & Wiener, J. (2006). Factors associated with perceptions and responses to bullying situations by children, parents, teachers, and principals. Victims & Offenders, 1 (3), 255–288. https://doi.org/10.1080/15564880600626163

Monks, C. P., Smith, P. K., Naylor, P., Barter, C., Ireland, J. L., & Coyne, I. (2009). Bullying in different contexts: Commonalities, differences, and the role of theory. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14 , 146–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2009.01.004

Morcillo, C., Ramos-Olazagasti, M. A., Blanco, C., Sala, R., Canino, G., Bird, H., & Duarte, C. S. (2015). Socio-cultural context and bullying others in childhood. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24 (8), 2241–2249. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-014-0026-1

Moreno, M. A., Suthamjariya, N., & Selkie, E. (2018). Stakeholder perceptions of cyberbullying cases: Application of the uniform definition of bullying. The Journal of Adolescent Health, 62 (4), 444–449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.11.289

Musu, L., Zhang, A., Wang, K., Zhang, J., & Oudekerk, B. A. (2019). Indicators of school crime and safety: 2018 (NCES 2019–047/NCJ 252571). National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education, and Bureau of Justice Statistics, Office of Justice Programs, U.S. Department of Justice. https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2019/2019047.pdf

Nelson, H. J., Burns, S. K., Kendall, G. E., & Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (2019). Preadolescent children's perception of power imbalance in bullying: A thematic analysis. PLoS One, 14 (3), e0211124. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211124

Nickerson, A. B. (2019). Preventing and intervening with bullying in schools: A framework for evidence-based practice. School Mental Health, 11 (4), 15–28. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-017-9221-8

Nickerson, A. B., Fredrick, S. S., Allen, K. P., & Jenkins, L. N. (2019). Social emotional learning (SEL) practices in schools: Effects on perceptions of bullying victimization. Journal of School Psychology, 73 , 74–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2019.03.002

Nocentini, A., & Menesini, E. (2016). KiVa anti-bullying program in Italy: Evidence of effectiveness in a randomized control trial. Prevention Science, 17 (8), 1012–1023. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-016-0690-z

Office of the Surgeon General (2023). Social media and youth mental health: The U.S. Surgeon General’s advisory . U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. https://www.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/sg-youth-mental-health-social-media-advisory.pdf

Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do . Blackwell Publishing.

Olweus, D., & Limber, S. P. (2018). Some problems with cyberbullying research. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19 , 139–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.012

Peeters, M., Cillessen, A. H., & Scholte, R. H. (2010). Clueless or powerful? Identifying subtypes of bullies in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39 (9), 1041–1052. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9478-9

Pepler, D. J. (2006). Bullying interventions: A binocular perspective. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15 (1), 16–20.

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Pepler, D., Jiang, D., Craig, W., & Connolly, J. (2008). Developmental trajectories of bullying and associated factors. Child Development, 79 (2), 325–338. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01128.x

Perren, S., & Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, E. (2012). Cyberbullying and traditional bullying in adolescence: Differential roles of moral disengagement, moral emotions, and moral values. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9 (2), 195–209. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2011.643168

Petrosino, A., Guckenburg, S., DeVoe, J., & Hanson, T. (2010). What characteristics of bullying, bullying victims, and schools are associated with increased reporting of bullying to school officials? (Issues & Answers Report, REL 2010–No. 092). U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Regional Educational Laboratory Northeast and Islands. August 2010. https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/edlabs/regions/northeast/pdf/REL_2010092_sum.pdf

Polanin, J. R., Espelage, D. L., & Pigott, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of school-based bullying prevention programs’ effects on bystander intervention behavior. School Psychology Review, 41 (1), 47–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2012.12087375

Polanin, J. R., Espelage, D. L., Grotpeter, J. K., Ingram, K., Michaelson, L., Spinney, E., Valido, A., Sheikh, A. E., Torgal, C., & Robinson, L. (2022). A systematic review and meta-analysis of interventions to decrease cyberbullying perpetration and victimization. Prevention Science, 23 (3), 439–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-021-01259-y

Raman, S., Muhammad, T., Goldhagen, J., Seth, R., Kadir, A., Bennett, S., D'Annunzio, D., Spencer, N. J., Bhutta, Z. A., & Gerbaka, B. (2021). Ending violence against children: What can global agencies do in partnership? Child Abuse & Neglect, 119 (Pt 1), 104733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104733

Rettew, D. C., & Pawlowski, S. (2016). Bullying. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 25 (2), 235–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2015.12.002

Rideout, V. (2017). The Common-sense census: Media use by tweens and teens . Common Sense Media. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/census_researchreport.pdf .

Rideout, V., & Robb, M. B. (2019). Social media, social life: Teens reveal their experiences . Common Sense Media. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/2019-census-8-to-18-key-findings-updated.pdf .

Rodkin, P. C., Espelage, D. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2015). A relational framework for understanding bullying: Developmental antecedents and outcomes. American Psychologist, 70 (4), 311–321. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038658

Ross, S. W., Horner, R. H., & Higbee, T. (2009). Bully prevention in positive behavior support. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42 (4), 747–759. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-747

Sabia, J. J., & Bass, B. (2017). Do anti-bullying laws work? New evidence on school safety and youth violence. Journal of Population Economics, 30 (2), 473–502. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-016-0622-z

Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: A review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 15 (2), 112–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2009.08.007

Singham, T., Viding, E., Schoeler, T., Arseneault, L., Ronald, A., Cecil, C. M., McCrory, E., Rijsdijk, F., & Pingault, J. B. (2017). Concurrent and longitudinal contribution of exposure to bullying in childhood to mental health: The role of vulnerability and resilience. JAMA Psychiatry, 74 (11), 1112–1119. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2678

Smith, P. K., Singer, M., Hoel, H., & Cooper, C. L. (2003). Victimization in the school and the workplace: Are there any links? British Journal of Psychology, 94 (2), 175–188. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661868

Smith, J. D., Schneider, B., Smith, P. K., & Ananiadou, K. (2004). The effectiveness of whole-school anti-bullying programs: A synthesis of evaluation research. School Psychology Review, 33 (4), 548–561. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2004.12086267

Solberg, M., & Olweus, D. (2003). Prevalence estimation of school bullying with the Olweus/bully victim questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior, 29 (3), 239–268. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.10047

Srabstein, J. C., & Leventhal, B. L. (2010). Prevention of bullying-related morbidity and mortality: A call for public health policies. Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 88 (6), 403. https://doi.org/10.2471/BLT.10.077123

Stopbullying.gov. (2021). Laws, policies, and regulations . https://www.stopbullying.gov/resources/laws

Sullivan, T. N., Farrell, A. D., Sutherland, K. S., Behrhorst, K. L., Garthe, R. C., & Greene, A. (2023). Evaluation of the Olweus bullying prevention program in US urban middle schools using a multiple baseline experimental design. Prevention Science, 22 (8), 1134–1146. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-021-01244-5

Swearer, S. M., Wang, C., Maag, J. W., Siebecker, A. B., & Frerichs, L. J. (2012). Understanding the bullying dynamic among students in special and general education. Journal of School Psychology, 50 (4), 503–520. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2012.04.001

Thornberg, R. (2015). The social dynamics of school bullying: The necessary dialogue between the blind men around the elephant and the possible meeting point at the social ecological square. Confero: Essays on Education, Philosophy and Politics, 3 (1), 161–203. https://doi.org/10.3384/confero.2001-4562.1506245

Tiiri, E., Luntamo, T., Mishina, K., Sillanmäki, L., Brunstein Klomek, A., & Sourander, A. (2020). Did bullying victimization decrease after nationwide school-based antibullying program? A time-trend study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59 (4), 531–540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.03.023

Troop-Gordon, W. (2017). Peer victimization in adolescence: The nature, progression, and consequences of being bullied within a developmental context. Journal of Adolescence, 55 , 116–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.012

Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., & Lösel, F. (2012). School bullying as a predictor of violence later in life: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17 (5), 405–418. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2012.05.002

Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., Lösel, F., Crago, R. V., & Theodorakis, N. (2016). School bullying and drug use later in life: A meta-analytic investigation. School Psychology Quarterly, 31 (1), 8–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000120

Vaillancourt, T., McDougall, P., Hymel, S., Krygsman, A., Miller, J., Stiver, K., & Davis, C. (2008). Bullying: Are researchers and children/youth talking about the same thing? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32 (6), 486–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408095553

Volk, A. A., Dane, A. V., & Marini, Z. A. (2014). What is bullying? A theoretical redefinition. Developmental Review, 34 (4), 327–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.09.001

Waasdorp, T. E., Bradshaw, C. P., & Leaf, P. J. (2012). The impact of schoolwide positive behavioral interventions and supports on bullying and peer rejection: A randomized controlled effectiveness trial. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 166 (2), 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.755

Waseem, M., Paul, A., Schwartz, G., Pauzé, D., Eakin, P., Barata, I., Holtzman, D., Benjamin, L. S., Wright, J. L., Nickerson, A. B., & Joseph, M. (2017). Role of pediatric emergency physicians in identifying bullying. The Journal of Emergency Medicine, 52 (2), 246–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jemermed.2016.07.107

Williford, A., Boulton, A., Noland, B., Little, T. D., Kärnä, A., & Salmivalli, C. (2012). Effects of the KiVa anti-bullying program on adolescents’ depression, anxiety, and perception of peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40 , 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9562-y

Winding, T. N., Skouenborg, L. A., Mortensen, V. L., & Anderson, J. H. (2020). Is bullying in adolescence associated with the development of depressive symptoms in adulthood?: A longitudinal cohort study. BMC Psychology, 8 , 122. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-020-00491-5

Xu, M., Macrynikola, N., Waseem, M., & Miranda, R. (2020). Racial and ethnic differences in bullying: Review and implications for intervention. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 50 , 101340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2019.101340

Ybarra, M. L., Boyd, D., Korchmaros, J. D., & Oppenheim, J. K. (2012). Defining and measuring cyberbullying within the larger context of bullying victimization. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51 (1), 53–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.12.031

Yeager, D. S., Fong, C., Lee, H., & Espelage, D. (2015). Declines in efficacy or anti-bullying programs among older adolescents: Theory and a three-level meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 37 (1), 36–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.11.005

Zhang, Q., Cao, Y., & Tian, J. (2021). Effects of violent video games on aggressive cognition and aggressive behavior. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 24 (1), 5–10. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-139260/v1

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Lincoln Medical Center, 234 East 149th Street, Bronx, NY, 10451, USA

Muhammad Waseem

Weill Cornell Medicine, New York, NY, USA

New York Medical College, Valhalla, NY, USA

Alberti Center for Bullying Abuse Prevention, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, 428 Baldy Hall, Buffalo, NY, 14260-1000, USA

Amanda B. Nickerson

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Muhammad Waseem .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

We do not have any conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

N/A (It is a literature review).

Informed consent

N/A (it does not involve any patient involvement).

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Waseem, M., Nickerson, A.B. Bullying: issues and challenges in prevention and intervention. Curr Psychol 43 , 9270–9279 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-05083-1

Download citation

Accepted : 02 August 2023

Published : 12 August 2023

Issue Date : March 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-05083-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Intervention
  • Cyberbullying
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of hhspa

A systematic review and content analysis of bullying and cyber-bullying measurement strategies

Alana m. vivolo-kantor.

a Division of Violence Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, United States

Brandi N. Martell

Kristin m. holland, ruth westby.

b Rollins School of Public Health, Emory University, United States

Bullying has emerged as a behavior with deleterious effects on youth; however, prevalence estimates vary based on measurement strategies employed. We conducted a systematic review and content analysis of bullying measurement strategies to gain a better understanding of each strategy including behavioral content. Multiple online databases (i.e., PsychInfo, MedLine, ERIC) were searched to identify measurement strategies published between 1985 and 2012. Included measurement strategies assessed bullying behaviors, were administered to respondents with ages of 12 to 20, were administered in English, and included psychometric data. Each publication was coded independently by two study team members with a pre-set data extraction form, who subsequently met to discuss discrepancies. Forty-one measures were included in the review. A majority used differing terminology; student self-report as primary reporting method; and included verbal forms of bullying in item content. Eleven measures included a definition of bullying, and 13 used the term “bullying” in the measure. Very few definitions or measures captured components of bullying such as repetition, power imbalance, aggression, and intent to harm. Findings demonstrate general inconsistency in measurement strategies on a range of issues, thus, making comparing prevalence rates between measures difficult.

1. Introduction

Bullying is a form of interpersonal violence that can cause short- and long-term physical, emotional, and social problems among victims, and is, therefore, a serious public health concern ( Copeland, Wolke, Angold, & Costello, 2013 ; Gini & Pozzoli, 2009 ; Nakamoto & Schwartz, 2009 ). However, the magnitude of the problem, the prevalence of bullying behavior, the common antecedents of perpetration, and the consequences of bullying are difficult to interpret because the measurement of bullying remains inconsistent among researchers ( Atik, 2011 ; Furlong, Sharkey, Felix, Tanigawa, & Green, 2010 ). Only recently has the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) and the Department of Education (ED) released the first uniform definition of bullying ( Gladden, Vivolo-Kantor, Hamburger, & Lumpkin, 2014 ), however, standardization of bullying measurement 1 is still needed to provide a better understanding of this problem.

