essay on online world

45,000+ students realised their study abroad dream with us. Take the first step today

Meet top uk universities from the comfort of your home, here’s your new year gift, one app for all your, study abroad needs, start your journey, track your progress, grow with the community and so much more.

essay on online world

Verification Code

An OTP has been sent to your registered mobile no. Please verify

essay on online world

Thanks for your comment !

Our team will review it before it's shown to our readers.

Leverage Edu

  • School Education /

Essay On Online Education: In 100 Words, 150 Words, and 200 Words

essay on online world

  • Updated on  
  • November 6, 2024

Essay-On-Online-Education

Online education has emerged as a significant transformation in the global education landscape, particularly in the wake of the COVID-19 pandemic . This essay explores the various facets of online education, from its inception to its advantages and disadvantages and its impact on learners and educators alike. The evolution of online education presents a new horizon for accessible and flexible learning .

Table of Contents

  • 1 Paragraph on Online Education in 100 words
  • 2 Essay on Online Education in 150 words
  • 3 Essay on Online Education in 250 words
  • 4 Short Essay on Online Education

Also Read: English Essay Topics

Paragraph on Online Education in 100 words

Online education is a modern educational pattern where students access instructional content through the internet. This innovative approach has gained immense popularity, especially after the pandemic. Due to its convenience and adaptability, individuals are more inclined towards this way of learning and teaching. It has enabled students of all ages to acquire knowledge from the comfort of their homes crossing all the geographical barriers. Online education offers a diverse range of courses and resources and fosters continuous learning. However, it also presents challenges, such as dependency on technology and potential disengagement from the physical world.

Also Read: The Beginner’s Guide to Writing an Essay

Essay on Online Education in 150 words

Online education marks a revolutionary shift in how we acquire knowledge. It harnesses the power of the internet to deliver educational content to students, making learning more flexible and accessible. Technology advancements have accelerated the development of online education, enabling educational institutions to provide a wide range of courses and programmes through digital platforms.

One of the primary advantages of online education is its ability to cater to a diverse audience, regardless of geographical location or physical limitations. It reduces the need for travelling to study and offers a cost-effective alternative to traditional classroom learning. However, online education also comes with its challenges. It requires self-discipline and motivation as students often learn independently. Additionally, prolonged screen time can have adverse effects on students’ physical and mental well-being, potentially leading to social disconnection. Online education is a smart way to impact education but its benefits and consequences should be measured well.

Essay on Online Education in 250 words

Online education has witnessed remarkable growth in recent years, with the internet serving as the platform for delivering educational content. This transformation has been accelerated, especially in response to the global pandemic. Online education crosses all the boundaries of traditional learning, offering students the opportunity to acquire knowledge and skills from anywhere in the world.

One of the most compelling aspects of online education is its flexibility. Learners can access course materials and engage with instructors at their convenience, breaking free from rigid schedules. Moreover, this mode of education has expanded access to a vast array of courses, allowing individuals to pursue their interests and career goals without geographical constraints.

However, it’s important to acknowledge the challenges associated with online education. Limited face-to-face interaction can lead to feelings of isolation among the students. It demands a high degree of self-discipline, as students must navigate the coursework independently. Prolonged screen time can have adverse effects on health and may lead to a sense of disconnection from society. Technical issues can also hinder the online learning experience for some students. For underprivileged students, the access to Internet can be challenging.

In conclusion, online education represents a significant shift in how we approach learning. It offers unprecedented access and flexibility but also requires learners to adapt to a more self-directed approach to education. Striking a balance between the benefits and challenges of online education is key to harnessing its full potential. As technology continues to improve, online education will likely become an even more integral part of our educational landscape.

Also Read: Essay on Fire Safety in 200 and 500+ words in English for Students

Short Essay on Online Education

Find a sample essay on online education below:

An organised thought backed up by proof and examples is the key to writing a convincing essay. Create a clear thesis statement in the introduction, follow a logical order of points, and then summarise your main points.

To improve readability, use clear and concise language, break your essay into paragraphs with clear topic sentences, and vary your sentence structure.

If you’re struggling to meet the word count, review your content to see if you can expand on your ideas, provide more examples, or include additional details to support your arguments. Additionally, check for any irrelevant information that can be removed.

Related Reads

We hope that this essay on Online Education helps. For more amazing daily reads related to essay writing , stay tuned with Leverage Edu .

' src=

Manasvi Kotwal

Manasvi's flair in writing abilities is derived from her past experience of working with bootstrap start-ups, Advertisement and PR agencies as well as freelancing. She's currently working as a Content Marketing Associate at Leverage Edu to be a part of its thriving ecosystem.

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Contact no. *

essay on online world

Connect With Us

45,000+ students realised their study abroad dream with us. take the first step today..

essay on online world

Resend OTP in

essay on online world

Need help with?

Study abroad.

UK, Canada, US & More

IELTS, GRE, GMAT & More

Scholarship, Loans & Forex

Country Preference

New Zealand

Which English test are you planning to take?

Which academic test are you planning to take.

Not Sure yet

When are you planning to take the exam?

Already booked my exam slot

Within 2 Months

Want to learn about the test

Which Degree do you wish to pursue?

When do you want to start studying abroad.

January 2025

September 2025

What is your budget to study abroad?

essay on online world

How would you describe this article ?

Please rate this article

We would like to hear more.

Have something on your mind?

essay on online world

Make your study abroad dream a reality in January 2022 with

essay on online world

India's Biggest Virtual University Fair

essay on online world

Essex Direct Admission Day

Why attend .

essay on online world

Don't Miss Out

Home — Essay Samples — Sociology — Self Identity — Who Am I in the Digital World

test_template

Who Am I in The Digital World

  • Categories: Digital Era Self Identity Self Reflection

About this sample

close

Words: 669 |

Published: Aug 31, 2023

Words: 669 | Page: 1 | 4 min read

Image of Dr. Oliver Johnson

Cite this Essay

To export a reference to this article please select a referencing style below:

Let us write you an essay from scratch

  • 450+ experts on 30 subjects ready to help
  • Custom essay delivered in as few as 3 hours

Get high-quality help

author

Prof Ernest (PhD)

Verified writer

  • Expert in: Information Science and Technology Sociology Life

writer

+ 120 experts online

By clicking “Check Writers’ Offers”, you agree to our terms of service and privacy policy . We’ll occasionally send you promo and account related email

No need to pay just yet!

Related Essays

2 pages / 1039 words

4 pages / 1827 words

2 pages / 760 words

3 pages / 1332 words

Remember! This is just a sample.

You can get your custom paper by one of our expert writers.

121 writers online

Who Am I in The Digital World Essay

Still can’t find what you need?

Browse our vast selection of original essay samples, each expertly formatted and styled

Related Essays on Self Identity

Self-control is a big deal in how we humans behave. Lots of smart folks in psychology and sociology have studied it. It's about being able to manage our thoughts, feelings, and actions to reach our goals and resist doing stuff [...]

Embarking on a journey of introspection, the question "what is your social identity?" propels us to delve into the depths of self-awareness and self-discovery. This essay endeavors to explore the profound significance of my [...]

As human beings, our social behavior and interactions are often guided by a set of fundamental motives. These five core social motives include the need for belonging, understanding and control, self-enhancement, trust, and [...]

Pallasmaa J, Mallgrave HF, Arbib M. Architecture and Neuroscience. Tidwell P, editor. Finland: Tapio Wirkkala Rut Bryk Foundation; 2015.Colomina B, Wigley M. Are we human? notes on an archaeology of design. Netherlands: Lars [...]

How do you present yourself to the world online and offline? Are you the same person Online and Offline? Which version of you is most true? These questions have made me think about the difference between our online self vs [...]

?From the point of view of a reader, it is clear that Jamaica Kincaid is not satisfied with the way Antigua is now. By comparing pre-colonial Antigua with colonial and post-colonial Antigua, Kincaid creates a novel that is [...]

Related Topics

By clicking “Send”, you agree to our Terms of service and Privacy statement . We will occasionally send you account related emails.

Where do you want us to send this sample?

By clicking “Continue”, you agree to our terms of service and privacy policy.

Be careful. This essay is not unique

This essay was donated by a student and is likely to have been used and submitted before

Download this Sample

Free samples may contain mistakes and not unique parts

Sorry, we could not paraphrase this essay. Our professional writers can rewrite it and get you a unique paper.

Please check your inbox.

We can write you a custom essay that will follow your exact instructions and meet the deadlines. Let's fix your grades together!

Get Your Personalized Essay in 3 Hours or Less!

We use cookies to personalyze your web-site experience. By continuing we’ll assume you board with our cookie policy .

  • Instructions Followed To The Letter
  • Deadlines Met At Every Stage
  • Unique And Plagiarism Free

essay on online world

An official website of the United States government

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

The “online brain”: how the Internet may be changing our cognition

Joseph firth, john torous, brendon stubbs, josh a firth, genevieve z steiner, mario alvarez‐jimenez, john gleeson, davy vancampfort, christopher j armitage, jerome sarris.

  • Author information
  • Article notes
  • Copyright and License information

Issue date 2019 Jun.