Over the past two decades, the concept of bullying has evolved with research. In 1990, Dan Olweus provided a framework for the most widespread contemporary definition of bullying. Specifically, this definition states that bullying includes three key components: intentional aggression, repetition, and a power imbalance ( Olweus, 1993 ). To date, these three components remain a part of the definition of bullying and have been widely used to measure the problem, but they have not been used in a standardized or systematic manner ( Grief & Furlong, 2006 ; Rigby, 2004 ). There has also been much discussion about additional components that may be required for a behavior to be defined as bullying, including the perpetrator’s intent to cause harm and the victim’s report of experiencing harm ( Greene, 2000 ; Smith, del barrio, & Tokunaga, 2013 ; Smith & Thompson, 1991 ).

CDC and ED’s definition of bullying, while similar to previous definitions, introduces some new concepts. Similar to previous definitions, the three main components of unwanted aggressive behavior, observed or perceived power imbalance, and repetition of behaviors are included. However, the definition differs in three ways from other commonly used definitions of bullying. First, the definition requires aggressive behaviors to be unwanted . This helps to exclude rough and tumble play among youth. Second, bullying can involve a single act of aggression if it is perceived to have a high likelihood of being repeated (e.g., may involve threats of future aggression). The intent for this inclusion is to encourage timely intervention at the first sign of these behaviors instead of waiting for multiple incidents of aggression to occur. Third, the current definition excludes teen dating and sibling violence. Other bullying definitions do not distinguish teen and sibling violence from peer violence ( Gladden et al., 2014 ).

Researchers have also modified the scope of bullying by incorporating both direct modes of bullying (e.g., fighting) and indirect modes (e.g., rumor spreading), distinguishing between types (e.g., physical, verbal, and relational), and distinguishing similar and sometimes overlapping constructs, such as peer victimization and peer aggression ( Farrington, 1993 ; Furlong et al., 2010 ; Olweus, 1993 ). Most recently, bullying has been adapted to include “cyber-bullying,” a form of inter-net and electronic harassment ( Tokunaga, 2010 ).

Although research has provided unique perspectives about bullying and improved understandings of the nature of this form of violence, significant inconsistencies still remain with respect to bullying definitions and measurement strategies currently used in studies. These inconsistencies can provide confiicting prevalence estimates and scientific results. A review and meta-analysis of bullying in school-based studies using varying measurement strategies concluded that 53% of students, on average, reported exposure to bullying (as victims, bullies, or bully/victims). However, prevalence ranges drastically for each category; for bullying perpetration, the range was 5% to 44% ( Cook, Williams, Guerra, & Kim, 2010 ). Inconsistent measurement strategies can also increase the difficulty in monitoring the problem through public health surveillance initiatives and evaluating the impact and progress of public health bullying prevention interventions. For example, the Youth Risk Behavior Survey (YRBS) and the School Crime Supplement to National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS) measure bullying and cyberbullying in drastically different ways. The 2013 YRBS bullying questions starts with “ Bullying is when 1 or more students tease , threaten , spread rumors about , hit , shove , or hurt another student over and over again. It is not bullying when 2 students of about the same strength or power argue or fight or tease each other in a friendly way. During the past 12 months , have you ever been bullied on school property ?” and “ During the past 12 months , have you ever been electronically bullied ? ( Include being bullied through e - mail , chat rooms , instant messaging , Web sites , or texting .)” Using response options of “yes” or “no”, CDC found that 19.6% reported being bullied and 14.8% reported being electronically bullied ( Kann et al., 2014 ). Yet, using the NCVS with a similar age group and time frame, the 2009 results demonstrated larger prevalence rates for school bullying victimization (32%) and smaller prevalence for electronic bullying (4%) ( DeVoe & Murphy, 2011 ). To measure bullying, the NCVS asks youth, “ Now I have some questions about what students do at school that make you feel bad or are hurtful to you. We often refer to this as being bullied. You may include events you told me about already. During this school year , has any student bullied you ? That is , has another student …” followed by a set of seven behavioral questions (i.e., made fun of you, called you names, or insulted you; spread rumors about you; threatened you with harm).

1.1. Goal of the current study

This review identifies various measurement strategies of bullying behaviors among youth and provides suggestions for standardizing measurement for research and surveillance purposes. Specifically, we examined youth bullying measurement strategies to identify variations in data collection methods, terminology used, and definitional components. We provide an overview of reliable measures that identify specific components of bullying (e.g., power imbalance, intention to harm, and repetition) and the content of bullying behaviors (e.g., hitting, kicking, rumor spreading). We also provide an overall assessment of bullying measurement strategies, indicate the usefulness of acknowledged measures, and recognize the next steps to establish a well-constructed measure of bullying behaviors.

2.1. Literature search

A systematic search was conducted for all bullying and cyber-bullying measurement strategies published between 1985 and 2012. First, key search terms were drawn from a review of the literature and included such terms as bully*, violen*, aggress*, victim*, harass*, exclude*, bystand*, measure*, tool*, and survey*. The search terms were used in combination with each other to narrow the search results. For example, the terms “bullying”, “victimization” and “survey” were entered simultaneously to retrieve relevant publications. For a full list of included search terms, please contact the corresponding author of this article.

Using these terms, a search was performed of the following electronic databases: PsychInfo, PsychArticles, MedLine, ERIC, the Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, the Professional Development Collection, SocIndex with Full Text, Expanded Academic Index ASAP, and Science Direct. The abstracts of all relevant articles were screened for inclusion eligibility. When there was sufficient indication that a publication abstract was appropriate for consideration, the publication was retrieved, the full publication was reviewed, and the measure was obtained by contacting authors or copyright holders if it was not available within the publication.

In addition to the search described above, we also included measurement strategies published in the CDC document, “ Measuring Bullying Victimization , Perpetration , and Bystander Experiences: A Compendium of Assessment Tools ” (also known as the Bullying Compendium ) ( Hamburger, Basile, & Vivolo, 2011 ). The Bullying Compendium represents a comprehensive list of bullying measurement strategies used by researchers in the field; however, the Compendium does not provide an in-depth overview of the measures. The current paper, on the other hand, provides a detailed review of constructs measured and definitions used for each bullying measurement strategy, and also identifies advantages and drawbacks of each in an attempt to more consistently guide research efforts.

2.2. Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Measurement strategies included in this study were ones that: (a) assessed bullying behaviors , including physical/psychological bullying and victimization, cyber-bullying/electronic aggression, relational aggression, sexualized and homophobic bullying, and bullying bystander behaviors; (b) was administered to respondents between, but not limited to, age 12 and 20, or administered to parents, teachers, peers, or other individuals who could report on the behaviors of youth within this age range; (c) was developed or revised and examined between 1985 and 2012; and (d) was administered in English. We also reviewed measurement strategies described in published peer-reviewed and non-peer reviewed journals, book chapters, and online sources. Regardless of how the measurement strategy was published, the final inclusion criterion was that (e) the publication included psychometric data. Studies were excluded if they did not meet the abovementioned criteria or the measurement strategy: (a) only assessed beliefs, attitudes, or perceptions; (b) was not a minor adaptation of included measures; (c) did not include psychometric data; (d) was not a scale or index; or (e) explicitly explored workplace bullying. We also excluded measures if we (f) could not retrieve the publication for review after contacting the developer(s).

Minor adaptations were defined as any adaptation that kept intact the response options, time frame, and content of the items. In most cases, minor adaptations included decreasing the number of items. In several situations, measures were shortened to improve reliability and validity. In these situations, both measures were included and reviewed. Also, this review focuses on measures that can be defined as scales or indices. Scales are defined as multiple items that measure only one concept using the same or similar response options, while indices include multiple questions with different response options that may not be correlated with other index items ( Bollen & Lennox, 1991 ; DiIorio, 2006 ; Salazar, DiClemente, & Crosby, 2011 ). We have excluded one-item scales because these typically lack precision, may change over time, and are narrowly defined ( Spector, 1992 ). One-item measures of bullying victimization and perpetration typically involve assessing whether an individual has ever experienced a behavior. While this may be relevant information for assessing bullying prevalence, these measures do not provide a thorough understanding of bullying behaviors.

2.3. Search results

The search began in October, 2012 and concluded in April, 2013. Over 1000 publications were screened, and a total of 164 bullying measures were deemed relevant for abstract screening. Following abstract screening, the publications for 69 measurement strategies were retrieved and reviewed for eligibility. Twenty-eight were subsequently excluded because they failed to meet our inclusion criteria. Forty-one publications met the inclusion criteria and were included in this review. Fig. 1 provides detailed information regarding reasons for publication exclusion.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms748290f1.jpg

PRISMA flow of information through the different phases of a systematic review.

2.4. Data extraction and coding process

2.4.1. data extraction form.

A data extraction form was developed to capture all information required to complete this review. The standardized form included 32 questions covering a range of information. The form included two items on publication information: publication type (e.g., journal versus report) and publication year. We collected data on sample size characteristics, including the total number of participants in the sample, the target age range, and the target grade level. The form was also used to collect information about the measurement strategy implementation setting (e.g., school-based survey, clinic-based survey) and location (e.g., domestic vs. international).

The field currently uses an array of terminology and definitions to describe bullying ( Swearer, Siebecker, Johnsen-Frerichs, & Wang, 2010 ). The extraction form captured information about how the authors described bullying in each publication. For example, some publications used the term “peer victimization” or “peer aggression” when describing bullying. Our form also captured information on whether participants were provided a specific definition of bullying and if so, what information the definition encompassed. Because little consensus exists among researchers regarding which components should be included in a bullying definition ( Gladden et al., 2014 ), we noted which of the five most commonly used components, if any, were included (i.e., power imbalance, repetition, intent to harm, victim experiences harm, and intentional aggressive behaviors).

The reliability and validity of each measure were recorded. In situations where the primary publication used for coding referenced a different publication when discussing psychometric data, the study team retrieved and coded that publication only for psychometric data.

We captured data specific to each measurement strategy such as the author-reported type(s) of bullying included in the measure (i.e., relational, verbal, physical, indirect, and/or direct). We also noted the reporter for each strategy (e.g., youth self-report, peer nomination), the number of items in the measure, the response options (e.g., yes/no, Likert scale), the time frame assessed (e.g., past week), whether the measurement strategy specifically included the term “bullying”, how the measure was scored, and if the measure captured victimization, perpetration, or bystander experiences.

Because of potential differences between the manner in which the author defined bullying and the measures administered, we captured data on the components that were measured. For example, if a measure assessed repetition, a description of how repetition was measured (e.g., one incident occurring over a span of time, more than one incident involving the same perpetrator or group of perpetrators) was subsequently recorded. Similarly, we specified how power imbalance was measured (e.g., perpetrator(s) has more physical strength, multiple perpetrators, perpetrator(s) is more popular). In addition, we captured data on the content of behaviors assessed by each measure such as hitting, kicking, throwing objects, destructing property, spreading rumors, name calling, homophobic teasing, and threatening.

2.4.2. Coding process

Prior to coding all publications, all four members of the study team independently coded the same publication. After confirming high levels of agreement across coders and coding form questions using Cohen’s kappa (range per question κ = 0.82–1.00) ( Landis & Koch, 1977 ), each publication was coded independently by two study team members. The two coders assigned to each publication subsequently met to discuss any discrepancies. When two coders did not agree on a code, the other team members were consulted and provided recommendations, so that a consensus was reached among all team members.

2.5. Data analyses

IBM SPSS Statistics 21 was used to conduct analyses focused on characterizing the features of included measurement strategies with frequencies and other descriptive statistics. In several situations, specifically those requiring analysis of responses to open-ended questions from the data extraction form, new variables were created to re-code the data extracted from the measurement strategies.

To best capture pertinent information about the content of bullying behaviors assessed by each measure, a summative, or manifest, content analysis ( Potter & Levine-Donnerstein, 1999 ) was completed, where the appearance of particular content within the items (e.g., hit, kick, or push) was coded and grouped to include all similar contents in each measure. Using the example provided above, all items that included hitting, kicking, or pushing of another youth were combined to form a content area of “physical bullying”. This process was used to determine all relevant bullying behavior contents. A total of 17 behavior content categories were identiied.

3.1. Measurement strategy characteristics

Of the 41 included measurement strategies, most were published in peer-reviewed journal articles (n = 39, 95.1%) between 1988 and 2012, with the majority published after 2003 (n = 27; 65.8%). Measures were administered among samples within the United States (n = 19, 46.3%) and internationally (n = 15, 36.6%), as well as in multiple countries simultaneously (n = 3, 7.3%). The mean sample size was 1089 (SD = 1638; range, 47–8693). Almost all measurement strategies were implemented in schools (n = 38, 92.8%), but several were implemented in other settings such as prisons (n = 1, 2.4%) and by mail (n = 1, 2.4%). One setting (2.4%) was unknown. The included measures were administered among youth between the ages of 3 and 25; the average age range was 10.59–15.73 years. Table 1 provides more information on measure characteristics.

Characteristics of the 41 included measures.

VariablesMean (Range)Count (Percent)
Sample size1089.4 (47–8693)
Age range 10.5–15.7 (3–25)
Grade level range 5.9–8.9 (0–16)
Number of items27.4 (5–96)
Publication year2003.4 (1988–2012)
U.S.19 (46.3)
International15 (36.5)
Multiple countries3 (7.3)
“ ”
Yes13 (31.7)
No28 (68.3)
Yes11 (26.8)
No21 (51.2)
Unknown9 (22)
,
Power imbalance9 (81.8)
Intention to cause harm9 (81.8)
Victim experiences harm6 (66.7)
Repetition7 (63.6)
Aggressive behaviors8 (72.7)
Youth self-report only31 (75.6)
Peer nomination only5 (12.2)
Combination of youth self-report and peer nomination2 (4.9)
Teacher report2 (4.9)
Parent report1 (2.4)
Binary (e.g., yes/no, true/false)11 (26.8)
Scale/index33 (80.5)
Open-ended (includes peer nomination)10 (24.4)
Multiple choice2 (4.9)
Past 7 days/week4 (9.8)
Past 30 days/month9 (22)
Past 2 to 3 months or school semester3 (7.3)
Past 4 months1 (2.4)
Current school year2 (4.9)
Past 12 months/year1 (2.4)
Other (e.g., recently or when growing up)4 (9.8)
Unknown17 (41.4)
Perpetration-only3 (7.3)
Victimization-only8 (19.5)
Both perpetration and victimization29 (70.8)
Bystander with either perpetration or victimization1 (2.4)
All three: perpetration, victimization, and bystander7 (17.1)

3.1.1. Terminology and types of bullying

The use of varying terminology by authors was captured in this review. The majority described bullying as the behavior(s) assessed by the measure (n = 29, 70.7%); however, a notable proportion used the terms “peer victimization” (n = 14, 34.1%) and “peer aggression” (n = 12, 29.3%) to describe the behaviors they measured. About a third of the strategies used the term bullying either as a precursor to the measure (e.g., “How did you get bullied?” followed by several behavioral items) ( Swearer & Cary, 2003 ) or as an item within the measure (e.g., “I was bullied at school,” [( Hinduja & Patchin, 2010 )]) (n = 13, 31.7%). In most cases, youth self-report of bullying behavior was the primary assessment method (n = 35, 85.4%), followed by peer nomination (n = 9, 22%). Occasionally (n = 4, 9.7%), self-report and peer nomination were used together. In two measures, self-report, peer nomination, and teacher-report were used in conjunction.

The author-reported type(s) of bullying were captured for 38 of the 41 measures (7.3% were unknown). Most often, authors described the inclusion of items assessing verbal forms of bullying (n = 34, 82.9%), followed by direct bullying (n = 30, 73.2), physical bullying (n = 29, 70.7%), relational bullying (n = 22, 53.7%), indirect bullying (n = 17, 41.5%), and cyber-bullying (n = 7, 17.1%). For 29 measures (70.7%), the authors stated that physical and verbal bullying were both included, while no authors said physical, verbal, and relational bullying were all included in the measurement strategy. However, 13 (31.7%) authors stated that physical, verbal, and indirect bullying were measured.

3.1.2. Assessment of behavioral content

The content of the items included in the measures also varied drastically across publications. While we captured the author-stated type of bullying being measured as mentioned earlier (i.e., physical, verbal, relational, cyber, indirect, direct, other), a more in-depth content analysis of the measures’ items was conducted to accurately categorize the behavioral content. Table 2 lists the 17 behavioral content categories developed by analyzing the items in all measures and reports the measures that assessed each construct. Many measures included items on making fun/teasing/embarrassing others (n = 33, 80.5%), physical bullying (n = 32, 78%), specific name calling (n = 30, 73.2%), threatening others (n = 25, 61%), socially excluding others from groups or activities (n = 23, 56.1%), and making rude comments and gestures (n = 22, 53.7%).

Behavior content of the 41 included measures.

Behavior contentCount (Percent)
Making fun, teasing, embarrassing33 (80.5)
Physical acts32 (78.0)
Calling names30 (73.2)
Making threats25 (61.0)
Rude comments and gestures23 (56.1)
Social exclusion23 (56.1)
Stealing or damaging property18 (43.9)
Spreading rumors15 (36.6)
Cyber-bullying10 (24.4)
Sexual harassment8 (19.5)
Legal harassment7 (17.1)
Group bullying7 (17.1)
Bystander or witness7 (17.1)
Other broad behaviors6 (14.6)
Weight-based teasing6 (14.6)
Homophobic teasing2 (4.9)
Weapon-carrying2 (4.9)

3.1.3. Use of definitions and measuring definitional components

Eleven measurement strategies (26.8%) included a definition of bullying. Among those with definitions, less than half (n = 4, 36.4%) captured all five components (i.e., power imbalance, intention to harm, victim experiences harm, repetition, and aggressive behaviors) recommended by the field ( Gladden et al., 2014 ; Olweus, 1993 ) for inclusion in a bullying definition. Three (27.2%) captured four components, and four (36.4%) captured three or less. The components most often included in the definition were power imbalance (n = 9, 81.8%), intention to cause harm (n = 9, 81.8%), aggressive behaviors (n = 8, 72.7%), repetition (n = 7, 63.6%), and victim experiences harm (n = 6, 54.5%).

Because most measures did not include an explicit definition of bullying, we identified which components of the recommended bullying definition were assessed by the measure (e.g., inclusion of items or response options that allowed for a power imbalance to be measured). Regardless of whether a definition was provided, items measuring aggressive behaviors were included most often (n = 39, 95.1%), followed by repetition (n = 32, 78%), intention to harm (n = 26, 63.4%), victim experiences harm (n = 16, 39%), and power imbalance (n = 9, 22%). When segregating the data by those measures with and without a definition, Fisher’s exact test yielded no significant differences at the p = 0.01-level for the definitional components actually measured in the scale or index.

For the 32 measures where repetition was denoted, the study team determined how repetition was measured. In about half of these measures (n = 17, 53.1%), authors used broad frequency response options for each behavioral item such as “How often have you taken things from other students?” with response choices “never”, “sometimes”, and “often” ( Raine et al., 2006 ). In the rest of the measures (n = 15, 46.9%), repetition was assessed using the actual number of times the incident occurred (e.g., “Some kids call each other names such as gay, lesbo, fag, etc. How many times in the last week did you say these things to a friend?” Response options included never, 1 or 2 times, 3 or 4 times, 5 or 6 times, and 7 or more times) ( Poteat & Espelage, 2005 ).

When a power imbalance was denoted in the measurement strategy (n = 9), the same process as was used for repetition was implemented. Most often (n = 5, 55.6%) items included mention of the perpetrators’ physical strength (e.g., “Please think about the main person or leader who did these things to you in the past month. How physically strong is this student?” Response options included “less than me”, “same as me”, and “more than me”) ( Felix, Sharkey, Green, Furlong, & Tanigawa, 2011 ), followed by items describing multiple perpetrators (n = 3, 33.3%) (e.g., “In the past month a group of kids tried to beat me up.” Response options included never, once or twice, three or four times, and five or more times) ( Peters & Bain, 2011 ), perpetrators who were older/in a higher grade (n = 3, 33.3%), perpetrators who were more popular (n = 1, 11.1%), perpetrators who were adults (n = 1, 11.1%), and perpetrators who were smarter (n = 1, 11.1%). Table 3 provides additional information about the measures that included definitions, the components included in the definition, and a detailed breakdown of these components.

Definition characteristics and measured components of included measures.

MeasureTotal itemsReliabilityDefinition usedDefinition componentsComponents measured
Power imbalanceIntent to cause harmRepetitionAggressive behaviorsVictim experiences harm
Adolescent Peer Relations Instrument ( )36Victimization: α = 0.89; perpetration: α = 0.82Non/aXX
Aggression Scale ( )11Perpetration: α = 0.88Non/aXXX
Bull-S Questionnaire ( )25Victimization: α = 0.83; Perpetration: α = 0.82Non/aX
Bullying-Behavior Scale ( )12Victimization: α = 0.82; perpetration: α = 0.83Non/aXX
California Bullying Victimization Scale ( )12κ for 8 items = 0.46–0.64; κ for bully/victim = 0.71Non/aXXXXX
Child Social Behavior Questionnaire ( )53Peer nomination: α = 0.90; teacher report: α = 0.90; self-report: α = 0.68Non/aXXXX
Children’s Scale of Hostility and Aggression: Reactive/Proactive (C-SHARP) ( )58Verbal aggression: α = 0.92; bullying: α = 0.89; covert aggression: α = 0.88; physical aggression: α = 0.74Non/aXX
Children’s Social Behavior Scale — Self Report ( )15Overt aggression: α = 0.94; relational aggression: α = 0.83Non/aXXX
Cyber Victim & Bullying Scale ( )44Victimization: α = 0.89, split half = 0.79, test–retest = 0.85; perpetration: α = 0.89, split half = 0.79, test–retest = 0.90Non/aXX
Direct and Indirect Patient Behavior Checklist — Prisoner Version ( )87Direct bullying: α = 0.91; verbal bullying: α = 0.72; physical bullying: α = 0.78; indirect bullying: α = 0.77; theft: α = 0.85; sexual harassment: α = 0.79Non/aXXXX
Gatehouse Bullying Scale ( )12Any bullying: rho test–retest = 0.65, κ= 0.63Non/aXXX
Homophobic Bullying Scale ( )42Homophobic aggression towards gay men: α = 0.82; homophobic aggression towards lesbian women: α = 0.87; homophobic victimization: α = 0.86; witnessing homophobic aggression towards gay men: α = 0.82; witnessing homophobic aggression towards lesbian women: α = 0.83Non/aXXX
Homophobic Content Agent Target Scale ( )10Victimization: α = 0.85; perpetration: α = 0.85YesNoneXX
Illinois Bully Scale ( )18Victimization: α = 0.88; perpetration: α = 0.87; physical fighting: α = 0.83Non/aXXX
Introducing My Classmates ( )8n/aNon/aXX
Modified Aggression Scale ( )5Perpetration: α = 0.83Non/aXXX
Modified Peer Nomination Inventory ( )14Victimization: α = 0.96; split-half alpha range = 0.78–0.98; test–retest r = 0.93 over 3 monthsNon/aXX
Multidimensional Peer-Victimization Scale ( )16Physical victimization: α = 0.85; verbal victimization: α = 0.75; social manipulation: α = 0.77; attacks on property: α = 0.73YesPower imbalance
Intent to cause harm
XXX
New Participant Role Scale ( )32Perpetrator: α = 0.96; follower: α = 0.95; outsider: α = 0.94; defender: α = 0.88; victim: α = 0.93Non/aXX
Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire ( )36Victimization: α = 0.88; perpetration: α = 0.87YesPower imbalance
Intent to cause harm
Repetition
Aggressive behaviors
Victim experiences harm
XXXXX
Pacific Rim Bullying Measure ( )12Victimization: α = 0.73; perpetration: α = 0.77YesPower imbalance
Repetition
Aggressive behaviors
XXX
Participant Role Questionnaire ( )15Perpetration: α = 0.93; assistant α = 0.95; reinforcer α = 0.90; defender α = 0.89; outsider α = 0.88YesPower imbalance
Intent to cause harm
Repetition
Aggressive behaviors
Victim experiences harm
XXX
Peer Interactions in Primary School Questionnaire ( )22Total α = 0.90; victimization: test–retest r = 0.87; perpetration: test–retest r = 0.76Non/aXXXX
Peer Relations Assessment Questionnaire ( )20Victimization α = 0.86 (School A), α = 0.78 (School B); perpetration α = 0.75 (School A), α = 0.78 (School B)Non/aXXXX
Peer Relationship Survey ( )20Victimization: α = 0.89; perpetration: α = 0.90YesPower imbalance
Intent to cause harm
Repetition
Aggressive behaviors
Victim experiences harm
XX
Perception of Teasing Scale ( )22Weight-teasing victimization α = 0.88, test–retest for frequency = 0.90, test–retest for effect = 0.85; competence-teasing victimization α = 0.75, test–retest for frequency = 0.82, test–retest for effect = 0.66Non/aXXX
Personal Experiences Checklist ( )32Overall test–retest r = 0.79 (0.61– 0.86); verbal–relational bullying: α = 0.91, test–retest r = 0.75; cyber bullying α = 0.90, test–retest r = 0.86; physical bullying α = 0.91, test–retest r = 0.61; bullying based on culture α = 0.78, test–retest r = 0.77Non/aXX
Physical Appearance Related Teasing Scale ( )18Weight-size teasing victimization α = 0.91, test–retest = 0.86; general appearance teasing victimization α = 0.71, test–retest = 0.87Non/aX
Reactive–Proactive Aggression Questionnaire ( )23Reactive perpetration: α = 0.84; proactive perpetration: α = 0.86; total perpetration: α = 0.90Non/aXXXXX
Retrospective Bullying Questionnaire ( )44Primary school victimization: test– retest r = 0.88; secondary school victimization test–retest r = 0.87YesPower imbalance
Intent to cause harm
Repetition
Aggressive behaviors
XX
Reynolds Bully-Victimization Scale for Schools ( )46Victimization: α = 0.93, test–retest r = 0.80; perpetration: α = 0.93, test–retest r = 0.81Non/aXXXXX
School Climate and Bullying Scale ( )59Victimization: α = 0.75YesPower imbalance
Intent to cause harm
Aggressive behaviors
Victim experiences harm
XXXXX
Self Report Inventory of Setting the Record Straight ( )15Victimization: α = 0.88; perpetration: α = 0.72Non/aXX
Social Bullying Involvement Scales ( )96Social victim: α = 0.97; social bully α = 0.93; social witness: α = 0.96; social intervener: α = 0.97YesPower imbalance
Intent to cause harm
Repetition
Aggressive behaviors
Victim experiences harm
XXXX
Survey of Knowledge of Internet Risk & Internet Behavior ( )10Total: α = 0.69Non/aXX
The School Life Survey ( )24Victimization α = 0.83, test–retest r = 0.939; perpetration test–retest r = 0.835Non/aXXX
The Swearer Bully Survey ( )31Total: α = 0.87; physical bullying: α = 0.79; verbal bullying: α = 0.85YesPower imbalance
Intent to cause harm
Repetition
Aggressive behaviors
XXXXX
Traditional Bullying and Cyberbullying Survey ( )34Victimization: α = 0.88; perpetration: α = 0.88; cyber victimization: α = 0.74; cyber perpetration: α = 0.76Non/aXXXX
Victimization of Self and Victimization of Others ( )18Victimization: α = 0.85; perpetration: α = 0.78YesIntent to cause harm
Victim experiences harm
XXXX
Victimization Scale ( )10Victimization: α = 0.86Non/aXXXX
Weight-Based Teasing Scale ( )5Victimization: α = 0.84Non/aXX

3.2. Scoring strategies

Scoring strategies for each measure varied by publication. However, in over half of the measures (n = 21, 51.2%), responses were summed to yield a total score for the overall scale/index or subscale. This summed score was then used as a continuous outcome variable where higher scores were predictive of higher levels of perpetration, victimization, or bystander experiences. Eleven measures (26.8%) classified bullying into binary categories by either summing across responses and dichotomizing based on “never” versus “ever” or creating binary categories by using a cut-off score. For example, the Traditional Bullying and Cyber-bullying Scale ( Hinduja & Patchin, 2010 ) creates a summed score for each subscale (e.g., bullying victimization, bullying perpetration, cyber-bullying victimization, and cyber-bullying perpetration) and then dichotomizes each subscale into “never/once or twice” to denote no or low frequency of bullying versus “three or more times” to denote higher frequency of bullying. In another example, the California Bully Victimization Scale ( Felix et al., 2011 ) categorized participants into “bullied victims” by using a cut-off score where youths were classified as bullied victims if they reported victimization of one type of bullying (i.e., teasing) at least 2–3 times a month or more and endorsed at least one type of power imbalance. Scoring for measures that included peer nomination (n = 5, 12.2%) was mostly determined by calculating an individual score for each youth nomination and summing across all categories (i.e., victimization or perpetration). The Participant Role Questionnaire ( Salmivalli & Voeten, 2004 ) computes youths’ peer-evaluated sum scores on each of the five subscales and divides by the number of peer evaluators, which produces a continuous score from 0 (never) to 2 (a lot) for each student on each subscale. Scores range from 0 to 24 for victimization and 0 to 20 for perpetration, with higher scores indicating more experiences as a victim or bully.