The impact of the Internet across multiple aspects of modern society is clear. However, the influence that it may have on our brain structure and functioning remains a central topic of investigation. Here we draw on recent psychological, psychiatric and neuroimaging findings to examine several key hypotheses on how the Internet may be changing our cognition. Specifically, we explore how unique features of the online world may be influencing: a) attentional capacities, as the constantly evolving stream of online information encourages our divided attention across multiple media sources, at the expense of sustained concentration; b) memory processes, as this vast and ubiquitous source of online information begins to shift the way we retrieve, store, and even value knowledge; and c) social cognition, as the ability for online social settings to resemble and evoke real‐world social processes creates a new interplay between the Internet and our social lives, including our self‐concepts and self‐esteem. Overall, the available evidence indicates that the Internet can produce both acute and sustained alterations in each of these areas of cognition, which may be reflected in changes in the brain. However, an emerging priority for future research is to determine the effects of extensive online media usage on cognitive development in youth, and examine how this may differ from cognitive outcomes and brain impact of uses of Internet in the elderly. We conclude by proposing how Internet research could be integrated into broader research settings to study how this unprecedented new facet of society can affect our cognition and the brain across the life course.

Keywords: Internet, cognition, attention, memory, social structures, social media, addiction, virtual reality

The Internet is the most widespread and rapidly adopted technology in the history of humanity. In only decades, Internet use has completely re‐invented the ways in which we search for information, consume media and entertainment, and manage our social networks and relationships. With the even more recent advent of smartphones, Internet access has become portable and ubiquitous to the point at which the population of the developed world can be considered “online” 1 , 2 , 3 .

However, the impact that this new channel for connection, information, communication, and screen time is having on our brains and cognitive functioning is unclear. Prior to the Internet, a large body of research had convincingly demonstrated that the brain is somewhat malleable to environmental demands and stimuli, particularly with regards to learning new processes, due to its capacity for neuroplasticity 4 . Various scenarios have been observed to induce long‐term changes in the neuronal architecture of the human brain, including second‐language acquisition 5 , learning new motor skills (such as juggling) 6 , and even formal education or exam preparation 7 . The widespread use of the Internet across the globe has introduced, for many, the necessity and opportunity to learn a myriad of new skills and ways to interact with society, which could bring about neural changes. As an example, even simple interactions with the Internet through the smartphone's touchscreen interface have been demonstrated to bring about sustained neurocognitive alterations due to neural changes in cortical regions associated with sensory and motor processing of the hand and thumb 8 . Beyond this, the Internet also presents a novel platform for almost‐endless learning of new information and complex processes, relevant to both the online and offline world 9 .

Along with neuroplastic mechanisms, other environmental and biological factors can also cause changes in the brain's structure and function, resulting in cognitive decline 10 . In aging samples, for instance, there is evidence to indicate that age‐related cognitive decline may be partly driven by a process of atrophy. Some studies have shown that adopting a less engaging lifestyle across the lifespan may accelerate loss of cognitive function 11 , due to lower “cognitive reserve” (the ability of the brain to withstand insult from age and/or pathology) 12 . Some emerging evidence indicates that disengaging from the “real world” in favor of virtual settings may similarly induce adverse neurocognitive changes. For example, a recent randomized controlled trial (RCT) 13 found that six weeks of engaging in an online role playing game caused significant reductions in grey matter within the orbitofrontal cortex – a brain region implicated in impulse control and decision making. However, the study did not address the extent to which these results were specific to online gaming, rather than general internet usage. Nonetheless, this raises the possibility that various types of Internet usage could differentially affect the brain and cognitive processes – in both adverse and beneficial ways. This may be of particular relevance to the developing brains of children and adolescents, as many cognitive processes (particularly those relevant to higher executive functions and social cognition) are not entirely innate, but rather are strongly influenced by environmental factors 14 .

Although only recently emerging, this possibility has led to a substantial body of research empirically investigating the multiple potential pathways through which the Internet could affect our brains’ structure, function, and cognitive development. Specifically, the bulk of existing research can be separated into three specific domains, examining how the internet is affecting: a) attention (i.e., how the constant influx of online information, prompts and notifications competing for our attention may encourage individuals to displace their concentration across multiple incoming media streams – and the consequences this may have for attentional‐switching versus sustained‐attention tasks); b) memory and knowledge (i.e., the extent to which we rely on the Internet as our primary informational resource, and how unique properties of online information access may affect how we process new memories and value our internal knowledge); c) social cognition (along with the personal and societal consequences of increasingly embedding our social networks, interactions, and status within the online world).

In this state‐of‐the‐art review, we present the current leading hypotheses of how the Internet may alter these cognitive processes, subsequently examining the extent to which these hypotheses are supported by recent findings from psychological, psychiatric and neuroimaging research. In this way, we aggregate the contemporary evidence arising from multiple fields of research to produce revised models on how the Internet may be affecting our brains and cognition. Furthermore, whereas studies to date have focused upon only specific age groups, we examine the effects of the Internet on the human brain across the entire life course. In particular, we explore how the potential benefits/drawbacks of extensive Internet integration with cognitive processes may differ among children and older adults. Finally, we identify important gaps in the existing literature to present key priorities for future research in order to gain new insights for minimizing detrimental effects of the Internet, while capitalizing on this new feature of our societies to potentially influence neurocognitive processes in a beneficial way.

“DIGITAL DISTRACTIONS”: A HIJACK OF ATTENTION ON THE INFORMATION HIGHWAY?

How does the internet gain and sustain our attention.

The Internet consumes a considerable chunk of our attention on a day‐to‐day basis. The vast majority of adults go online daily, and over a quarter report being online “almost constantly” 2 . Within this, one in five American adults are now “smartphone‐only” Internet users 1 . Importantly, the introduction of these Internet‐enabled mobile devices has also reduced the “digital divide” previously experienced by lower and middle income countries 15 . The amount and frequency of Internet usage is even more pronounced amongst younger people. Most adults today witnessed the beginning of the transition from “Internet‐free” to “Internet‐everywhere” societies. However, younger generations (termed “digital natives” 16 ) have been brought up entirely within a “connected world” , particularly in developed countries. Consequently, digital natives are often the first to adopt new online technologies as they arise 16 , and engage extensively with all existing features of the Internet. For instance, 95% of US teens have access to a smartphone, and 45% are online “almost constantly” 3 .

Multiple factors are driving the rapid uptake and extensive usage of Internet‐enabled technologies across the globe. This is partly due to the Internet now being unavoidable, ubiquitous, and a highly functional aspect of modern living. For instance, Internet use is now deeply entwined with education, travel, socializing, commerce, and the majority of workplaces. Along with pragmatic uses, the Internet also offers an endless array of recreational and entertainment activities, through podcasts, e‐books, videos, streaming movies and gaming. However, the ability of the Internet to capture and hold attention is not solely due to the quality of media content available online. Rather, it is also driven by the underlying design and presentation of the online world. One such example is the self‐evolving “attraction mechanism”; whereby aspects of the Internet that fail to gain attention are quickly drowned out in the sea of incoming information, while the successful aspects of the adverts, articles, apps or anything that does manage to capture our attention (even superficially) are logged (through clicks and scrolls), noticed (through online shares), and subsequently proliferated and expanded upon. Alongside this, leading technology companies have been accused of intentionally capitalizing on the addictive potential of Internet, by studying, testing, and refining the attention‐grabbing aspects of their websites and applications (“apps”) to promote extremely high levels of engagement, without due concern for user well‐being 17 .

Furthermore, even when not using the Internet for any specific purpose, smartphones have introduced widespread and habitual “checking” behaviours, characterized by quick but frequent inspections of the device for incoming information from news, social media, or personal contacts 18 . These habits are thought to be the result of behavioural reinforcement from “information rewards” that are received immediately on checking the device 19 , potentially engaging the cortico‐striatal dopaminergic system due to their readily available nature 20 . The variable‐ratio reinforcement schedule inherent to device checking may further perpetuate these compulsive behaviours 21 .

Cognitive consequences of the attention‐grabbing Internet

The unprecedented potential of the Internet to capture our attention presents an urgent need for understanding the impact that this may have on our thought processes and well‐being. Already, education providers are beginning to perceive detrimental effects of the Internet on children's attention, with over 85% of teachers endorsing the statement that “today's digital technologies are creating an easily distracted generation” 22 . The primary hypothesis on how the Internet affects our attentional capacities is through hyperlinks, notifications, and prompts providing a limitless stream of different forms of digital media, thus encouraging us to interact with multiple inputs simultaneously, but only on a shallow level, in a behavioural pattern termed “media multi‐tasking” 23 , 24 .

The seminal study by Ophir et al 23 was among the first to explore the sustained impact of media multi‐tasking on cognitive capacities. This was a cross‐sectional study of individuals who engaged in “heavy” (i.e., frequent and extensive) media multi‐tasking compared to those who did not. Cognitive testing of the two groups produced the then‐surprising finding that those involved in heavy media multi‐tasking performed worse in task‐switching tests than their counterparts – contrary to the authors’ expectation that the “extra practice” afforded by frequent media multi‐tasking would confer cognitive benefit in task‐switching scenarios. Closer inspection of findings suggested that the impeded task‐switching ability in heavy media multi‐tasking individuals was due to their increased susceptibility to distraction from irrelevant environmental stimuli 23 .