3.3. Validity and reliability

All included measurement strategies reported validity and/or reliability statistics. However, not all strategies reported the same types of statistics. Specifically, 13 (31.7%) reported several types of validity tests such as face; construct (e.g., convergent and discriminant); and criterion validity (e.g., concurrent and predictive). For construct validity, scales were compared to other bullying scales such as the Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire ( Solberg & Olweus, 2003 ) or The Swearer Bully Survey ( Swearer & Cary, 2003 ); teacher assessments and observations; and other student self-report measures such as prosocial behaviors or other behavioral or attitudinal predictor variables. Other scales, for example the Multidimensional Peer-Victimization Scale ( Mynard & Joseph, 2000 ), compared several bullying-related questions to ensure convergent validity. Youth were first asked to self-report victimization by answering yes or no to the question, “Have you ever been bullied?” and were grouped into “victims” and “non-victims.” Then youths responded to 16 behaviorally-based items that began with, “How often during the last school year has another pupil done these things to you?” Specific items included called me names, punched me, and made other people not talk to me. Comparisons found convergent validity with significant mean differences on self-reports of being bullied between victims and non-victims among all four main factors—physical victimization, verbal victimization, social manipulation, and attacks on property.

Of the 41 measurement strategies, 37 (90.2%) reported Cronbach’s alpha for internal consistency, 11 (26.8%) reported test–retest reliability statistics, and one (2.4%) reported split-half reliability. See Table 3 for details on the reliability of each measure.

3.3.1. Internal consistency

Internal consistency ranged from α = 0.25–0.96 (mean = 0.82) for overall bullying perpetration and α = 0.68–0.97 (mean = 0.84) for overall bullying victimization.

3.3.2. Test–retest reliability

Only one measure reported an overall test–retest (i.e., perpetration and victimization combined) correlation; r = 0.79 when youth ages 10–13 were surveyed two weeks apart. Test–retest correlations for victimization ranged from r = 0.61–0.94 (mean = 0.82) and perpetration ranged from r = 0.76–0.90 (mean = 0.83).

3.3.3. Split-half reliability

One measure reported split-half reliability, where the measure was split into two sections and scores for each section were compared to determine consistency in measurement. These correlations ranged from r = 0.55 to r = 0.82.

4. Discussion

The aim of the current study was to conduct a systematic review and content analysis of bullying measures administered to youth, teachers, and parents in an effort to gain a better understanding of the strategies employed and the specific components of bullying being measured. Findings suggest that there are important discrepancies between bullying measurement strategies, such as the time frame used to assess when bullying occurred, the components included in bullying definitions, and the behavioral content of measures provided to participants. Of the 41 measures included in this review, most were implemented in school settings, and very few measured bullying occurring outside of schools or in homes. Cyber-bullying, which has traditionally been viewed as an issue not addressed by schools, was not assessed by most of the measures included in this study.

The most predominant method used to assess bullying was youth self-report. While self-report has been the most widely used method, many have suggested that challenges exist in using this method as the sole strategy to collect information on an individual’s behavior ( Furlong, Sharkey, Bates, & Smith, 2004 ; Leff, Power, & Goldstein, 2004 ). Because it is important to achieve the most accurate assessment of the frequency and magnitude of these behaviors, multiple methods should be considered. For instance, prevalence estimates may increase as the awareness of what constitutes bullying increases, thus self-report alone may not be sensitive enough to detect real changes in the rate of bullying. In addition, the field knows very little about the accuracy of self-report bullying measurement. Only a handful of studies have introduced peer nomination, school records, or parent report to supplement information gleaned from youth self-report ( Cornell & Brockenbrough, 2004 ; Leff et al., 2011 ), and only four measures in this review used multiple reporters to assess bullying. In fact, research by Cornell and Brockenbrough (2004) found very low agreement among student self-report, peer nomination, and teacher nomination of students as victims of bullying in a rural sample of middle school youth. Interestingly, they found better agreement between peer nomination and teacher nomination at identifying both bullying perpetrators and victims. Because this research raises concern about the sole use of student self-report measurement methods, future research should aim to implement multiple-source reporting to assess bullying behaviors with a national sample of youth.

Regardless of reporting method, almost all of the measures in this review captured both victimization and perpetration of bullying. With increasing evidence that youth are often both victims and perpetrators, it is important to continue to capture both behaviors in measurement. These individuals, also called “bully/victims,” report negative outcomes as much as, if not more than, individuals who are only victims or only perpetrators ( Haynie et al., 2001 ; Nansel, Craig, Overpeck, Saluja, & Ruan, 2004 ; Veenstra et al., 2005 ). In this review, few measures included items to better understand bystanding or witnessing behaviors even though there is mounting evidence that bystanders or witnesses also experience similar deleterious effects of bullying ( Nishina & Juvonen, 2005 ; Rivers, Poteat, Noret, & Ashurst, 2009 ). Without capturing victimization, perpetration, and bystanding behavior measurement, it could be more difficult to target interventions for those most at risk.

The field of bullying is in desperate need of uniform terminology and definitions to describe these behaviors ( Swearer et al., 2010 ; Vivolo, Holt, & Massetti, 2011 ). In this review, authors used several terms to discuss bullying behaviors, including peer victimization and peer aggression. The use of inconsistent terminology is problematic for several reasons. First, specific to peer victimization and peer aggression, the term “peer” denotes someone of equal status, age, or grade. However, one of the key constructs in bullying definitions, as mentioned earlier, is the presence of a power differential or imbalance in the relationship between the victim and the perpetrator. Thus, this terminology is in direct conflict with the construct of bullying.

Second, there is a growing literature that distinguishes aggression, fighting, and even legally-defined harassment, from bullying ( Crick & Dodge, 1996 ). Many researchers have developed subscales that measure fighting separate from bullying. For example, analyses of the Illinois Bully Scale ( Espelage & Holt, 2001 ; Espelage, Low, Rao, Hong, & Little, 2013 ) have demonstrated using confirmatory factor analysis that the items capturing physical fighting result in a subscale different from bullying, which included behaviors such as teasing other students, upsetting other students for the fun of it, excluding others from their group of friends, helping to harass other students, and threatening to hit or hurt another student. It is possible that these forms of aggression and violence differ by perceived reasoning and decision processes. Research by Crick and Dodge (1996) interpreted the differences between children who use proactive (i.e., a deliberate behavior to obtain a desired goal) and reactive (i.e., a response driven by anger, frustration, or provocation) aggression. Additionally, there is some evidence that interventions that target physical fighting and other forms of aggression or youth violence are unsuccessful in preventing bullying behaviors ( Espelage, Low, Polanin, & Brown, 2013 ; Taub, 2002 ; Van Schoiack-Edstrom, Frey, & Beland, 2002 ), and some bullying prevention programs are not effective at preventing violence and aggression ( Ferguson, San Miguel, Kilburn, & Sanchez, 2007 ). This illustrates the need to address bullying as a distinct construct that should be examined separately from physical fighting and aggression that is neither repeated, nor involves a power imbalance.

Further, this review uncovered 13 measures that included the term bullying in their strategy and 11 that included a bullying definition. Using the term bullying without providing additional guidance for youth in the form of a definition or list of behaviors may be problematic, as research is mixed on how youth perceive this term. Smith, Cowie, Olafsson, and Liefooghe (2002) found that terms like “bullying” and “picking on” clustered together while terms such as “harassment” and “intimidation” fell into a separate cluster; thus, these terms are not always synonymous with bullying. Using the term bullying in measurement may also impact prevalence. Results from Kert, Codding, Tryon, and Shiyko (2010) show that youth reported significantly less bullying behavior when the word “bully” was provided in a measure than youth not provided a measure with the term included. Another problem with using the term “bullying” in measurement is the fact that recent research suggests that youths may not perceive bullying as researchers do. Land (2003) presented several terms to students (i.e., teasing, bullying, and sexual harassment) and asked them to provide examples of what constituted these terms. The author found that a key component of bullying (e.g., repetition) was not included in students’ examples. Similarly, research from Vaillancourt et al. (2008) revealed discrepancies with respect to youths not including power imbalance and intentionality in a definition of bullying.

Skepticism around using a researcher-developed definition without explicit examples of bullying behaviors has increased over time. Vaillancourt et al. (2008) found that students who were given a definition of bullying in measurement reported less victimization than students who were not provided a definition. This finding has important implications for establishing accurate prevalence rates of bullying. In the current review, the components most often included in the definitions of bullying that prefaced bullying measures were power imbalance, intention to cause harm, and aggressive behaviors, and only four measures included all five components of bullying as recognized by experts in the field. The exclusion of specific components of bullying behaviors from measures of bullying calls into question the validity of the construct being measured.

The vast majority of publications included in this review did not provide a definition in their measurement strategy. Without a presented definition it remains unclear what the author’s a priori definition of bullying included. The current review provides a thorough examination of how measurement strategies with and without explicit definitions integrate the definitional components of bullying and can be used as a guide for bullying researchers in their plans for measuring bullying-related behaviors. Because bullying has been used as a catch-all phrase to encompass a broad category of behaviors (i.e., physical, verbal, relational), Cornell, Sheras, and Cole (2006) have asked “whether all these forms of bullying are psychologically equivalent.” For example, several definitions include multiple behaviors such as hitting, teasing, and spreading rumors; however, incorporating these behaviors in the same definition may fail to capture the nuances associated with each type of behavior.

The time frames used for reporting also vary drastically based on measurement strategy. In fact, most of the measures included in the review did not provide a specified time range, which presents problems in terms of both comparing bullying rates within the sample of interest and between multiple samples. Among those that did provide a time frame, there were variations in the time frames assessed (e.g., nine used the time frame “past 30 days”, four used “past 7 days/week”, two used “current school year”). Although these variations in reporting periods may seem slight, the differences can result in great disparities in prevalence estimates, particularly depending on the time of year when measures are administered. For example, measures administered in September with the time frame “past 30 days” may have students reporting on behaviors that occurred over the summer; a similar measure administered in February would likely yield different results, given that school is in session in the month of January and therefore the opportunity for bullying perpetration/victimization is theoretically higher. Further, four measurement strategies instructed youths to indicate the frequency with which behaviors occurred with respect to other time frames, such as “recently”. Estimating prevalence rates using such broad terms can be problematic since individuals may interpret the meanings of such terms differently. Overall, the differences in reporting time frames make comparing prevalence rates between samples difficult, if not impossible.

Almost all of the included measures provided Likert-type response options, less than half used binary response options (e.g., yes/no, true/false) or open-ended questions, and two used multiple choice responses to assess bullying behaviors. The variation in response options likely impacts not only overall prevalence rates, but also the kind of information being reported. For instance, responses to open-ended questions may garner more or less detail about bullying behaviors, depending on the extent to which the respondent elaborates. Again, based on the various response options used in different measurement strategies, comparing prevalence rates of bullying overall, or even specific components of bullying behavior, becomes nearly impossible, as there is no clear way to draw parallels between behaviors that occur, for instance, “frequently” as judged by a 5-point Likert-type response option to those that have occurred at least once as judged by a “yes” response to a binary item.

Finally, in terms of scoring the measures that assess bullying behaviors, it is difficult to synthesize results across measures. Most often, measures were summed to yield a total score and then characterized as continuous. Eleven measures created binary categories by either summing across responses and dichotomizing based on “never” versus “ever” or creating binary categories by using a cut-off score. Five used peer nomination strategies by which youths identified peers as victims, bullies, and/or bully/victims. The characterization of bullying in a sample greatly depends on how measures are scored and on how bullies and victims are identified. Thus, scoring techniques are critical in establishing accurate estimates of bullying rates.