Since these initial findings, the effects of media multi‐tasking on cognition have come under increasing scrutiny, because the increasingly diverse forms of entertainment and activities available through the online world can further our capabilities (and temptation) of engaging in media multi‐tasking 25 , even on single devices. For instance, Yeykelis et al 26 measured participants’ media multi‐tasking between different types of online media content while using just one device (personal laptops), and found that switches occurred as frequently as every 19 seconds, with 75% of all on‐screen content being viewed for less than one minute. Measures of skin conductance during the study found that arousal increased in the seconds leading up to media switching, reaching a high point at the moment of the switch, followed by a decline afterward 26 . Again, this suggests that the proclivity for alternating between different computer windows, opening new hyperlinks, and performing new searches could be driven by the readily available nature of the informational rewards, which are potentially awaiting in the unattended media stream. Supporting this, the study also found that, whereas switching from work‐related content to entertainment was associated with increased arousal in anticipation of the switch, there was no anticipatory arousal spike associated with entertainment to work‐content switches 26 .

The growing concern around the increasing amount of media multi‐tasking with the spread of ubiquitous Internet access has resulted in further empirical studies. These have produced conflicting findings, with some failing to find any adverse effects on attention 27 , and others indicating that media multi‐tasking may even be linked to increased performance for other aspects of cognition, such as multisensory integration 28 . Nonetheless the literature, on balance, does seem to indicate that those who engage in frequent and extensive media multi‐tasking in their day‐to‐day lives perform worse in various cognitive tasks than those who do not, particularly for sustained attention 25 .

Imaging studies have shed light onto the neural differences which may account for these cognitive deficits. Functionally, those who engage in heavy media multi‐tasking perform poorer in distracted attention tasks, even though exhibiting greater activity in right prefrontal regions 29 . As right prefrontal regions are typically activated in response to distractor stimuli, the observed increases in recruitment of these regions alongside poorer performance suggests that heavy media multi‐taskers require greater cognitive effort to maintain concentration when faced with distractor stimuli 29 . Structurally, high levels of Internet usage 30 and heavy media multi‐tasking 31 are associated with decreased grey matter in prefrontal regions associated with maintaining goals in face of distraction (such as the right frontal pole and anterior cingulate cortex). However, the findings to date must be interpreted with caution, as various confounding factors may be affecting the results of these cross‐sectional imaging studies. Although the differences persist when controlling for general digital media use and other simple confounders (age, gender, etc.), further research is required to examine if the observed neural differences are specifically attributable to heavy vs. light media multi‐tasking, or in fact driven by broader differences in lifestyle between the two groups.

Given the amount of time that people now spend in media multi‐tasking via personal digital devices, it is increasingly relevant to consider not only sustained changes which arise in those who engage in large amounts of media multi‐tasking, but also the acute effects on immediate cognitive capacities. A meta‐analysis of 41 studies showed that engaging in multi‐tasking was associated with significantly poorer overall cognitive performance, with a moderate‐to‐large effect size (Cohen's d=–0.71, 95% CI: –0.86 to –0.57). This has been confirmed by more recent studies, further showing that even short‐term engagement with an extensively hyperlinked online environment (i.e., online shopping for 15 minutes) reduces attentional scope for a sustained duration after coming offline, whereas reading a magazine does not produce these deficits 32 .

Overall, the available evidence strongly indicates that engaging in multi‐tasking via digital media does not improve our multi‐tasking performance in other settings – and in fact seems to decrease this cognitive capacity through reducing our ability to ignore incoming distractions. Much of the multi‐tasking investigations so far have been focusing on personal computers. However, smartphone technologies may even further encourage people to engage in media multi‐tasking through high rates of incoming prompts from emails, direct messages and social media notifications occurring while both using and not using the device. Thus, along with determining long‐term consequences of media multi‐tasking, future research should examine how the constant multi‐tasking made possible by Internet‐enabled mobile devices may impact daily functioning through acute but high frequency effects.

Furthermore, both the immediate and chronic effects of media multi‐tasking are relatively unexplored in children and adolescents, who are the prime users of such technologies 33 and are at a phase of development that is crucial for refining higher cognitive abilities 14 . The first longitudinal study of media multi‐tasking in young people has recently found that frequent multi‐tasking behaviours do predict the development of attentional deficits specifically in early adolescents, but not in older teens 34 . Additionally, extensive media multi‐tasking during childhood and adolescence could also negatively impact cognitive development through indirect means, by reducing engagement with academic and social activities, as well as by interfering with sleep 35 , or reducing the opportunity to engage in creative thinking 36 , 37 . Clearly, further research is necessary to properly measure the effects of ubiquitous computing on children's cognitive development, and to find practical ways for ameliorating any detrimental impact this may be having.

“iFORMATION”: NEUROCOGNITIVE RESPONSES TO ONLINE INFORMATION GATHERING

The internet and transactive memory.

In response to the question “How has the Internet changed your life?” , some common answers include finding new friends, renewing old friendships, studying online, finding romantic relationships, furthering career opportunities, shopping, and travel 38 . However, the most common answer is people stating that the Internet has “changed the way in which they access information” 38 . Indeed, for the first time in human history, the majority of people living in the developed world have access to almost all factual information in existence literally at their fingertips.

Along with the obvious advantages, this unique situation also introduces the possibility of the Internet ultimately negating or replacing the need for certain human memory systems – particularly for aspects of “semantic memory” (i.e., memory of facts) – which are somewhat independent from other types of memory in the human brain 39 . An initial indication of Internet information gathering affecting typical memory processes was provided by Sparrow et al 40 , who demonstrated that the ability to access information online caused people to become more likely to remember where these facts could be retrieved rather than the facts themselves, indicating that people quickly become reliant on the Internet for information retrieval.

It could be argued that this is not unique to the Internet, but rather just an example of the online world acting as a form of external memory or “transactive memory” 40 , 41 . Transactive memory has been an integral part of human societies for millennia, and refers to the process by which people opt to outsource information to other individuals within their families, communities, etc., such that they are able to just remember the source of the knowledge, rather than attempting to store all of this information themselves 41 . Although beneficial at a group level, using transactive memory systems does reduce an individual's ability to recall the specifics of the externally stored information 42 . This may be due to individuals using transactive memory for “cognitive offloading” , implicitly reducing their allocation of cognitive resources towards remembering this information, since they know this will be available for future reference externally. This phenomenon has been demonstrated in multiple contexts, including those of team work 43 and other “non‐Internet” technologies (e.g., photography reducing individuals’ memories of the objects they photographed) 44 .

However, it is becoming clear that the Internet actually presents something entirely novel and distinct from previous transactive memory systems 45 , 46 . Crucially, the Internet seems to bypass the “transactional” aspect that is inherent to other forms of cognitive offloading in two ways. First, the Internet does not place any responsibility on the user to retain unique information for others to draw upon (as would typically be required in human societies) 45 . Second, unlike other transactive memory stores, the Internet acts as a single entity that is responsible for holding and retrieving virtually all factual information, and thus does not require individuals to remember what exact information is externally stored, or even where it is located. In this way, the Internet is becoming a “supernormal stimulus” 46 for transactive memory – making all other options for cognitive offloading (including books, friends, community) become redundant, as they are outcompeted by the novel capabilities for external information storage and retrieval made possible by the Internet.

How does a supernormal stimulus interact with normal cognition?

Unfortunately, the rapid methods of acquisition and constant availability of information afforded by the Internet may not necessarily lead to better use of information gained. For instance, an experimental study 47 found that individuals instructed to search for specific information online completed the information gathering task faster than those using printed encyclopedias, but were subsequently less able to recall the information accurately.

During Internet and encyclopedia information gathering tasks, functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to examine activation in the ventral and dorsal streams. These regions are referred to as the “what” and “where” streams, respectively, due to their indicated roles in storing either the specific content (ventral stream) or external location (dorsal stream) of incoming information 47 . Although there was no difference in activation of the dorsal stream, results showed that the poorer recall of Internet‐sought information compared to encyclopedia‐based learning was associated with reduced activation of the ventral (“what”) stream during online information gathering. These findings further support the possibility, initially raised by Sparrow et al 40 , that online information gathering, while faster, may fail to sufficiently recruit brain regions for storing information on a long‐term basis.

The potential for online searching to produce a sustained impact upon our cognitive processes has been investigated in a series of studies examining pre‐post changes following a six‐day Internet search training paradigm. In these studies, young adults were given an hour per day of Internet search tasks, and undertook an array of cognitive and neuroimaging assessments pre‐ and post‐training. Results showed that the six‐day Internet search training reduced regional homogeneity and functional connectivity of brain areas involved in long‐term memory formation and retrieval (e.g., temporal gyrus) 48 . This indicates that a reliance on online searching may impede memory retrieval by reducing the functional connectivity and synchronization of associated brain regions 48 . Furthermore, when faced with new questions after the six days, the training had increased participants’ self‐reported impulses towards using the Internet to answer those questions, which was reflected in a recruitment of prefrontal brain areas required for behavioural and impulse control 49 . This increased propensity for relying on Internet searches for gathering new information has been replicated in subsequent studies 50 , and is in keeping with the “supernormal stimulus” nature of the Internet, potentially suggesting that online information gathering quickly trains people to become dependent on this tool when faced with unknown issues.

However, despite the possible adverse effects on regular “offline” memory, the six‐days training did make people more efficient at using the Internet for retrieving information, as participants became faster at the search tasks, with no loss of accuracy 51 . Search training also produced increases in white matter integrity of the fiber tracts connecting the frontal, occipital, parietal and temporal lobes, significantly more than the non‐search control condition 52 . In other studies, cognitive offloading via digital devices has also been found to improve people's ability to focus on aspects that are not immediately retrievable, and thus remember these better in the future 53 .

These findings seem to support the emergent hypotheses that relying on the Internet for factual memory storage may actually produce cognitive benefit in other areas, perhaps by “freeing up” cognitive resources 54 , and thus enabling us to use our newly available cognitive capacities for more ambitious undertakings than previously possible 45 . Researchers advocating this view have pointed to multiple domains of collective human endeavor that have already been transformed by the Internet's provision of supernormal transactive memory, such as education, journalism and even academia 55 . As online technologies continue to advance (particularly with regards to “wearables”), it is conceivable that the performance benefits from the Internet, which are already visible at the societal level, could ultimately become integrated within individuals themselves, enabling new heights of cognitive function 56 .

Unfortunately, however, a more sobering finding with regards to the immediate possibility of ubiquitous Internet access enabling new heights of human intelligence is provided by Barr et al 57 , who observed that analytical thinkers, with higher cognitive capacities, actually use their smartphone less for transactive memory in day‐to‐day situations compared to individuals with non‐analytical thinking styles. Furthermore, the reduced smartphone usage in analytical versus non‐analytical thinkers was specific to online information searching, with no differences in social media or entertainment usages, thus indicating that the differences are likely due to the Internet furthering “cognitive miserliness” among less analytical thinkers 57 .

Alongside this, the increasing reliance on the Internet for information may cause individuals to “blur the lines” between their own capabilities and their devices’ 58 . In a series of experiments, Fisher et al 59 investigated how the Internet influences our self‐perceived knowledge. Results showed that online searching increases our sense of how much we know, even though the illusion of self‐knowledge is only perceived for the domains in which the Internet can “fill in the gaps” for us. The experiments also demonstrated how quickly individuals internalized the Internet's external knowledge as their own – as even immediately after using the Internet to answer the task questions, participants attributed their higher quality explanations to “increased brain activity” . More recent studies have shown that illusions of self‐knowledge similarly persist when using smartphones to retrieve online information 58 . As individuals become more and more connected with their personal digital devices (which are also always accessible), it seems inevitable that the distinction between self and Internet's abilities will become increasingly elusive, potentially creating a constant illusion of “greater than actual knowledge” among large portions of the population.

Overall, the Internet clearly can provide a “superstimulus” for transactive memory, which is already changing the way we store, retrieve, and even value knowledge. However, with popular online information sources such as Google and Wikipedia less than 20 years old, it is currently not possible to ascertain how this may eventually be reflected in long‐term changes to the structure and function of the human brain. Nonetheless, our constant connection with the online world through personal devices (i.e., smartphones), along with the emerging potential for more direct integration through wearable devices, certainly indicates that we are set to become more reliant on the Internet for factual information as time goes on. Also, whereas the studies described above have focused on factual knowledge, the Internet is also now becoming a superstimulus for spatial information (through providing constant access to online maps and global positioning system). As spatial memory is somewhat independent from semantic memory in the human brain 60 , further research should investigate the multitude of ways in which extensive use of these external memory systems may reduce, enhance or alter our cognitive capacities.

ONLINE SOCIAL NETWORKS: FAULTY CONNECTIONS, OR FALSE DICHOTOMY?

Human sociality in the online world.

Social relationships and having a sense of connection are important determinants of happiness and stress relief 61 , 62 , mental and physical well‐being 63 , 64 , and even mortality 65 . Over the past decade, the proportion of an individual's social interactions that take place online within social networking sites (e.g., Facebook, Instagram, Twitter) has grown dramatically 66 , 67 , and our connection with these sites is now strongly meshed with the offline world. The real‐world implications of this are perhaps best evidenced by the critical role that social media have played in multiple global affairs, including reportedly starting and precipitating the London Riots, the Occupy movement 68 , and even the Arab Spring 69 , along with potentially influencing the outcomes of the UK's European Union Referendum (“Brexit”) 70 and the 2016 US elections 71 . Clearly, understanding the shift from real‐world interactions into the online social environment (and vice versa) holds significance to almost all aspects of people's lives.

Our motivations towards using social media is broadly similar to the instinctual desires underlying “real world” social interactions, as people are drawn to online sociality in order to exchange information and ideas, along with gaining social support and friendships 72 . However, whether or not these virtual interactions engage the human brain in ways analogous to real‐world socialization remains a topic of debate since the turn of the century 73 . Whereas it would be highly beneficial if social media sites could fulfil the implicit human needs for social connection, it may be that the distinction between online and offline networks is so great that entirely different cognitive domains are involved in navigating these different environments 74 , 75 .

How does the online environment affect our fundamental social structures?

To investigate the neuroimaging correlates of offline and online networks, the seminal study by Kanai et al 74 collected real‐world social network size, online sociality (i.e., Facebook friends) and magnetic resonance imaging scans from 125 participants. Results showed that both real‐world social network size and number of Facebook friends were significantly associated with amygdala volume. As this has previously been established as a key brain region for social cognition and social network size 76 , these results present a strong case for the overlap between online and offline sociality in the human brain.

However, those authors also found that the grey matter volume of other brain regions (specifically, posterior regions of the middle temporal gyrus and superior temporal sulcus, and the right entorhinal cortex) were predicted by the numbers of participants’ Facebook friends, but held no relationship to their real‐world social networks. This suggests that certain unique aspects of social media implicate aspects of the brain that are not central in “real‐world” social settings. For instance, the tendency for online networks to encourage us towards holding many weak social connections, involving thousands of face‐to‐name pairs, could require high associative memory capacities, which is not typically required in real‐world networks (as these are comprised of fewer, but more familiar, relationships) 74 . As associative memory formation for name‐face pairs involves the right entorhinal cortex 77 , 78 , this could explain the exclusive relationship that this region holds with online social (but not real‐world) network size 74 .

Indeed, one key difference which may separate how the brain handles online and offline social networks is the unique capacity afforded by the Internet for people to hold, and simultaneously interact with, millions of “friendships” 79 , 80 . Empirical testing of this hypothesis is a most fruitful area of investigation stemming from research into the fundamental similarities and differences between these two social worlds at a biological level 66 . When defining “friendships” under a broad context (people who maintain contact and share an emotional bond) 66 , two patterns are prominent across a diverse range of real‐world social networks: a) the average individual has around 150 “friendships” (but this is highly variable between individuals), and b) this is made up of five hierarchical layers, consisting of primary partners, intimate relationships, best friends, close friends, and all friends, which follow a size‐scaling ratio of around 3 (i.e., each cumulative layer is 3 times bigger than the last), and therefore have set average (cumulative/inclusive) sizes of 1.5, 5, 15, 50 and 150 respectively 66 . The patterns of the average number of 150 total friendship connections, and the scaling sizes of the five hierarchical layers of relationships making this up, have been found across regions and time periods within various human organizations, ranging from hunter‐gatherer societies 81 , 82 and historical village populations 83 , armies 66 , residential camps 84 , to personal networks of modern Europeans 85 .

Thus, given the unprecedented potential that online social networks allow in terms of number of connections, and the varied contexts these take place over 79 , 80 , it is imaginable that this extraordinary environment may allow these two apparently set aspects of real‐world social networks to be bypassed. However, recent findings have confirmed that user‐to‐user friendship connections, posting patterns and exchanges within Twitter, Facebook, and even online gaming platforms, all indicate a similar average number of general friendships (around 150, despite high skew), along with maintaining the same scaled sizes of the hierarchical structure of the five distinct friendship layers (as determined by reciprocal communication exchanges) 86 , 87 , 88 , 89 . Therefore, even within the unique realms of online social networks, the most fundamental operations of human social networks appear to remain relatively unchanged 88 , 89 . So, it is highly conceivable that the social connections formed in the online world are processed in similar ways to those of the offline world, and thus have much potential to carry over from the Internet to shape “real‐world” sociality, including our social interactions and our perceptions of social hierarchies, in ways that are not restricted to the context of the Internet.

The driving forces that sustain the set structural patterns of social networks, even when faced with the immense connective potential of the online world, may be broadly explained by two overlapping mechanisms. First, constraints on social cognition within the human brain seem to carry over across social contexts 66 . For instance, humans struggle to engagingly interact with more than three individuals simultaneously in the real world, and this limitation on attention also appears to apply online 90 , 91 . This evidence is in agreement with the hypothesis that circumventing the cognitive constraints on social relationships may be difficult even when technology affords unnatural opportunities to do so 88 .