In addition to the variability in establishing solid prevalence estimates based on the criteria described earlier, most bullying measurement strategies used in the field lack sufficient psychometric properties, including reliability and validity. These characteristics are fundamental to accurately assessing bullying prevalence. In this review, almost all studies reported moderate to high reliability for their measures, indicating that they consistently yielded similar findings, but most did not assess the validity of the measure, thus leaving the question of whether the surveys accurately measured what they aimed to measure unanswered. Those that did assess validity mostly reported low convergent, discriminant, concurrent, and predictive validity, suggesting that these measures did not clearly assess the intended construct(s). While it is important for measures to be reliable so that researchers can be sure that they are consistently measuring their construct of interest, reliability does not imply validity. It is critical that researchers aiming to assess bullying behaviors are accurately measuring those behaviors, not only in terms readily interpreted by the researchers themselves, but also in terms that recognize the differing perspectives of the youths being surveyed. Thus, in future implementation of measurement strategies, researchers should determine both the reliability and validity of their measures.

4.1. Research limitations

There are several limitations of this review. One particular limitation is that several measures were excluded due to developer non-response to repeated emails for additional information. It is possible that the measures not included based on this factor may be different from the ones included, thus affecting our results. Second, the publications with enough information to describe the measurement strategy tended not to include prevalence data, and therefore we were unable to analyze the data on prevalence by measurement strategy. Next steps should determine how to capture best the relevant prevalence or incidence data to make comparisons across measurement strategies. Lastly, very few measures meeting our inclusion criteria included cyber-bullying items or were dedicated solely to measuring cyber-bullying behavior. A systematic review of cyber-bullying measurement conducted by Berne et al. (2013) included only measures that assessed web-based or electronic bullying behaviors. The authors found that very few cyber-bullying measures stated that their aim was to measure bullying, nor were most measures assessed for reliability and validity. Additional research is needed to better integrate cyber-bullying measures with traditional bullying measurement. Despite these limitations, the results of this study still provide important information about the measures currently being used to assess bullying behaviors, including the measurement strategies employed and the behavioral content assessed by the measures.

4.2. Conclusion

There is much inconsistency in the manner in which bullying is measured by researchers. These inconsistencies range from differences in terminology and temporal referent period to differences in definitional components and actual behaviors measured by the surveys. While these inconsistencies may seem minor, they most likely explain the wide variation in bullying prevalence rates obtained by researchers in the field. Our results further highlight the need for a consistent definition of bullying, which has major implications for the measurement of the construct and the prevention of its occurrence. Future research should focus on integrating a honed definition of bullying into the development of new or improved measurement strategies so that bullying can be more accurately and precisely assessed.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Laura Salazar for contributing feedback on drafts of the manuscript.

1 The term “measurement strategy” is used in this article to encompass the methods used to assess bullying such as reporter type, time frame, and content of behaviors. When the term “measure” is used, we are specifically describing the items and response options that comprise each scale or index.

Financial disclosure

The authors have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

All authors certify that the abovementioned manuscript represents valid work and not been submitted for publication elsewhere. The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

Funding source

  • Atik G. Assessment of school bullying in Turkey: A critical review of self-report instruments. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2011; 15 :3232–3238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.04.277 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Austin S, Joseph S. Assessment of bully/victim problems in 8 to 11 year-olds. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 1996; 66 (4):447–456. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1996.tb01211.x . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berne S, Frisén A, Schultze-Krumbholz A, Scheithauer H, Naruskov K, Luik P, et al. Cyberbullying assessment instruments: A systematic review. Aggression and violent behavior. 2013; 18 :320–334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2012.11.022 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bollen K, Lennox R. Conventional wisdom on measurement: A structural equation perspective. Psychological bulletin. 1991; 110 (2):305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.110.2.305 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bond L, Wolfe S, Tollit M, Butler H, Patton G. A comparison of the Gatehouse bullying scale and the peer relations questionnaire for students in secondary school. Journal of School Health. 2007; 77 (2):75–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1746-1561.2007.00170.x . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bosworth K, Espelage DL, Simon TR. Factors associated with bullying behavior in middle school students. Journal of Early Adolescence. 1999; 19 :341–362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431699019003003 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cerezo F, Ato M. Bullying in Spanish and English pupils: A sociometric perspective using the BULL-S questionnaire. Educational Psychology. 2005; 25 :353–367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410500041458 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cetin B, Yaman E, Peker A. Cyber victim and bullying scale: A study of validity and reliability. Computers & Education. 2011; 57 (4):2261–2271. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chan JHF, Myron RR, Crawshaw CM. The efficacy of non-anonymous measures of bullying. School Psychology International. 2005; 26 :443–458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0143034305059020 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cho J, Hendrickson J, Mock D. Bullying status and behavior patterns of preadolescents and adolescents with behavioral disorders. Education and Treatment of Children. 2009; 32 (4):655–671. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cook CR, Williams KR, Guerra NG, Kim TE. Variability in the prevalance of bullying and victimization: A cross-national and methodological analysis. In: Jimerson SR, Swearer SB, Espelage DL, editors. Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective. New York: NY: Routledge; 2010. pp. 347–362. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Copeland WE, Wolke D, Angold A, Costello EJ. Adult psychiatric outcomes of bullying and being bullied by peers in childhood and adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013 http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.5041-8 . [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ]
  • Cornell DG, Brockenbrough K. Identification of bullies and victims: A comparison of methods. Journal of School Violence. 2004; 3 (2–3):63–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J202v03n02_05 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cornell DG, Sheras PL, Cole JCM. Assessment of bullying. In: Jimerson S, Furlong M, editors. Handbook of school violence and school safety: From research to practice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. pp. 191–210. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crick NR, Dodge KA. Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child development. 1996; 67 (3):993–1002. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131875 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crick NR, Grotpeter JK. Relational aggression, gender, and social psychological adjustment. Child Development. 1995; 66 :710–722. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131945 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DeVoe J, Murphy C. Student reports of bullying and cyber-bullying: Results from the 2009 School Crime Supplement to the National Crime Victimization Survey. National Center for Education Statistics; 2011. p. 52. [ Google Scholar ]
  • DiIorio CK. Measurement in health behavior: Methods for research and evaluation. Vol. 1. San Francisco, CA: Wiley; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D, Perry CL. Peer harassment, school connectedness, and academic achievement. Journal of School Health. 2003; 73 :311–316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1746-1561.2003.tb06588.x . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Espelage DL, Holt MK. Bullying and victimization during early adolescence. Journal of Emotional Abuse. 2001; 2 (2–3):123–142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J135v02n02_08 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Espelage DL, Low S, Polanin JR, Brown EC. The impact of a middle school program to reduce aggression, victimization, and sexual violence. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2013 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.02.021 . [ PubMed ]
  • Espelage DL, Low S, Rao MA, Hong JS, Little TD. Family violence, bullying, fighting, and substance use among adolescents: A longitudinal mediational model. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2013 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jora.12060 .
  • Farmer CA, Aman MG. Development of the children’s scale of hostility and aggression: Reactive/proactive (C-SHARP) Research in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2009; 30 (6):1155–1167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2009.03.001 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Farrington D. Understanding and preventing bullying. In: Tonry M, editor. Crime and justice. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1993. pp. 381–458. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Felix ED, Sharkey JD, Green JG, Furlong MJ, Tanigawa D. Getting precise and pragmatic about the assessment of bullying: the development of the California bullying victimization scale. Aggressive Behavior. 2011; 37 (3):234–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20389 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ferguson CJ, San Miguel C, Kilburn JC, Sanchez P. The effectiveness of school-based anti-bullying programs: A meta-analytic review. Criminal Justice Review. 2007; 32 (4):401–414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0734016807311712 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Finger LF, Yeung AS, Craven RG, Parada RH, Newey K. Adolescent peer relations instrument: Assessment of its reliability and construct validity when use with upper primary students. Paper presented at the Australian Association for Research in Education; Australia: Brisbane; 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fitzpatrick S, Bussey K. The development of the social bullying involvement scales. Aggressive Behavior. 2011 Mar-Apr; 37 (2):177–192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20379 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Furlong MJ, Sharkey JD, Bates MP, Smith DC. An examination of the reliability, data screening procedures, and extreme response patterns for the Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance Survey. Journal of School Violence. 2004; 3 (2–3):109–130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J202v03n02_07 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Furlong MJ, Sharkey JD, Felix ED, Tanigawa D, Green J. Bullying assessment: A call for increased precision of self-reporting procedures. In: Jimerson SR, Swearer SB, Espelage DL, editors. Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective. New York: NY: Routledge; 2010. pp. 329–345. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gable RK, Ludlow LH, McCoach BD, Kite SL. Development and validation of the survey of knowledge of internet risk and internet behavior. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2011; 71 (1):217–230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164410387389 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gini G, Pozzoli T. Association between bullying and psychosomatic problems: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2009; 123 (3):1059–1065. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1215 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gladden RM, Vivolo-Kantor AM, Hamburger ME, Lumpkin C. Bullying surveillance: Uniform definitions and recommended data elements, Version 1.0. Atlanta, GA: 2014. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goossens FA, Olthof T, Dekker PH. New participant role scales: Comparison between various criteria for assigning roles and indications for their validity. Aggressive Behavior. 2006; 32 (4):343–357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20133 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gottheil NF, Dubow EF. The interrelationships of behavioral indices of bully and victim behavior. Journal of Emotional Abuse. 2001a; 2 (2–3):75–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J135v02n02_06 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gottheil NF, Dubow EF. Tripartite beliefs models of bully and victim behavior. Journal of Emotional Abuse. 2001b; 2 (2–3):25–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J135v02n02_03 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greene MB. Bullying and harassment in schools. In: Moser RS, Frantz CE, editors. Shocking violence. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas; 2000. pp. 72–101. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grief JL, Furlong MJ. The assessment of school bullying: Using theory to inform practice. Journal of School Violence. 2006; 5 :33–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J202v05n03_04 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamburger M, Basile K, Vivolo A. Measuring bullying victimization, perpetration, & bystander experiences: A compendium of assessment tools. Atlanta, GA: 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Haynie DL, Nansel T, Eitel P, Crump AD, Saylor K, Yu K, et al. Bullies, victims, and bully/victims: Distinct groups of at-risk youth. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2001; 21 (1):29–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431601021001002 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hinduja S, Patchin JW. Bullying, cyberbullying, and suicide. Archives of Suicide Research. 2010; 14 (3):206–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13811118.2010.494133 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hunt C, Peters L, Rapee RM. Development of a measure of the experience of being bullied in youth. Psychological Assessment. 2012; 24 :156–165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025178 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ireland JL. Bullying behaviors among male and female prisoners: A study of adult and young offenders. Aggressive Behavior. 1999; 25 :161–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1999)25:3<161::AID-AB1>3.0.CO;2-# . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kann L, Kinchen S, Shanklin SL, Flint KH, Hawkins J, et al. Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2013. MMWR Surveillance Summaries. 2014; 63 (SS-04):1–168. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kert AS, Codding RS, Tryon GS, Shiyko M. Impact of the word “bully” on the reported rate of bullying behavior. Psychology in the Schools. 2010; 47 (2):193–204. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Konishi C, Hymel S. Bullying and stress in early adolescence: The role of coping and social support. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2009; 29 :333–356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/02724316083201263 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Land D. Teasing apart secondary students’ conceptualizations of peer teasing, bullying and sexual harassment. School Psychology International. 2003; 24 (2):147–165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0143034303024002002 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977; 33 (1):159–174. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2529310 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leff SS, Power TJ, Goldstein AB. Outcome measures to assess the effectiveness of bullying prevention programs in the schools. In: Espelage DL, Swearer SB, editors. Bullying in American schools: A social–ecological perspective on prevention and intervention. 2004. pp. 269–294. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leff SS, Thomas DE, Shapiro ES, Paskewich B, Wilson K, Necowitz-Hoffman B, et al. Developing and validating a new classroom climate observation assessment tool. Journal of school violence. 2011; 10 (2):165–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15388220.2010.539167 . [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McConville D, Cornell D. Attitudes toward aggression and aggressive behavior among middle school students. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2003; 11 :179–187. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mynard H, Joseph S. Development of the multidimensional peer-victimization scale. Aggressive Behavior. 2000; 26 :169–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(2000)26:2<169::AID-AB3>3.3.CO;2-1 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nakamoto J, Schwartz D. Is peer victimization associated with academic achievement? A meta-analytic review. Social Development. 2009; 19 (2):221–242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00539.x . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nansel TR, Craig W, Overpeck MD, Saluja G, Ruan W. Cross-national consistency in the relationship between bullying behaviors and psychosocial adjustment. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2004; 158 (8):730. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.158.8.730 . [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nishina A, Juvonen J. Daily reports of witnessing and experiencing peer harassment in middle school. Child development. 2005; 76 (2):435–450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00855.x . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Olweus D. Bullying at School: What we know and what we can do. Malden, MA: Blackwell; 1993. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orpinas P, Frankowski R. The aggression scale. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2001; 21 (1):50–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431601021001003 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orpinas P, Horne AM, Staniszewski D. School bullying: Changing the problem by changing the school. School Psychology Review. 2003; 32 :431–444. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perry DG, Kusel SJ, Perry LC. Victims of peer aggression. Developmental Psychology. 1988; 24 :807–814. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.24.6.807 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peters MP, Bain SK. Bullying and victimization rates among gifted and high-achieving students. Journal for the Education of the Gifted. 2011; 34 (4):624–643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/016235321103400405 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Poteat VP, Espelage DL. Exploring the relation between bullying and homophobic verbal content: The homophobic content agent target (HCAT) scale. Violence and Victims. 2005; 20 (5):513–528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/vivi.2005.20.5.513 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Potter WJ, Levine-Donnerstein D. Rethinking validity and reliability in content analysis. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 1999; 27 :258–284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00909889909365539 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Prati G. Development and psychometric properties of the homophobic bullying scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2012; 72 :649–664. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164412440169 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raine A, Dodge K, Loeber R, Gatzke-Kopp L, Lynam D, Reynolds C, et al. The reactive–proactive aggression questionnaire: Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior. 2006; 32 :159–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20115 . [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rigby K. What it takes to stop bullying in schools: An examination of the rationale and effectiveness of school-based interventions. In: Furlong MJ, Bates MP, Smith DC, Kingery P, editors. Appraisal and prediction of school violence. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science; 2004. pp. 165–191. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rigby K, Slee PT. Dimensions of interpersonal relation among Australian children and implications for psychological well-being. The Journal of Social Psychology. 1993; 133 (1):33–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1993.9712116 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rivers I, Poteat VP, Noret N, Ashurst N. Observing bullying at school: The mental health implications of witness status. School Psychology Quarterly. 2009; 24 (4):211–223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018164 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salazar LF, DiClemente RJ, Crosby RA. Measurement and design related to theoretically-based public health research and practice. In: Salazar LF, DiClemente RJ, Crosby RA, editors. Health behavior theory for public health: Principles, foundations, and applications. Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett Publishers; 2011. pp. 255–285. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salmivalli C, Voeten M. Connections between attitudes, group norms, and behaviors associated with bullying in schools. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2004; 28 :246–258. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schäfer M, Korn S, Smith PK, Hunter SC, Mora-Merchán JA, Singer MM, et al. Lonely in the crowd: Recollections of bullying. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2004; 22 :379–394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/0261510041552756 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith PK, Cowie H, Olafsson RF, Liefooghe A. Definitions of bullying: A comparison of terms used, and age and gender differences, in a fourteen-country international comparison. Child development. 2002; 73 (4):1119–1133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00461 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith PK, del barrio C, Tokunaga RS. Definitions of bullying and cyberbullying: How useful are the terms? In: Bauman S, Cross D, Walker J, editors. Principles of cyberbullying research: Definitions, measures and methodology. New York: NY: Routledge; 2013. pp. 26–40. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith PK, Thompson D. Practical approaches to bullying. London, UK: David Fulton Publishers; 1991. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Solberg ME, Olweus D. Prevalence estimation of school bullying with the Olweus bully/victim questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior. 2003; 29 (3):239–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.10047 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spector PE. Summated rating scale construction: An introduction. Sage; 1992. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swearer SM, Cary PT. Perceptions and attitudes toward bullying in middle school youth. Journal of Applied School Psychology. 2003; 19 (2):63–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J008v19n02_05 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swearer SM, Siebecker AB, Johnsen-Frerichs LA, Wang C. Assessment of bullying/victimization: The problem of comparability across studies and across methodologies. In: Jimerson SR, Swearer SB, Espelage DL, editors. Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective. New York: NY: Routledge; 2010. pp. 305–327. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tarshis TP, Huffman LC. Psychometric properties of the peer interactions in primary school (PIPS) questionnaire. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2007; 28 (2):125–132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.DBP.0000267562.11329.8f . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taub J. Evaluation of the second step violence prevention program at a rural elementary school. School Psychology Review. 2002; 31 (2):186–200. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thompson JK, Fabian LJ, Moulton DO, Dunn ME, Altabe MN. Development and validation of the physical appearance related teasing scale. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1991; 56 (3):513–521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5603_12 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thompson JK, Cattarin J, Fowler B, Fisher E. The perception of teasing scale (POTS): A revision and extension of the physical appearance related teasing scale (PARTS) Journal of Personality Assessment. 1995; 65 :146–157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa6501_11 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tokunaga RS. Following you home from school: A critical review and synthesis of research on cyberbullying and victimization. Computers in Human Behavior. 2010; 26 :277–287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2009.11.014 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vaillancourt T, McDougall P, Hymel S, Krygsman A, Miller J, Stiver K, et al. Bullying: Are researchers and children/youth talking about the same thing? International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2008; 32 (6):486–495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025408095553 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Schoiack-Edstrom L, Frey KS, Beland K. Changing adolescents’ attitudes about relational and physical aggression: An early evaluation of a school-based intervention. School Psychology Review. 2002; 31 (2):201–216. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Veenstra R, Lindenberg S, Oldehinkel AJ, De Winter AF, Verhulst FC, Ormel J. Bullying and victimization in elementary schools: a comparison of bullies, victims, bully/victims, and uninvolved preadolescents. Developmental psychology. 2005; 41 (4):672. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.672 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vernberg EM, Jacobs AK, Hershberger SL. Peer victimization and attitudes about violence during early adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1999; 28 (3):386–395. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vivolo A, Holt M, Massetti G. Individual and contextual factors for bullying and peer victimization: Implications for prevention. Journal of School Violence. 2011; 10 :201–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15388220.2010.539169 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Warden D, Cheyne B, Christie D, Fitzpatrick H, Reid K. Assessing children’s perceptions of prosocial and antisocial peer behaviour. Educational Psychology. 2003; 23 (5):547–567. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0144341032000123796 . [ Google Scholar ]