The second driver of set boundaries on social activity is that simple underlying factors may produce social constraints, even within online settings. Most obviously, investment in social relationships is limited by time constraints, and this may contribute to the set patterns of both the number and type of social connections 93 , 94 . In line with this, analyses across various social contexts have shown that temporal limitations govern the number of social interactions that individuals engage in, and how they distribute these across their different kinds of relationships 93 , 94 . Again, these general interaction rates remain similar within online social networks 87 , 88 .

The possibility that the parameters on all social networks (online or offline) are governed by basic underlying factors is further supported by research showing that similar structures also exist within simpler social systems, such as animal societies 66 , 95 . For instance, the sizes and scaling of hierarchical “friendship” layers found in online and offline human networks are also found in dolphins, elephants, and various primate species 96 , and the phenomena of humans increasing the number and strength of their social networks connections following the death of a friend on Facebook 97 is also seen in wild birds, which show compensatory up‐regulation of their social network connections upon experiencing the loss of a social associate 98 .

Supporting the idea that limited cognitive capacities govern our social structures is research showing that the brain regions predicting individual variation in social network size in humans also do so for macaques 99 . Strong support for simple underlying factors (such as time) governing our general patterning of social interactions can be found in studies demonstrating that entirely computationally simulated systems replicate some of the apparent complexities of human social networks, even under relatively simple rules 100 , 101 . Examples include agent‐based models generating similar social layering structures as humans when sociality is defined as time‐limited 100 .

In light of the current evidence regarding how the Internet may have affected human thinking surrounding social networks, it is undeniable that the online environment poses unique potential and context for social activity 79 , 80 , 102 , 103 , which may invoke some non‐identical cognitive processes and brain areas in comparison to the offline world 74 , 75 . Nevertheless, aside from these comparatively fine‐scale differences, it appears that our brains process the online and offline social networks in surprisingly similar ways, as demonstrated by the shared cognitive capacities and simple underlying factors ultimately governing their fundamental structure 87 , 88 . As such, the online social world has very significant implications for not only measuring and understanding human sociality, but also for governing the outcomes of social processes across various aspects of life.

Social cognitive responses to the online social world

Given the evidence above, an appropriate metaphor for the relationship between online and real‐world sociality could be a “new playing field for the same game” . Even beyond the fundamental structure, emerging research suggests that neurocognitive responses to online social occurrences are similar to those of real‐life interactions. For instance, being rejected online has been shown to increase activity in brain regions strongly linked with social cognition and real‐world rejection (medial prefrontal cortex 104 ) in both adults and children 105 , 106 , 107 . However, within the “same old game” of human sociality, online social media is bending some of the rules – potentially at the expense of users 17 . For instance, whereas real‐world acceptance and rejection is often ambiguous and open to self‐interpretation, social media platforms directly quantify our social success (or failure), by providing clear metrics in the form of “friends” , “followers” , and “likes” (or the potentially painful loss/absence of these) 107 . Given the addictive nature of this immediate, self‐defining feedback, social media companies may even capitalize upon this to maximally engage users 17 . However, growing evidence indicates that relying on online feedback for self‐esteem can have adverse effects on young people, particularly those with low social‐emotional well‐being, due to high rates of cyberbullying 108 , increased anxiety and depression 109 , 110 , and increased perceptions of social isolation and exclusion among those who feel rejected online 111 .

Another process common to human social behaviour in both online and offline worlds is the tendency to make upward social comparisons 112 , 113 . Whereas these can be adaptive and beneficial under regular environmental conditions 112 , this implicit cognitive process can also be hijacked by the artificial environmental manufactured on social media 113 , 114 , which showcases hyper‐successful individuals constantly putting their best foot forward, and even using digital manipulation of images to inflate physical attractiveness. By facilitating exposure to these drastically upward social comparisons (which would rarely be encountered in everyday life), online social media can produce unrealistic expectations of oneself – leading to poor body image and negative self‐concept, particularly for younger people 107 , 111 , 115 , 116 . For instance, in adolescents (particularly females), those who spent more time on social media and smartphones have a greater prevalence of mental health problems, including depression, than those who spent more time on “non‐screen” activities 116 , with greater than 5 hrs/day (versus 1 hr/day) associated with a 66% increased risk of one suicide‐related outcome 117 .

However, a causal relationship between high levels of social media use and poorer mental health is currently difficult to establish, as there is most likely a complex interaction between several confounding factors, including reduced sleep and in‐person social interaction, and increased sedentary behaviour and perceived loneliness 116 , 118 . Nonetheless, given the large amounts of social media use observed among young people, future research should thoroughly examine the potentially detrimental effects that this new setting for sociality may have on health and well‐being, along with aiming to establish the driving factors – such that adjustments can be made in subsequent iterations of social media in order to produce more positive outcomes.

Whereas young people with mental disorders may be the most vulnerable to negative input from social media, these media may also present a new platform for improving mental health in this population, if used correctly. In future, social media may also be exploited to promote ongoing engagement with Internet‐based interventions, while addressing key (but frequently neglected) targets such as social connectedness, social support and self‐efficacy, to aim to bring about sustained functional improvements in severe and complex mental health conditions 119 . To achieve these goals, online social media‐based interventions need to be designed to promote engagement by harnessing, in an ethical and transparent manner, effective strategies used by the industry. For instance, developing technologies which are increasingly adopted by online marketing and tech companies, such as natural language processing, sentiment analyses and machine learning, could be capitalized upon, for example making it possible to identify those at increased risk for suicide or relapse 120 , and rationalizing human driven support to those who need it most at the time they need it 121 . In addition, online systems will be able to learn from what helps individuals and when, opening a window into personalized, real time interventions 121 .

While the use of online social media‐based interventions is in its infancy, pioneering efforts indicate that these interventions are safe, engaging, and have the potential to improve clinical and social outcomes in both patients and their relatives 122 , 123 , 124 , 125 , 126 , 127 . That said, online interventions have failed up to now to be adopted by mental health services 128 , 129 . The main reasons include high attrition rates, poor study designs which reduce translational potential, and a lack of consensus around the required standards of evidence for widespread implementation of Internet‐delivered therapies 130 , 131 , 132 . Efforts are currently underway to determine the long‐term effects of the first generation of social media‐based interventions for mental illness via large randomized controlled trials 133 , 134 . Alongside this clinical use, developing public health strategies for young adults in the general population to avoid the potential adverse effects and negative aspects of typical social media are also warranted.

CONCLUSIONS AND DIRECTIONS

As digital technologies become increasingly integrated with everyday life, the Internet is becoming highly proficient at capturing our attention, while producing a global shift in how people gather information, and connect with one another. In this review, we found emerging support for several hypotheses regarding the pathways through which the Internet is influencing our brains and cognitive processes, particularly with regards to: a) the multi‐faceted stream of incoming information encouraging us to engage in attentional‐switching and “multi‐tasking” , rather than sustained focus; b) the ubiquitous and rapid access to online factual information outcompeting previous transactive systems, and potentially even internal memory processes; c) the online social world paralleling “real world” cognitive processes, and becoming meshed with our offline sociality, introducing the possibility for the special properties of social media to impact on “real life” in unforeseen ways.

However, with fewer than 30 years since the Internet became publicly available, the long‐term effects have yet to be established. Within this, it seems particularly important that future research determines the impact of the Internet on us throughout different points in the lifespan. For instance, the Internet's digital distractions and supernormal capacities for cognitive offloading seem to create a non‐ideal environment for the refinement of higher cognitive functions in critical periods of children and adolescents’ brain development. Indeed, the first longitudinal studies on this topic have found that adverse attentional effects of digital multi‐tasking are particularly pronounced in early adolescence (even compared to older teens) 34 , and that higher frequency of Internet use over 3 years in children is linked with decreased verbal intelligence at follow‐up, along with impeded maturation of both grey and white matter regions 135 .

On the other hand, the opposite may be true in older adults experiencing cognitive decline, for whom the online environment may provide a new source of positive cognitive stimulation. For instance, Internet searching engaged more neural circuitry than reading text pages in Internet savvy older adults (aged 55‐76 years) 9 . Furthermore, experimental studies have found that computer games available online and through smartphones can be used to attenuate aging‐related cognitive decline 136 , 137 , 138 . Thus, the Internet may present a novel and accessible platform for adults to maintain cognitive function throughout old age. Building from this, successful cognitive aging has previously been shown to be dependent upon learning and deploying cognitive strategies, which can compensate for aging‐related decline in “raw” memory capacities 139 . This has previously been referred to as optimizing internal cognitive processes (e.g., through mnemonic strategies), or taking advantage of cognitive offloading in traditional formats (list making, transactive memory, etc.) 139 . Nonetheless, as Internet‐based technologies become more deeply integrated with our daily cognitive processing (through smartphones, wearables, etc.), digital natives could feasibly develop forms of “online cognition” in the aging brain, whereby older adults can increasingly take advantage of web‐based transactive memory and other emerging online processes to fulfil (or even exceed) the typical capacities of a younger brain.

Although it is an emerging area of study, the same could apply for social aspects of the online world. Whereas young people seem particularly prone to the rejections, peer pressure, and negative appraisals this world may induce 107 , older adults may ultimately be able to harness social media in order to overcome isolation and thus continue to benefit from the diverse range of physical, mental and neurocognitive benefits associated with social connection 73 . Viewed collectively, the nascent research in this area already indicates that equivalent types of Internet usage may have differential effects on individuals’ cognitive and social functioning depending on their point in the lifespan.