Home — Essay Samples — Social Issues — Bullying — Bullying In Schools: Causes, Effects, And Solutions

test_template

Bullying in Schools: Causes, Effects, and Solutions

  • Categories: Bullying Youth Violence

About this sample

close

Words: 1534 |

Published: Dec 16, 2021

Words: 1534 | Pages: 3 | 8 min read

Works Cited

  • Bradshaw, C. P., Sawyer, A. L., & O'Brennan, L. M. (2007). Bullying and peer victimization at school: Perceptual differences between students and school staff. School Psychology Review, 36(3), 361-382.
  • Espelage, D. L., & Swearer, S. M. (2003). Research on school bullying and victimization: What have we learned and where do we go from here?. School Psychology Review, 32(3), 365-383.
  • Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. W. (2018). Cyberbullying fact sheet: Identification, prevention, and response. Cyberbullying Research Center.
  • National Bullying Prevention Center. (2021). Resources. https://www.pacer.org/bullying/resources/
  • National Center for Education Statistics. (2022). Student reports of bullying and cyberbullying: Results from the 2020–21 School Crime Supplement to the National Crime Victimization Survey. US Department of Education.
  • Olweus, D. (2013). School bullying: Development and some important challenges. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 751-780.
  • Patchin, J. W., & Hinduja, S. (2020). School climate 2.0: Preventing cyberbullying and sexting one classroom at a time. Corwin Press.
  • StopBullying.gov. (2021). Prevent bullying. https://www.stopbullying.gov/prevention/index.html
  • Thompson, F., Smith, P. K., & Rigby, K. (2022). Addressing bullying in schools: Theory and practice. Routledge.
  • Ttofi, M. M., & Farrington, D. P. (2011). Effectiveness of school-based programs to reduce bullying: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 7(1), 27-56.

Image of Dr. Oliver Johnson

Cite this Essay

Let us write you an essay from scratch

  • 450+ experts on 30 subjects ready to help
  • Custom essay delivered in as few as 3 hours

Get high-quality help

author

Dr. Heisenberg

Verified writer

  • Expert in: Social Issues

writer

+ 120 experts online

By clicking “Check Writers’ Offers”, you agree to our terms of service and privacy policy . We’ll occasionally send you promo and account related email

No need to pay just yet!

Related Essays

2 pages / 895 words

5 pages / 2604 words

2 pages / 998 words

2 pages / 797 words

Remember! This is just a sample.

You can get your custom paper by one of our expert writers.

121 writers online

Bullying in Schools: Causes, Effects, and Solutions Essay

Still can’t find what you need?

Browse our vast selection of original essay samples, each expertly formatted and styled

Related Essays on School Bullying

Kidscape. (n.d.). About Kidscape. Retrieved from https://www.childline.org.uk/info-advice/bullying-abuse-safety/types-bullying/

Introduction to the issue of animal testing in the cosmetic industry The ethical concerns surrounding animal testing Arguments in favor of animal testing, including potential medical advancements [...]

In all fairness, and in all matters of life, including medicine and health science, the state of the human race is foremost, before any other considerations are made. This is why there is the use of non-human animal testing in [...]

Yesterday, I was scrolling through my Facebook feed and saw a picture of this week's newspaper dated for October 25, 2016, on the shooting at Union Middle School in Sandy, Utah. The article reported that a fourteen year old [...]

That is a true story that came about in Japan in 1988. A 17-year vintage female named Furuta Junko was kidnapped after which tortured with the aid of four boys with a very inconceivable manner and in the end demise after [...]

The case below is of a judge (Patricia Doninger) who allowed her court marshals to degrade and sexually abuse a lady who was attending court hearings about her divorce case. After an investigation on the August 12 incident where [...]

Related Topics

By clicking “Send”, you agree to our Terms of service and Privacy statement . We will occasionally send you account related emails.

Where do you want us to send this sample?

By clicking “Continue”, you agree to our terms of service and privacy policy.

Be careful. This essay is not unique

This essay was donated by a student and is likely to have been used and submitted before

Download this Sample

Free samples may contain mistakes and not unique parts

Sorry, we could not paraphrase this essay. Our professional writers can rewrite it and get you a unique paper.

Please check your inbox.

We can write you a custom essay that will follow your exact instructions and meet the deadlines. Let's fix your grades together!

Get Your Personalized Essay in 3 Hours or Less!

We use cookies to personalyze your web-site experience. By continuing we’ll assume you board with our cookie policy .

  • Instructions Followed To The Letter
  • Deadlines Met At Every Stage
  • Unique And Plagiarism Free

disadvantages of bullying essay

More From Forbes

Anti-bullying programs in schools may do more harm than good.

  • Share to Facebook
  • Share to Twitter
  • Share to Linkedin

Bullying can exact a huge emotional and mental toll on the victims (Pic: Getty Creative)

Anti-bullying programs in schools may be doing more harm than good.

Programs that enlist fellow students to intervene or help support the victim can lead to an increase in bullying and make it worse for the victims, according to a new study.

Peer involvement is a popular feature of anti-bullying programs at many schools, prompted by the belief that they can prove more effective than teacher-led interventions alone.

This has seen many schools train students as playground buddies, given the job of trying to solve problems between children without the need for adult involvement.

Previous research has shown that the actions of bystanders, such as laughing or joining in, can intensify bullying, while the majority of playground incidents have been found to stop within 10 seconds of a peer intervention.

But there is little evidence that it was the peer interventions themselves that caused the bullying to stop, and no indication that training peers to intervene would have the same effect as spontaneous interventions based on feelings of empathy and injustice, according to the researcher behind the new study.

Instead, she identified that training peers to get involved in a bullying incident could increase both the severity of victimization and the level of distress among victims.

Victims can feel disempowered if peers intervene to protect them, the presence of trained anti-bullying students can actually reinforce or provoke bullying, and interventions can erode wider peer support for the victim, the study found.

Best High-Yield Savings Accounts Of 2024

Best 5% interest savings accounts of 2024.

‘Many school bullying prevention programs encourage and train peer bystanders (helpers) to get actively involved in assisting with possible instances of bullying,’ said Karyn L. Healy, research officer from QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute and the University of Queensland, Brisbane, author of the study.

‘Although this approach is very common and well-intentioned, there is no evidence that it helps victims. Encouraging peers to actively defend victims of bullying may actually produce adverse outcomes for victims.’

Involving other students can make an incident more public, which can damage the victim’s social status, while if peers step in then it stops the victim from dealing with it themselves, making them look weak in the eyes of the bully, Healy said.

Students trained in intervention strategies may feel it gives them higher status or may enjoy the feeling of belonging, potentially leading them to misuse their power and intervene where it was not necessary, she added.

The apparent success of anti-bullying programs involving peers may be due to other elements of the strategies, such as staff training and interventions, parent newsletters or lessons incorporating anti-bullying messages, according to the study, published today in Child Development Perspectives, the journal of the Society for Research in Child Development.

Evaluation of anti-bullying programs can also often be based entirely on whether bullying incidents have fallen, and does not always take into account the overall impact on the victim, Healy said.

Instead, schools should focus on strategies that examine the outcomes for victims, as well as on the bullying incidents themselves.

Approaches are likely to be more effective if they support victims to stand up for themselves, are motivated by genuine empathy, rather than a desire to help, and do not provoke bullies or increase the visibility of victimization, Healy added.

Nick Morrison

  • Editorial Standards
  • Reprints & Permissions
  • Student Resources
  • Teacher Resources

Anti-Bullying Programs: Do they Help or Hurt?

Lucy Lawrence

EL Haynes PCS

Share Post:

We all know those signs: the ones about reporting bullying and speaking out-the signs in every school hallway. Those signs that, like most kids, you never give a second glance. Do those signs and other more assertive anti-bullying programs actually help reduce bullying? According to research, they don’t have much of an impact at all. In fact, bullying and anti-bullying efforts have almost synonymous results because both have negative effects on people’s futures.

As bullied children grow older, their social and emotional lives tend to be less content than people who were not bullied. Bullied children tend to become less mentally stable when they get older, compared to their non-bullied counterparts. This could mean that kids who were bullied end up less happy when they get older, or it could even mean that being bullied as a child could increase the chance of someone getting mentally sick when they’re older. A study of roughly 18,000 kids in Europe tracked how bullying, or the lack thereof, affected them later in life. “The researchers found that people who were bullied either occasionally or frequently continued to suffer higher levels of psychological distress decades after the bullying occurred.”(Kaplan, paragraph 6). This evidence shows that anyone who is bullied, whether it’s often or not, ends up suffering from it years later. As a result, bullying affects people’s brains and makes them less psychologically healthy in the future.