For better or for worse, we are already conducting a mass‐scale experiment of extensive Internet usage across the global population. A more fine‐scale analysis is essential to gaining a fuller understanding of the sustained impact of this usage across our society. This could include measuring frequency, duration and types of Internet usage as a standard part of national data projects, for instance through collecting Internet data (from either device‐based or self‐report measures) in “biobank” assessment protocols. Combining this with the extensive genetic, socio‐demographic, lifestyle and neuroimaging data gathered by some ongoing projects, researchers could be able to establish the impact of Internet usage on psychological well‐being and brain functioning across entire populations (rather than the currently limited study samples), while also controlling for multiple confounders.

Overall, this early phase of the Internet's introduction into our society is a crucial period for commencing rigorous and extensive research into how different types of Internet usage interact with human cognition, in order to maximize our opportunities for harnessing this new tool in a beneficial manner, while minimizing the potentially adverse effects.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

J. Firth is supported by a Blackmores Institute Fellowship. J. Sarris is supported by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Clinical Research Fellowship (APP1125000). B. Stubbs is supported by the Health Education England and the National Institute for Health Research Integrated Clinical Academic Programme Clinical Lectureship (ICA‐CL‐2017‐03‐001). G.Z. Steiner is supported by an NHMRC‐Australian Research Council (ARC) Dementia Research Development Fellowship (APP1102532). M. Alvarez‐Jimenez is supported by an NHMRC Career Development Fellowship (APP1082934). C.J. Armitage is supported by National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Manchester Biomedical Research Centre and NIHR Greater Manchester Patient Safety Translational Research Centre. The views expressed in this paper are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the above‐mentioned entities.

  • 1. Pew Research Center . Internet/broadband fact sheet. Pew Research Center, February 5, 2018.
  • 2. Perrin A, Jiang J. About a quarter of US adults say they are ‘almost constantly’ online. Pew Research Center, March 14, 2018. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 3. Anderson M, Jiang J. Teens, social media & technology 2018. Pew Research Center, May 31, 2018. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 4. Draganski B, Gaser C, Busch V et al. Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature 2004;427:311. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 5. Osterhout L, Poliakov A, Inoue K et al. Second‐language learning and changes in the brain. J Neurolinguistics 2008;21:509‐21. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 6. Scholz J, Klein MC, Behrens TE et al. Training induces changes in white‐matter architecture. Nature Neurosci 2009;12:1370. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 7. Draganski B, Gaser C, Kempermann G et al. Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J Neurosci 2006;26:6314‐7. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 8. Gindrat A‐D, Chytiris M, Balerna M et al. Use‐dependent cortical processing from fingertips in touchscreen phone users. Curr Biol 2015;25:109‐16. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 9. Small GW, Moody TD, Siddarth P et al. Your brain on Google: patterns of cerebral activation during internet searching. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;17:116‐26. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 10. Levy R. Aging‐associated cognitive decline. Int Psychogeriatr 1994;6:63‐8. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 11. Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ et al. Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging 1999;14:245‐63. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 12. Small BJ, Dixon RA, McArdle JJ et al. Do changes in lifestyle engagement moderate cognitive decline in normal aging? Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology 2012;26:144‐55. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 13. Zhou F, Montag C, Sariyska R et al. Orbitofrontal gray matter deficits as marker of Internet gaming disorder: converging evidence from a cross‐sectional and prospective longitudinal design. Addict Biol 2019;24:100‐9. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 14. Paus T. Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9:60‐8. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 15. Kay M, Santos J, Takane M. mHealth: new horizons for health through mobile technologies. Geneva: World Health Organization, 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 16. Prensky M. Digital natives, digital immigrants part 1. On the Horizon 2001;9:1‐6. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 17. Alter A. Irresistible: the rise of addictive technology and the business of keeping us hooked. London: Penguin, 2017. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 18. Wilcockson TD, Ellis DA, Shaw H. Determining typical smartphone usage: what data do we need? Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Networking 2018;21:395‐8. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 19. Oulasvirta A, Rattenbury T, Ma L et al. Habits make smartphone use more pervasive. Pers Ubiquit Comput 2012;16:105‐14. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 20. McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G et al. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 2004;306:503‐7. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 21. Skinner BF. Operant behavior. Am Psychol 1963;18:503‐15. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 22. Purcell K, Rainie L, Heaps A et al. How teens do research in the digital world. Pew Research Center, November 1, 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 23. Ophir E, Nass C, Wagner AD. Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2009;106:15583‐7. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 24. Loh KK, Kanai R. How has the Internet reshaped human cognition? Neuroscientist 2016;22:506‐20. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 25. Uncapher MR, Wagner AD. Minds and brains of media multitaskers: current findings and future directions. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2018;115:9889‐96. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 26. Yeykelis L, Cummings JJ, Reeves B. Multitasking on a single device: arousal and the frequency, anticipation, and prediction of switching between media content on a computer. J Commun 2014;64:167‐92. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 27. Ralph BC, Thomson DR, Cheyne JA et al. Media multitasking and failures of attention in everyday life. Psychol Res 2014;78:661‐9. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 28. Lui KF, Wong AC. Does media multitasking always hurt? A positive correlation between multitasking and multisensory integration. Psychon Bull Rev 2012;19:647‐53. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 29. Moisala M, Salmela V, Hietajärvi L et al. Media multitasking is associated with distractibility and increased prefrontal activity in adolescents and young adults. NeuroImage 2016;134:113‐21. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 30. Kühn S, Gallinat J. Brains online: structural and functional correlates of habitual Internet use. Addict Biol 2015;20:415‐22. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 31. Loh KK, Kanai R. Higher media multi‐tasking activity is associated with smaller gray‐matter density in the anterior cingulate cortex. PLoS One 2014;9:e106698. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 32. Peng M, Chen X, Zhao Q et al. Attentional scope is reduced by Internet use: a behavior and ERP study. PLoS One 2018;13:e0198543. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 33. Rideout VJ. The Common Sense census: media use by tweens and teens. San Francisco: Common Sense Media Inc., 2015. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 34. Baumgartner SE, van der Schuur WA, Lemmens JS et al. The relationship between media multitasking and attention problems in adolescents: results of two longitudinal studies. Hum Commun Res 2017;44:3‐30. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 35. van Der Schuur WA, Baumgartner SE, Sumter SR et al. The consequences of media multitasking for youth: a review. Comput Human Behav 2015;53:204‐15. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 36. Altmann EM, Trafton JG, Hambrick DZ. Momentary interruptions can derail the train of thought. J Exp Psychol Gen 2014;143:215‐26. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 37. Baird B, Smallwood J, Mrazek MD et al. Inspired by distraction: mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychol Sci 2012;23:1117‐22. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 38. Colley A, Maltby J. Impact of the Internet on our lives: male and female personal perspectives. Comput Human Behav 2008;24:2005‐13. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 39. Vargha‐Khadem F, Gadian DG, Watkins KE et al. Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science 1997;277:376‐80. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 40. Sparrow B, Liu J, Wegner DM. Google effects on memory: cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science 2011;333:776. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 41. Wegner DM. Transactive memory: a contemporary analysis of the group mind In: Mullen B, Goethals GR. (eds). Theories of group behavior. Basel: Springer Nature, 1987:185‐208. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 42. Liang DW, Moreland R, Argote L. Group versus individual training and group performance: the mediating role of transactive memory. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1995;21:384‐93. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 43. Lewis K, Herndon B. Transactive memory systems: current issues and future research directions. Organ Sci 2011;22:1254‐65. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 44. Henkel LA. Point‐and‐shoot memories: the influence of taking photos on memory for a museum tour. Psychol Sci 2014;25:396‐402. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 45. Wegner DM, Ward AF. The internet has become the external hard drive for our memories. Sci Am 2013;309:58‐61. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 46. Ward AF. Supernormal: how the Internet is changing our memories and our minds. Psychol Inq 2013;24:341‐8. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 47. Dong G, Potenza MN. Behavioural and brain responses related to Internet search and memory. Eur J Neurosci 2015;42:2546‐54. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 48. Liu X, Lin X, Zheng M et al. Internet search alters intra‐ and inter‐regional synchronization in the temporal gyrus. Front Psychol 2018;9:260. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 49. Wang Y, Wu L, Luo L et al. Short‐term Internet search using makes people rely on search engines when facing unknown issues. PLoS One 2017;12:e0176325. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 50. Storm BC, Stone SM, Benjamin AS. Using the Internet to access information inflates future use of the Internet to access other information. Memory 2017;25:717‐23. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 51. Dong G, Potenza MN. Short‐term Internet‐search practicing modulates brain activity during recollection. Neuroscience 2016;335:82‐90. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 52. Dong G, Li H, Potenza MN. Short‐term internet‐search training is associated with increased fractional anisotropy in the superior longitudinal fasciculus in the parietal lobe. Front Neurosci 2017;11:372. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 53. Storm BC, Stone SM. Saving‐enhanced memory: the benefits of saving on the learning and remembering of new information. Psychol Sci 2015;26:182‐8. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 54. Bell V, Bishop DVM, Przybylski AK. The debate over digital technology and young people. BMJ 2015;351:h3064. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 55. Heersmink R. The Internet, cognitive enhancement, and the values of cognition. Minds and Mach 2016;26:389‐407. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 56. Heersmink R, Sutton J. Cognition and the Web: extended, transactive, or scaffolded. Erkenntnis (in press). [ Google Scholar ]
  • 57. Barr N, Pennycook G, Stolz JA et al. The brain in your pocket: evidence that smartphones are used to supplant thinking. Comput Human Behav 2015;48:473‐80. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 58. Hamilton KA, Yao MZ. Blurring boundaries: effects of device features on metacognitive evaluations. Comput Human Behav 2018;89:213‐20. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 59. Fisher M, Goddu MK, Keil FC. Searching for explanations: how the Internet inflates estimates of internal knowledge. J Exp Psychol Gen 2015;144:674‐87. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 60. Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A et al. Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. J Anat 2005;207:35‐66. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 61. Fowler JH, Christakis NA. Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social network: longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham Heart Study. BMJ 2008;337:a2338. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 62. Smith KP, Christakis NA. Social networks and health. Annu Rev Sociol 2008;34:405‐29. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 63. House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D. Social relationships and health. Science 1988;241:540‐5. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 64. Yang YC, Boen C, Gerken K et al. Social relationships and physiological determinants of longevity across the human life span. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2016;113:578‐83. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 65. Holt‐Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta‐analytic review. PLoS Med 2010;7:e1000316. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 66. Dunbar R. The anatomy of friendship. Trends Cogn Sci 2018;22:32‐51. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 67. Grabowicz PA, Ramasco JJ, Moro E et al. Social features of online networks: the strength of intermediary ties in online social media. PLoS One 2012;7:e29358. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 68. Jurgenson N. When atoms meet bits: social media, the mobile web and augmented revolution. Future Internet 2012;4:83‐91. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 69. Eltantawy N, Wiest JB. Social media in the Egyptian revolution: reconsidering resource mobilization theory. Int J Commun 2011;5:1207‐24. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 70. Sunstein CR. # Republic. Divided democracy in the age of social media. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 2018. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 71. Allcott H, Gentzkow M. Social media and fake news in the 2016 election. J Econ Perspect 2017;31:211‐36. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 72. Ridings CM, Gefen D. Virtual community attraction: why people hang out online. J Comput Mediat Commun 2004;10:JCMC10110. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 73. Wellman B. Computer networks as social networks. Science 2001;293:2031‐4. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 74. Kanai R, Bahrami B, Roylance R et al. Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure. Proc Biol Sci 2012;279:1327‐34. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 75. Falk EB, Bassett DS. Brain and social networks: fundamental building blocks of human experience. Trends Cogn Sci 2017;21:674‐90. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 76. Bickart KC, Wright CI, Dautoff RJ et al. Amygdala volume and social network size in humans. Nat Neurosci 2011;14:163. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 77. Sperling R, Chua E, Cocchiarella A et al. Putting names to faces: successful encoding of associative memories activates the anterior hippocampal formation. Neuroimage 2003;20:1400‐10. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 78. Sperling RA, Bates JF, Cocchiarella AJ et al. Encoding novel face‐name associations: a functional MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 2001;14:129‐39. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 79. Lewis K, Kaufman J, Gonzalez M et al. Tastes, ties, and time: a new social network dataset using Facebook. com. Soc Net 2008;30:330‐42. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 80. Szell M, Lambiotte R, Thurner S. Multirelational organization of large‐scale social networks in an online world. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2010;107:13636‐41. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 81. Zhou W‐X, Sornette D, Hill RA et al. Discrete hierarchical organization of social group sizes. Proc Biol Sci 2005;272:439‐44. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 82. Hamilton MJ, Milne BT, Walker RS et al. The complex structure of hunter‐gatherer social networks. Proc Biol Sci 2007;274:2195‐203. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 83. Dunbar RI. Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behav Brain Sci 1993;16:681‐94. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 84. Kordsmeyer T, Mac Carron P, Dunbar R. Sizes of permanent campsite communities reflect constraints on natural human communities. Curr Anthropol 2017;58:289‐94. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 85. Hill RA, Dunbar RI. Social network size in humans. Hum Nat 2003;14:53‐72. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 86. Fuchs B, Sornette D, Thurner S. Fractal multi‐level organisation of human groups in a virtual world. Sci Rep 2014;4:6526. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 87. Dunbar RI, Arnaboldi V, Conti M et al. The structure of online social networks mirrors those in the offline world. Soc Net 2015;43:39‐47. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 88. Dunbar RI. Do online social media cut through the constraints that limit the size of offline social networks? R Soc Open Sci 2016;3:150292. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 89. Arnaboldi V, Passarella A, Conti M et al. Online social networks: human cognitive constraints in Facebook and Twitter personal graphs. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 2015. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 90. Krems JA, Dunbar R. Clique size and network characteristics in hyperlink cinema. Hum Nat 2013;24:414‐29. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 91. Dezecache G, Dunbar RI. Sharing the joke: the size of natural laughter groups. Evol Hum Behav 2012;33:775‐9. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 92. Dunbar RI. The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol 1998;6:178‐90. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 93. Sutcliffe A, Dunbar R, Binder J et al. Relationships and the social brain: integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. Br J Psychol 2012;103:149‐68. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 94. Miritello G, Moro E, Lara R et al. Time as a limited resource: communication strategy in mobile phone networks. Soc Net 2013;35:89‐95. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 95. Massen JJ, Sterck EH, De Vos H. Close social associations in animals and humans: functions and mechanisms of friendship. Behaviour 2010;147:1379‐412. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 96. Hill RA, Bentley RA, Dunbar RI. Network scaling reveals consistent fractal pattern in hierarchical mammalian societies. Biol Lett 2008;4:748‐51. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 97. Hobbs WR, Burke MK. Connective recovery in social networks after the death of a friend. Nat Hum Behav 2017;1:0092. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 98. Firth JA, Voelkl B, Crates RA et al. Wild birds respond to flockmate loss by increasing their social network associations to others. Proc R Soc B 2017;284:20170299. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 99. Sallet J, Mars RB, Noonan MP et al. The organization of dorsal frontal cortex in humans and macaques. J Neurosci 2013;33:12255‐74. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 100. Sutcliffe A, Dunbar R, Wang D. Modelling the evolution of social structure. PLoS One 2016;11:e0158605. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 101. Firth JA, Sheldon BC, Brent LJN. Indirectly connected: simple social differences can explain the causes and apparent consequences of complex social network positions. Proc Biol Sci 2017;284:20171939. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 102. Watts DJ. A twenty‐first century science. Nature 2007;445:489. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 103. Bargh JA, McKenna KY. The Internet and social life. Annu Rev Psychol 2004;55:573‐90. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 104. Grossmann T. The role of medial prefrontal cortex in early social cognition. Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:340. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 105. Achterberg M, van Duijvenvoorde ACK, van der Meulen M et al. The neural and behavioral correlates of social evaluation in childhood. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2017;24:107‐17. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 106. Achterberg M, van Duijvenvoorde AC, Bakermans‐Kranenburg MJ et al. Control your anger! The neural basis of aggression regulation in response to negative social feedback. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2016;11:712‐20. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 107. Crone EA, Konijn EA. Media use and brain development during adolescence. Nat Commun 2018;9:588. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 108. Hamm MP, Newton AS, Chisholm A et al. Prevalence and effect of cyberbullying on children and young people: a scoping review of social media studies. JAMA Pediatr 2015;169:770‐7. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 109. Vannucci A, Flannery KM, Ohannessian CM. Social media use and anxiety in emerging adults. J Affect Disord 2017;207:163‐6. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 110. Lin LY, Sidani JE, Shensa A et al. Association between social media use and depression among US young adults. Depress Anxiety 2016;33:323‐31. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 111. Common Sense Media. Social media, social life: teens reveal their experiences. https://www.commonsensemedia.org .
  • 112. Collins RL. For better or worse: the impact of upward social comparison on self‐evaluations. Psychol Bull 1996;119:51. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 113. Verduyn P, Ybarra O, Résibois M et al. Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective well‐being? A critical review. Soc Issues Pol Rev 2017;11:274‐302. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 114. Holmgren HG, Coyne SM. Can't stop scrolling!: pathological use of social networking sites in emerging adulthood. Addict Res Theory 2017;25:375‐82. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 115. Royal Society of Public Health . Status of mind: social media and young people's mental health. London: Royal Society for Public Health, 2017. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 116. Twenge JM, Joiner TE, Rogers ML et al. Increases in depressive symptoms, suicide‐related outcomes, and suicide rates among U.S. adolescents after 2010 and links to increased new media screen time. Clin Psychol Sci 2017;6:3‐17. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 117. Twenge JM, Joiner TE, Martin G et al. Digital media may explain a substantial portion of the rise in depressive symptoms among adolescent girls: response to Daly. Clin Psychol Sci 2018;6:296‐7. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 118. Twenge JM, Joiner TE, Martin G et al. Amount of time online is problematic if it displaces face‐to‐face social interaction and sleep. Clin Psychol Sci 2018;6:456‐7. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 119. Gleeson JF, Cotton SM, Alvarez‐Jimenez M et al. A randomized controlled trial of relapse prevention therapy for first‐episode psychosis patients: outcome at 30‐month follow‐up. Schizophr Bull 2013;39:436‐48. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 120. Torous J, Larsen ME, Depp C et al. Smartphones, sensors, and machine learning to advance real‐time prediction and interventions for suicide prevention: a review of current progress and next steps. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2018;20:51. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 121. D'Alfonso S, Santesteban‐Echarri O, Rice S et al. Artificial intelligence‐assisted online social therapy for youth mental health. Front Psychol 2017;8:796. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 122. Alvarez‐Jimenez M, Alcazar‐Corcoles MA, Gonzalez‐Blanch C et al. Online, social media and mobile technologies for psychosis treatment: a systematic review on novel user‐led interventions. Schizophr Res 2014;156:96‐106. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 123. Alvarez‐Jimenez M, Bendall S, Lederman R et al. On the HORYZON: moderated online social therapy for long‐term recovery in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res 2013;143:143‐9. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 124. Alvarez‐Jimenez M, Gleeson JF, Bendall S et al. Enhancing social functioning in young people at ultra high risk (UHR) for psychosis: a pilot study of a novel strengths and mindfulness‐based online social therapy. Schizophr Res 2018;202:369‐77. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 125. Gleeson J, Lederman R, Koval P et al. Moderated online social therapy: a model for reducing stress in carers of young people diagnosed with mental health disorders. Front Psychol 2017;8:485. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 126. Rice S, Gleeson J, Davey C et al. Moderated online social therapy for depression relapse prevention in young people: pilot study of a ‘next generation' online intervention. Early Interv Psychiatry 2018;12:613‐25. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 127. Insel TR. Digital phenotyping: a global tool for psychiatry. World Psychiatry 2018;17:276‐7. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 128. Aboujaoude E. Telemental health: why the revolution has not arrived. World Psychiatry 2018;17:277‐8. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 129. Bhugra D, Tasman A, Pathare S et al. The WPA‐Lancet Psychiatry commission on the future of psychiatry. Lancet Psychiatry 2017;4:775‐818. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 130. Goekint M, Bos I, Heyman E et al. Acute running stimulates hippocampal dopaminergic neurotransmission in rats, but has no influence on brain‐derived neurotrophic factor. J Appl Physiol 2012;112:535‐41. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 131. Torous J, Anderson G, Bertagnoli A et al. Towards a consensus around standards for smartphone apps and digital mental health. World Psychiatry 2019;18:97‐8. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 132. Torous J, Firth J. Bridging the dichotomy of actual versus aspirational digital health. World Psychiatry 2018;17:108‐9. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 133. Gleeson J, Lederman R, Herrman H et al. Moderated online social therapy for carers of young people recovering from first‐episode psychosis: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials 2017;18:27. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 134. Alvarez‐Jimenez M, Bendall S, Koval P et al. The Horyzons trial: protocol for a randomised controlled trial of a moderated online social therapy to maintain treatment effects from first episode psychosis services. BMJ Open 2019;9:e024104. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 135. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Asano K et al. Impact of frequency of internet use on development of brain structures and verbal intelligence: longitudinal analyses. Hum Brain Mapp 2018;39:4471‐79. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 136. Kühn S, Gleich T, Lorenz RC et al. Playing Super Mario induces structural brain plasticity: gray matter changes resulting from training with a commercial video game. Mol Psychiatry 2014;19:265‐71. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 137. Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 2013;501:97‐101. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 138. Oh SJ, Seo S, Lee JH et al. Effects of smartphone‐based memory training for older adults with subjective memory complaints: a randomized controlled trial. Aging Ment Health 2018;22:526‐34. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 139. Baltes PB, Baltes MM. Psychological perspectives on successful aging: the model of selective optimization with compensation In: Baltes PB, Baltes MM. (eds). Successful aging: perspectives from the behavioral sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1990:1‐34. [ Google Scholar ]
  • View on publisher site
  • PDF (333.1 KB)
  • Collections