Bullying also causes people to become more anti-social. “Adults who were bullied as kids were more socially isolated too. At age 50, bullying victims are less likely to be living with a spouse or a partner; less likely to have spent time with friends recently; less likely to have friends or family to lean on if they got sick.” (Kaplan, paragraph 8). This shows that people who are bullied end up less mentally stable, that they don’t have many friends, and that they isolate themselves. People who were bullied, overall, end up with a much sadder life than those who weren’t bullied. It’s important that we recognize the significance of bullying and the effect it has on the victim’s mental health, even if the amount of bullying is small, it will still change someone’s life, be it in a small way or a life altering way.

Anti-bullying programs are not as distinguished and effective as they could be, and sometimes have the opposite effect they were intending. In fact, anti-bullying programs can lead to children getting bullied more often. For example, in an article from USA Today by Amanda Oglesby, it talks about how teens experience bullying in schools with anti-bullying programs compared to those in schools without programs. “In the study of 7,000 students ages 12 to 18 who completed a survey in the 2005-06 school year, researchers found that a higher percentage of students who attended schools with anti-bullying programs had reported experiencing bullying than in schools without programs.” (Oglesby, paragraph 4). The fact that more students are bullied in those schools with the program is proof that anti-bullying programs don’t work, according to those who know best: the students.

In the same article, the author elaborates on why the anti-bullying programs don’t work. “…bullies may simply choose not to practice prevention techniques, or perhaps, learned more effective bullying techniques through the programs.” (Oglesby, paragraph 6). This shows that anti-bullying programs aren’t only defective due to the fact they don’t do much to prevent bullies, but also because they are helping bullies become better at being bullies. So even if anti-bullying programs were intended to be helpful, they end up hurting more than doing good.

Some believe that anti-bullying programs are at least making kids more aware of bullying, and that helps to slow bullies from harming other students, but that doesn’t necessarily mean it’s true. In one article, the author makes the claim that children in communities with the anti-bullying programs are more likely to report bullying than children without that resource. “Students who are aware of bullying because of programs report it more often than children who know little about it.” (Oglesby, paragraph 6). Children reporting bullying is a start, but all that’s saying is that students are reporting bullying. It isn’t saying anything about how adults then react to said report. Adults and administrators could just ignore these kids who are reporting all this bullying. The people who have the power could not be doing anything, which could be a major factor in the statistics. The adults who have the power to change the situation are not doing everything necessary to solve the problem.

Another recent study shows that the results of anti-bullying programs show little progress. “…investigations into harassment, intimidation or bullying happened in New Jersey schools in the 2012-2013 school year…that number decreased by nearly 5,000 from the prior year- a decrease of 19%…” (Oglesby, paragraph 9). Even though the number of bullying investigations has gone down by 5,000, or 19%, is 19% really a significant difference, considering the time and money put into these anti-bullying programs? For these reasons, anti-bullying programs may help kids report more bullying, but it doesn’t really help with how administrators and teachers respond to said bullying. And, the actual results show that they don’t help as much as was intended.

Bullying is a huge problem, and it changes children’s lives forever, but the methods that are being used to combat bullying today aren’t working. In some cases, these anti-bullying programs are even making bullying worse. At some schools, the teachers monitor kids more carefully, checking in and watching in the hallways. This may be a very invasive option, but it really helps more than any poster or bullying awareness assembly. We need to find an anti-bullying program that works, holding both the students and teachers accountable of making the school a better place.

Works Cited

Kaplan, Karen, “Victims of Bullying Live With the Consequences for decades, study says,” LA Times, April 2014.

Oglesby, Amanda, “Researchers Unsure of Success of Anti-Bullying Programs,” Asbury Park USA Today 20 Mar. 2014, News sec. Web

Lucy Lawrence 1

Written By:

Jesse Marczyk Ph.D.

Benefits to Bullying

Just because it's mean doesn't make it maladaptive..

Posted November 29, 2015 | Reviewed by Ekua Hagan

  • How to Handle Bullying
  • Take our Anger Management Test
  • Find a therapist to support kids or teens

Diego Grez/Wikimedia

When it comes to assessing hypotheses of evolutionary function, there is a troublesome pair of intuitions which frequently trip many people up.

The first of these is commonly called the naturalistic fallacy, though it also goes by the name of an appeal to nature: the idea that because something is natural, it ought to be good. As a typical argument using this line might go, because having sex is natural, we ought to — morally and socially — approve of it. The corresponding intuition to this is known as the moralistic fallacy: If something is wrong, then it’s not natural (or, alternatively, if something is good, it is natural ). An argument using this type of reasoning might (and has, more or less) gone, because rape is morally wrong, it cannot be a natural behavior. In both cases, "natural" is a bit of a wiggle word but, in general, it seems to refer to whether or not a species possesses some biological tendency to engage in the behavior in question.

Put another way, "natural" refers to whether a species possesses an adaptation(s) that functions so as to bring about a particular outcome. Extending these examples a little further, we might come up with the arguments that, because humans possess cognitive mechanisms which motivate sexual behavior, sex must be a moral good; however, because rape is a moral wrong, the human must not contain any adaptations that were selected for because they promoted such behavior.

This type of thinking is, of course, fallacious, as per the namesakes of the two fallacies. It’s quite easy to think of many moral wrong which might increase one’s reproductive fitness (and thus select for adaptations that produce them), just as it is easy to think of morally-virtuous behaviors that could lower one’s fitness: infanticide is certainly among the things people would consider morally wrong, and yet there is often an adaptive logic to be found in the behavior; conversely, while the ideal of universal altruism is praised by many as morally virtuous, altruistic behavior is often limited to contexts in which it will later be reciprocated or channeled towards close kin. As such, it’s probably for the best to avoid tethering one’s system of moral approval to natural-ness, or vice versa; you end up in some weird places philosophically if you do.

Now, this type of thinking is not limited to any particular group of people: Scientists and laypeople alike can make use of these naturalistic and moralistic intuitions (intentionally or not), leading to cases where hypotheses of function are violently rejected for even considering that certain condemned behaviors might be the result of an adaptation for generating them, or other cases where weak adaptive arguments are made in the service of making other behaviors with which the arguer approves seem more natural and, accordingly, more morally acceptable.

With that in mind, we can turn to the matter of bullying : aggression enacted by more powerful individuals against weaker ones, typically peaking in frequency during adolescence . Bullying is a candidate behavior that might fall prey to the former fallacies because, well, it tends to generate many consequences people find unpleasant: having their lunch money taken, being hit, being verbally mocked, having slanderous rumors about them being spread, or other such nastiness. As bullying generates such proximately negative consequences for its victims, I suspect that many people would balk at the prospect that bullying might reflect a class of natural, adaptive behaviors, resulting in the bully gaining greater access to resources and reputation; in other words, doing evolutionarily useful things. Now that’s not to say that if you were to start bullying people you would suddenly find your lot in life improving, largely because bullying others tends to carry consequences; many people will not sit idly by and suffer the costs of your bullying; they will defend themselves. In order for bullying to be effective, then, the bully needs to possess certain traits that minimize, withstand, or remove the consequences of this retaliation, such as a greater physical formidability than their victim, a stronger social circle willing to protect them, or other means of backing up their aggression.

Accordingly, only those in certain conditions and possessing particular traits are capable of effectively bullying others (inflicting costs without suffering them in turn). Provided that is the case, those who engaged in bullying behaviors more often might be expected to achieve correspondingly greater reproductive success, as the same traits that make bullying an effective strategy also make the bully an attractive mating prospect. It’s probably worse to select a mate unable to defend themselves from aggression, relative to one able and willing to do so; not only would your mate (and perhaps you) be exploited more regularly, but such traits may well be passed onto your children in turn, leaving them open for exploitation as well. Conversely, the bully able to exploit others can likely can access to more plentiful resources, protect you from exploitation, and pass such useful traits along to their children. That bullying might have an adaptive basis was the hypothesis examined in a recent paper by Volk et al (2015). As noted in their introduction, previous data on the subject is consistent with the possibility that bullies are actually in relatively better condition than their victims, with bullies displaying comparable or better mental and physical health, as well as improved social and leadership skills, setting the stage for the prospect of greater mating success (as all of those traits are valuable in the mating arena). Findings like those run counter to some others suggestions floating around the wider culture that people bully others precisely because they lack social skills, intelligence , or are unhappy with themselves. While I understand that no one is particularly keen to paint a flattering picture of people they don’t like and their motives for engaging in behavior they seek to condemn, it’s important to not lose sight of reality while you try reduce the behavior and condemn its perpetrators.

Volk et al (2015) examined the mating success of bullies by correlating people’s self-reports of their bullying behavior with their reports of dating and sexual behavior across two samples: 334 younger adolescents (11-18 years old) and 143 college freshman, all drawn from Canada. Both groups answered questions concerning how often they engaged in, and were a victim of, bullying behaviors, whether they have had sex and, if they had, how many partners they’ve had, whether they have dated and, if so, how many people they’ve dated, as well as how likable and attractive they found themselves to be. Self-reports are obviously not the ideal measures of such things, but at times they can be the best available option.

Focusing on the bullying results, Volk et al (2015) reported a positive relationship between bullying and engaging in dating and sexual relationships in both samples: controlling for age, sex, reported victimization, attractiveness , and likability, bullying not only emerged a positive predictor as to whether the adolescent had dated or had sex at all (about 1.3 to 2 times more likely), but also correlated with the number of sexual and, sometimes, dating partners; those who bullied people more frequently tended to have a greater number of sexual partners, though this effect was modest (bs ranging from 0.2 to 0.26). By contrast, being a victim of bullying did not consistently or appreciably affect the number of sexual partners one had (while victimization was positively correlated with participant’s number of dating partners, it was not correlated with their number of sexual partners. This might reflect the possibility that those who seek to date frequently might be viewed as competitors by other same-sex individuals and bullied in order to prevent such behavior from taking place, though that much is only speculation).

While this data is by no means conclusive, it does present the possibility that bullying is not indicative of someone who is poor shape physically, mentally, or socially; quite the opposite, in fact. Indeed, that is probably why bullying often appears to be so one-sided: those being victimized are not doing more to fight back because they are aware of how well that would turn out for them. Understanding this relationship between bullying and sexual success might prove rather important for anyone looking to reduce the prevalence of bullying. After all, if bullying is providing access to desirable social resources – including sexual partners – it will be hard to shift the cost/benefit analysis away from bullying being the more attractive option barring some introduction of more attractive alternatives for achieving that goal. If, for instance, bullying serves a cue that potential mates might use for assessing underlying characteristics that make the bully more attractive to others, finding new, less harmful ways of signaling those traits (and getting bullies to use those instead) could represent a viable anti-bully technique.

disadvantages of bullying essay

As these relationships are merely correlational, however, there are other ways of interpreting them. It could be possible, for example, that the relationship between bullying and sexual success is accounted for by those who bully being more coercive towards their sexual partners as well as their victims, achieving a greater number of sexual partners, but not in the healthiest fashion. This interpretation would be somewhat complicated by the lack of sex differences between men and women in the current data, however, as it seems unlikely that women who bully are also more likely to coerce their male partners into sex they don’t really want. The only sex difference reported involved the relationship between bullying and dating, with the older sample of women who bullied people more often having a greater number of dating relationships (r = 0.5), relative to men (r = 0.13), as well as a difference in the younger sample with respect to desire for dating relationships (female r = 0.28, male r = 0.03). It is possible, then, that men and women might bully others, at least at times, to obtain different goals , which ought to be expected when the interests of each sex diverge. Understanding those adaptive goals should prove key for effectively reducing bullying; at least I feel that understanding would be more profitable than positing that bullies are mean because they wish to make others as miserable as they are, crave attention , or other such implausible evolutionary functions.

Volk, A., Dane, A., Marini, Z., & Vaillancourt, T., (2015). Adolescent bullying, dating, and mating: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Evolutionary Psychology , DOI: 10.1177/1474704915613909

Jesse Marczyk Ph.D.

Jesse Marczyk, Ph.D. , studies evolutionary psychology and writes the blog Pop Psychology.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Self Tests NEW
  • Therapy Center
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

July 2024 magazine cover

Sticking up for yourself is no easy task. But there are concrete skills you can use to hone your assertiveness and advocate for yourself.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

Try AI-powered search

  • Why people have fallen out of love with dating apps

Tinder and Bumble are struggling as singles refuse to pay up

The illustration features a large, red broken heart on a dark red background. Scattered between the two halves of the broken heart are icons of various dating apps, such as Tinder, Bumble, OKCupid, Hinge, Plenty of Fish, and others.

Your browser does not support the <audio> element.

W hen Tinder , a mobile dating app, launched on college campuses in America in 2012, it quickly became a hit. Although online dating had been around since Match.com, a website for lonely hearts, launched in 1995, it had long struggled to shed an image of desperation. But Tinder, by letting users sift through photos of countless potential dates with a simple swipe, made it easy and fun.

Soon Tinder and its rivals had transformed courtship. A report published last year by the Pew Research Centre found that 30% of American adults had used an online dating service, including more than half of those aged between 18 and 29. One in five couples of that age had met through such a service. Usage surged during the pandemic, as lonely locked-down singles sought out partners. The market capitalisation of Bumble, a rival to Tinder, surged to $13bn on its first day of trading in February 2021. Later that year the value of Match Group, which owns Tinder, Hinge and scores of other dating services, reached nearly $50bn. Today roughly 350m people around the world have a dating app on their phone, up from 250m in 2018, according to Business of Apps, a research firm. In June Tokyo’s government even said it would launch a matchmaking app of its own to pair up singles in the city.

disadvantages of bullying essay

Yet lately online dating has lost its spark. The apps were downloaded 237m times globally last year, down from 287m in 2020. According to Sensor Tower, another research firm, the number of people who use them at least once a month has dwindled from 154m in 2021 to 137m in the second quarter of this year (see chart 1). On August 7th Bumble reported revenue growth of just 3%, year on year, in the quarter from April to June, and lowered its forecast for the full year to 1-2%. Its shares plunged by a third in after-hours trading. On July 30th Match Group reported that its revenue for the same quarter grew by only 4%. Both companies’ market values have cratered since Bumble’s listing (see chart 2). That reflects users’ increasing disillusionment with dating apps, decreasing willingness to pay for them—and growing interest in offline alternatives.

Start with the disillusionment. Apps that once felt fun have, for many, become wellsprings of frustration. The network effects that initially propelled services such as Tinder, in which a widening choice of partners lured in ever more users, have now made them exasperating. Users grumble about spending hours sorting through tens of thousands of profiles. Half of women surveyed by Pew said they felt overwhelmed by the number of messages they received. It doesn’t help that 84% of Tinder users are men. So are 61% of those on Bumble, which is targeted at women. Many users also fret about scams.

disadvantages of bullying essay

Younger adults are growing especially weary of the apps. One survey commissioned last year by Axios, a news site, found that only a fifth of American college students were using them at least once a month. “It’s not fun, it’s so superficial and it’s also just like really exhausting,” laments one youthful influencer on TikTok, a short-video app. “I’m kind of over it,” sums up Wunmi Williams, a 27-year-old who, after years of swiping and matching, has been unable to find a partner through a dating app. In a sign of growing despair, the Marriage Pact, an annual event in which participants are matched with a “backup” spouse should their future romantic endeavours fail, has spread to 88 college campuses across America.

All this helps explain why dating-app developers are struggling to convince users to part with cash—the second reason for their lacklustre performance. In an effort to boost margins, dating apps have been peddling paid upgrades to supplement their lowly ad revenues. Hinge has a separate feed with popular profiles it thinks you might like, but demands that you hand over $3.99 for a “rose” before you can chat with them. Tinder’s paid plans range from $17.99 a month (which gives you unlimited swipes and lets you change your location) to a hefty $499 a month (which lets you see the most popular profiles on the app and message users you haven’t matched with).

Got the ick

Online dating may no longer look desperate, but users seem to worry that paying for it might. The share of people who are willing to spend money on dating apps has been falling. Tinder’s paid users have declined for seven consecutive quarters. Men are more likely to cough up, which may be worsening the feeling common among women of being bombarded by messages on the apps.

Perhaps the biggest threat to the future of dating apps, though, is the growing share of singles looking offline for love. Last year some began wearing an aqua-coloured ring, made by a startup called Pear, to show their openness to being wooed. Thursday, a company that organises in-person events for singles, has expanded its service to roughly 30 cities, from Stockholm to Sydney. Its app works only on Thursday, when the events are held.

The romance is not confined to bars. Running clubs have become a place for athletic types to meet. Cooking classes, too, have become a place to look for partners, says Julia Hartz, the boss of Eventbrite, a ticketing platform. Attendance at its singles events rose 42% between 2022 and 2023. “You are bonding with someone, you’re having an experience, even if they’re not the love of your life,” says Casey Lewis, a blogger on youth culture, of such events.

Dating apps are looking for ways to lure users back. Some are hoping to spice things up with artificial intelligence ( AI ). Whitney Wolfe Herd, Bumble’s founder, recently mused that the future of courtship could involve one person’s AI bot going on “dates” with another’s. One new app, Volar, has begun offering just that.

In time, society might be willing to leave matchmaking to machines—but it is hard to imagine the strategy paying off just yet. A more fruitful approach for dating apps may instead be to focus on narrower markets. Grindr, an app for gay men, continues to grow quickly. So does Feeld, which targets the polyamorous. In the past few years Match Group has launched apps targeted at gay men (Archer), single parents (Stir), ethnic minorities ( BLK , Chispa) and snobs (The League). Revenue from this portfolio of brands grew by 17%, year on year, in the second quarter of 2024.

In addition to offering a smaller pool of partners, such apps also serve as a community for like-minded people. Grindr, for example, acts as a travel guide for tourists looking for gay bars and a hub for information on HIV . The company says its average user sends 50 messages a day, about the same as for WhatsApp, a messaging service. Its success in that regard might explain why Lidiane Jones, the chief executive of Bumble, has said she wants her firm to be known as a “connections company, rather than a dating company”. Pulling off such a rebrand may prove tricky. But love has never been an easy business. ■

To stay on top of the biggest stories in business and technology, sign up to the Bottom Line , our weekly subscriber-only newsletter.

This article appeared in the Business section of the print edition under the headline “Swiped out”

Business August 10th 2024

China’s manufacturers are going broke, china is overhauling its company law, what can olympians teach executives.

  • A court says Google is a monopolist. Now what?
  • A history-lover’s guide to the market panic over AI

How to respond

From the August 10th 2024 edition

Discover stories from this section and more in the list of contents

More from Business

disadvantages of bullying essay

Can Chipotle’s boss turn Starbucks around?

Brian Niccol faces three big challenges

disadvantages of bullying essay

How bosses should play politics: the cautionary tale of Elon Musk

A new book considers the complex relationship between presidents and company bosses

disadvantages of bullying essay

Patriotism is replacing purpose in American business

Beware the costs

Citius, altius, spurious

Its leaders want to make business less volatile—and easier to control

Overcapacity is leading to soaring bankruptcies

IMAGES

  1. Long-Term Consequences of Bullying on Victims and Bullies Free Essay

    disadvantages of bullying essay

  2. Bullying Essay: Popular Topics and Useful Samples

    disadvantages of bullying essay

  3. Stop Bullying Essay

    disadvantages of bullying essay

  4. Essay on Bullying

    disadvantages of bullying essay

  5. ≫ Types, Causes, and Effects of Bullying Free Essay Sample on Samploon.com

    disadvantages of bullying essay

  6. ≫ Bullying in Middle School Free Essay Sample on Samploon.com

    disadvantages of bullying essay

COMMENTS

  1. Consequences of Bullying in Schools: [Essay Example], 523 words

    Mental Health Impact. One of the primary consequences of bullying in schools is its impact on the mental health of the victims. Studies have shown that victims of bullying are more likely to experience depression, anxiety, and other mental health issues than their peers who have not been bullied. The constant fear and stress caused by bullying ...

  2. Top 5 Negative Effects of Bullying

    We could prevent unnecessary deaths by teaching children the importance of respect and acceptance. Bullying victims are twice as likely to bring a weapon to school. 1. Depression and Suicide. One of the worst-case outcomes from bullying is, of course, depression and suicide.

  3. Cyberbullying: Examples, Negative Effects, How to Stop It

    Walk away: Walking away online involves ignoring the bullies, stepping back from your computer or phone, and finding something you enjoy doing to distract yourself from the bullying.; Don't retaliate: You may want to defend yourself at the time.But engaging with the bullies can make matters worse. Keep evidence: Save all copies of the cyberbullying, whether it be posts, texts, or emails, and ...

  4. Preventing Bullying: Consequences, Prevention, and Intervention

    Bullying is considered to be a significant public health problem with both short- and long-term physical and social-emotional consequences for youth. A large body of research indicates that youth who have been bullied are at increased risk of subsequent mental, emotional, health, and behavioral problems, especially internalizing problems, such as low self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and ...

  5. Effects of Bullying

    Kids who are bullied are more likely to experience: Depression and anxiety, increased feelings of sadness and loneliness, changes in sleep and eating patterns, and loss of interest in activities they used to enjoy. These issues may persist into adulthood. Health complaints. Decreased academic achievement—GPA and standardized test scores—and ...

  6. Consequences of Bullying

    Psychological Effects. The psychological effects of bullying include depression, anxiety, low self-esteem, self-harming behavior (especially for girls), alcohol and drug use and dependence, aggression, and involvement in violence or crime (especially for boys). While bullying can lead to mental health problems for any child, those who already ...

  7. Consequences of Bullying Behavior

    Bullying behavior is a serious problem among school-age children and adolescents; it has short- and long-term effects on the individual who is bullied, the individual who bullies, the individual who is bullied and bullies others, and the bystander present during the bullying event. In this chapter, the committee presents the consequences of bullying behavior for children and youth. As ...

  8. Understanding Bullying: Causes, Effects, and Solutions

    To combat bullying effectively, a multi-pronged approach is necessary, involving individuals, families, schools, and communities. Here are some key solutions: Educational Programs: Schools should implement comprehensive anti-bullying programs that educate students about the consequences of bullying and promote empathy and tolerance.

  9. Defining school bullying and its implications on education ...

    School violence, including bullying, is widespread: one in three learners is bullied at school every month globally. The growing use of digital devices has exacerbated cyberbullying. In 2019, at least 10% of learners aged 8-10 had experienced cyberbullying, rising to 20% of learners aged 12-14. School violence can leave long-lasting impacts on learners' safety, physical and mental health ...

  10. Bullying in children: impact on child health

    Bullying in childhood is a global public health problem that impacts on child, adolescent and adult health. Bullying exists in its traditional, sexual and cyber forms, all of which impact on the physical, mental and social health of victims, bullies and bully-victims. Children perceived as 'different' in any way are at greater risk of ...

  11. Bullying Essay for Students and Children

    500+ Words Essay on Bullying. Bullying refers to aggressive behavior so as to dominate the other person. It refers to the coercion of power over others so that one individual can dominate others. It is an act that is not one time, instead, it keeps on repeating over frequent intervals. The person (s) who bullies others can be termed as bullies ...

  12. Pros and Cons of Cyber Bullying

    5. Larger Audience. Cyber bullying is very much public, and because it is published it has the ability to reach a very broad audience. Furthering the humiliation of the person being harassed. Filed Under: Psychological Articles and Infographics. All of the pros and cons to consider about the new form of bullying, cyber.

  13. Bullying: Types, Effects and Government Initiatives

    The main types of bullying within school, especially within secondary schools, these are physical school bullying, emotional/verbal school bullying, electronic bullying or Cyber - bullying and sexual/homophobic bullying. Physical bullying is when an individual bully or a group of bullies physical harm their victim, examples of this type of ...

  14. Bullying: issues and challenges in prevention and intervention

    Bullying is a public health issue that persists and occurs across several contexts. In this narrative review, we highlight issues and challenges in addressing bullying prevention. Specifically, we discuss issues related to defining, measuring, and screening for bullying. These include discrepancies in the interpretation and measurement of power imbalance, repetition of behavior, and ...

  15. School Bullies: Unmasking the Causes, Effects, and Solutions: [Essay

    The Effects of School Bullying. School bullying has far-reaching and detrimental effects on the lives of those involved. These effects extend beyond the school environment and can impact the mental, emotional, and physical well-being of individuals: Psychological Impact: Victims of bullying often experience anxiety, depression, low self-esteem ...

  16. Disadvantages Of Bullying

    The disadvantages of bullying in the twenty-first century are that many types of bullying exist, which include cyber, physical, verbal and emotional bullying. The reckless acts leave a large amount of long-term damage towards too many victims. "Every year at least 3.2 million children are bullied in any type of form" (11 Facts About ...

  17. A systematic review and content analysis of bullying and cyber-bullying

    1. Introduction. Bullying is a form of interpersonal violence that can cause short- and long-term physical, emotional, and social problems among victims, and is, therefore, a serious public health concern (Copeland, Wolke, Angold, & Costello, 2013; Gini & Pozzoli, 2009; Nakamoto & Schwartz, 2009).However, the magnitude of the problem, the prevalence of bullying behavior, the common antecedents ...

  18. Bullying in Schools: Causes, Effects, and Solutions

    Published: Dec 16, 2021. It is common to see bullying happen in the high school age group. Many teenagers often believe they can get away with bullying due to their lack of consequences or unclear consequences. Due to the advancement of technology, kids now have another source of bullying; social media. Children are no longer able to escape the ...

  19. Anti-Bullying Programs In Schools May Do More Harm Than Good

    getty. Anti-bullying programs in schools may be doing more harm than good. Programs that enlist fellow students to intervene or help support the victim can lead to an increase in bullying and make ...

  20. Anti-Bullying Programs: Do they Help or Hurt?

    This shows that anti-bullying programs aren't only defective due to the fact they don't do much to prevent bullies, but also because they are helping bullies become better at being bullies. So even if anti-bullying programs were intended to be helpful, they end up hurting more than doing good.

  21. Bullying: The Role Of Social Harassment In Our Society

    In the essay "Beyond Bullying" by Krissy Darch and Fazeela Jiwa the readers got a chance to further their understanding of the injustice many victims bullying receive in Canada. The essay starts off with focusing mainly on two victims of bullying; Amanda Todd and Rehtaeh Parsons. These two victims cases were posted on social media under the ...

  22. Benefits to Bullying

    Bullying is a candidate behavior that might fall prey to the former fallacies because, well, it tends to generate many consequences people find unpleasant: having their lunch money taken, being ...

  23. Advantage and Disadvantage of Bullying\

    Read this essay on Advantage and Disadvantage of Bullying\. Come browse our large digital warehouse of free sample essays. Get the knowledge you need in order to pass your classes and more. ... Advantages and Disadvantages of Social Networking in English Language Learning POSTED ON AUGUST 3, 2013 Social Networking in Language Learning ...

  24. Why people have fallen out of love with dating apps

    Younger adults are growing especially weary of the apps. One survey commissioned last year by Axios, a news site, found that only a fifth of American college students were using them at least once ...