Similar articles

Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.

  • Download .nbib .nbib
  • Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

Add to Collections

Logo

Essay on Digital World

Students are often asked to write an essay on Digital World in their schools and colleges. And if you’re also looking for the same, we have created 100-word, 250-word, and 500-word essays on the topic.

Let’s take a look…

100 Words Essay on Digital World

What is the digital world.

The digital world is like a giant playground online. It’s where we use computers, smartphones, and the internet to talk to friends, play games, and learn new things. It’s made up of websites, apps, and videos that we can see on screens.

Learning Online

Schools are now in our homes too, thanks to the internet. We can watch lessons, do quizzes, and even talk to our teachers through our computers. It’s like having a classroom at your fingertips!

Fun and Games

Playing games has never been easier. With a click, we can join adventures, solve puzzles, or compete in sports on our devices. It’s all fun, and we can play with people from all over the world.

Staying Safe

While the digital world is exciting, we must stay safe. This means setting strong passwords, not talking to strangers online, and always telling an adult if something seems wrong or makes us uncomfortable.

Also check:

  • Advantages and Disadvantages of Digital World
  • Speech on Digital World

250 Words Essay on Digital World

The digital world is like a huge playground on the internet. It’s full of websites, apps, and all sorts of fun and useful things that we can see and do using computers, smartphones, and other gadgets. Just like in a real playground, you can meet friends, play games, learn new stuff, and share stories.

Learning and School

In this digital playground, you can go to a place called ‘online school’ where you can learn about anything from dinosaurs to how to solve math problems. It’s like having a library that never ends, with books and videos that can teach you about every single thing in the world.

Playing and Having Fun

Games are a big part of the digital world. There are games that let you build cities, play sports, or go on adventures. And the cool thing is, you can play with people from all over the world, not just your neighborhood.

But remember, just like any playground, you need to be safe in the digital world. This means not talking to strangers, not sharing personal information, and telling an adult if something doesn’t feel right.

So, the digital world is a place where you can learn, play, and do lots of exciting things. But always be careful and use it in a way that is safe and good for you. It’s like a big city that’s always awake, always buzzing with new things to discover, but you should know the rules to stay safe and enjoy it to the fullest.

500 Words Essay on Digital World

The digital world is like a giant, invisible playground that exists on computers, phones, and other electronic devices. It is made up of all the information that we can send, receive, and share using technology. This includes everything from the videos we watch, the messages we send to our friends, to the games we play.

Communication in the Digital World

One of the biggest parts of the digital world is how we talk to each other. Long ago, people had to send letters that took days or even weeks to arrive. Now, we can send a message to someone on the other side of the planet, and they’ll get it in seconds! Social media platforms like Facebook and Instagram let us share what we’re doing and see what others are up to.

Learning and the Internet

Schools are also a part of the digital world. Many classrooms use computers and the internet to help students learn. You can find information about almost any topic online. There are also special programs and games that make learning fun and interesting.

Entertainment Everywhere

Entertainment in the digital world is endless. We can watch movies, listen to music, or play games whenever we want. Websites like YouTube have millions of videos on every topic you can think of. And with smartphones and tablets, we can enjoy all these things not just at home, but anywhere we go.

Shopping with a Click

Shopping has also changed because of the digital world. Instead of going to a store, we can buy things online. This can be anything from clothes and toys to groceries. We pick what we want, pay online, and then wait for it to arrive at our door.

Staying Safe Online

While the digital world is fun and useful, it’s important to stay safe. We should keep our personal information private and think carefully before we share something online. Remember, once something is on the internet, it can be hard to remove it completely.

Jobs and the Digital World

Many people work in the digital world. They create websites, make apps, and help keep all the information online safe and organized. These jobs are important because they keep the digital world running smoothly for everyone to enjoy.

The Future of the Digital World

The digital world is always growing and changing. New inventions and ideas appear all the time. We might not know exactly what the future holds, but it’s sure to be exciting. We’ll see new ways to learn, play, and talk to each other.

In conclusion, the digital world is a big part of our lives. It helps us communicate, learn, have fun, shop, and work. As long as we use it wisely and stay safe, the digital world will continue to be a place where we can discover and explore new things every day.

That’s it! I hope the essay helped you.

If you’re looking for more, here are essays on other interesting topics:

  • Essay on Digital Marketing
  • Essay on Digital Life
  • Essay on Determinism

Apart from these, you can look at all the essays by clicking here .

Happy studying!

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

IMAGES

  1. Online Education Essay

    essay on online world

  2. Essay on Online Education

    essay on online world

  3. Free Essay

    essay on online world

  4. 10 lines essay on online education in english writing

    essay on online world

  5. Online Classes Essay

    essay on online world

  6. Essay on "The use of the internet"| essay writing|English writing

    essay on online world

VIDEO

  1. Pros And Cons Of Online Learning || PLS Education || Essay Writing

  2. Amazing online essay I Top amazing essay

  3. write essay on internet

  4. 10 lines essay on world earth day in english || World earth day 10 lines

  5. Order Essay Online Achieve Academic Excellence with GoEssayWriter

  6. Cheap essay online I The best essay writing services